Skip to Main Content

Psychiatry Grand Rounds: November 13, 2020

November 13, 2020

Psychiatry Grand Rounds: November 13, 2020

 .
  • 00:00Good morning everyone.
  • 00:01It's great to see so many friendly faces
  • 00:05in that bar on the right of my screen.
  • 00:08I have the pleasure and honor of
  • 00:11introducing Craig Brian this morning.
  • 00:13I first actually got to know Craig
  • 00:15in 2015 after reading a paper in
  • 00:18the American Journal of Psychiatry,
  • 00:20randomized controlled trial that
  • 00:22he had done with David Rudd.
  • 00:25That actually showed a 60%
  • 00:27reduction in suicide attempts among
  • 00:28active duty military personnel,
  • 00:30which really caught my interest.
  • 00:32So I reached out to Craig and David
  • 00:35and started talking with them,
  • 00:37learning more about their work
  • 00:39and ultimately became a friend
  • 00:42and admirer of his work.
  • 00:43I looking very much looking forward
  • 00:45to his talk today because I think
  • 00:47there's data in it I'm not aware of.
  • 00:50Craig is a clinical psychologist.
  • 00:52He's the star professor of Psychiatry
  • 00:54and Behavioral Health at the Ohio
  • 00:57State University Wexner Medical Center.
  • 00:59And is the division director for
  • 01:02Recovery and resilience there.
  • 01:03He did his doctorate in Clinical
  • 01:05Psychiatry at Baylor University,
  • 01:07completed clinical residency at
  • 01:08the Wilford Hall Medical Center,
  • 01:10Lackland Air Force Base,
  • 01:11and was deployed in Iraq in 2009 and
  • 01:14was the director of the traumatic
  • 01:17Brain Injuries Clinic at the Air
  • 01:19Force Theater Hospital there.
  • 01:21He left active duty service and started
  • 01:23researching post traumatic stress disorder,
  • 01:25suicidal behaviors and
  • 01:26suicide prevention strategies,
  • 01:27as well as psychological health
  • 01:29and resiliency.
  • 01:30He's had faculty appointments at
  • 01:31the University of Texas Health,
  • 01:33same health system in San Antonio,
  • 01:35the University of Utah,
  • 01:37and now at the Ohio State University.
  • 01:39He's had 10s of millions of dollars
  • 01:42in grants focused on these topics.
  • 01:44Suicide prevention,
  • 01:45PTS di trauma and resilience.
  • 01:47One has won many awards.
  • 01:48I won't go into all of them.
  • 01:51And I'll just let him dive in and
  • 01:54get right to it.
  • 01:55So Craig, thanks so much for coming.
  • 01:57Thanks for everyone for attending.
  • 01:58If you have questions,
  • 02:00please enter them into the chat and
  • 02:02when Craig is done we can go through
  • 02:04them and try to get to them all.
  • 02:06So thanks.
  • 02:09Alright well thanks for the introduction.
  • 02:11It's really great to be able to join all
  • 02:14of you this morning and seeing a little
  • 02:16bit of windows and in my boxes here,
  • 02:19it looks like you guys are having a nice day.
  • 02:23Up there in the Connecticut area and a
  • 02:26beautiful day here in central Ohio as well.
  • 02:28So today what I thought
  • 02:30I would talk about is.
  • 02:32More of some conceptual work around
  • 02:35suicide that we've really been focusing
  • 02:37on over the past several years now.
  • 02:40Certainly present some of the
  • 02:42empirical findings at our lab and
  • 02:45other labs have published in recent
  • 02:47years that really kind of backup.
  • 02:50This sort of approach to understanding
  • 02:52suicide and suicidal behavior.
  • 02:54And then, after we kind of start with
  • 02:57more kind of conceptual ideas about
  • 03:00thinking of suicide in new ways.
  • 03:03You know,
  • 03:04I'll conclude with some of the work
  • 03:06that we've done from an intervention
  • 03:08and prevention standpoint,
  • 03:10and how I think this alternative approach
  • 03:12to understanding the emergence of
  • 03:14suicidal behavior and might lead us to
  • 03:17think about a range of interventions
  • 03:19in a number of strategic ways.
  • 03:22So the you know the basis for
  • 03:24this line of research really was,
  • 03:27you know, founded in.
  • 03:28I think some traditional ideas
  • 03:30about suicide prevention.
  • 03:31I think some of the traditional
  • 03:33ways that we have approached
  • 03:35our understanding of suicide and
  • 03:37approached prevention efforts are
  • 03:39encapsulated in many ways by a lot
  • 03:41of the catch phrases and sayings
  • 03:43that I have on the screen right now,
  • 03:46that I'm sure many of you are familiar with.
  • 03:49You probably heard this plenty often.
  • 03:52I'm going to maybe you as well as
  • 03:54mental health professionals and
  • 03:56researchers have said one or more of
  • 03:58these things as well when talking
  • 04:01about suicide and I start here because
  • 04:03I kind of think that where we're
  • 04:06heading now in in our research on
  • 04:08suicide prevention is really a mini
  • 04:10wasted on the cusp of a paradigm shift.
  • 04:13And I think it's critical for us
  • 04:16to understand where we come from
  • 04:18as we talk about and think about
  • 04:21where we're heading.
  • 04:22So one of the challenges, of course,
  • 04:25when it comes to suicide,
  • 04:27is that despite decades of fairly consistent,
  • 04:30I would argue fairly uniform thinking
  • 04:32about suicide over the past two
  • 04:35decades since the turn of the century,
  • 04:37we've seen a steady rise in
  • 04:40suicides in the US,
  • 04:41and which is not really mirrored
  • 04:43anywhere else in the world.
  • 04:46About 3/4 of nations around the
  • 04:48world during the same time frame.
  • 04:50If you look at statistics reported too.
  • 04:53Mental Health Organization.
  • 04:55See that most nations are actually
  • 04:58experiencing a decline in suicide
  • 04:59rates in the United States is one of
  • 05:02the minority that is actually seeing
  • 05:04the reverse pattern occur ING in.
  • 05:07On top of that, if we were to rank order.
  • 05:11You know all the world's nations in
  • 05:14terms of change in suicide rates since 2000,
  • 05:17we see that the United States is
  • 05:19actually #3 in terms of the fastest
  • 05:21and the largest increase in suicides.
  • 05:24And so there's something very unique I think
  • 05:26about the United States that's different
  • 05:29that Bing's very clearly are not working,
  • 05:31at least not in the way that we would like,
  • 05:35which has really stimulated
  • 05:36me and others to second guess.
  • 05:38A lot of those initial assumptions
  • 05:41that I shared with you.
  • 05:42Maybe there's something about the way
  • 05:45that we've approached suicide and.
  • 05:46Thought about it.
  • 05:48That isn't really accurate.
  • 05:49Or maybe we're missing the mark in some way.
  • 05:52'cause if we were if we were correct
  • 05:55about how we understand suicide in
  • 05:58our interventions were as effective
  • 06:00as we think they should be,
  • 06:02then probably we would not be seeing
  • 06:05this upward steady rise in suicide.
  • 06:07We should be seeing either things
  • 06:10holding steady or even a decrease.
  • 06:13So as I think about what are some of those
  • 06:16classic ways that we conceptualize suicide,
  • 06:19I really think that many of the theories
  • 06:21that have been proposed and there are
  • 06:24a multitude of theories about suicide.
  • 06:27In many ways,
  • 06:28conform to this general framework.
  • 06:30We could certainly probably quibble
  • 06:32and argue about differences and
  • 06:34the different theoretical models,
  • 06:36but I think that this this sort of
  • 06:40sequence of events, so to speak,
  • 06:42kind of serves as a sort of a
  • 06:45foundational way of capturing how
  • 06:48we have thought about suicide.
  • 06:50Where you know someone experiences
  • 06:52an intensification or elevation of
  • 06:55emotional distress and then some things
  • 06:57happen, certain mechanisms occur.
  • 06:59Different theories would posit
  • 07:01different mechanisms,
  • 07:02but something is happening whereby
  • 07:04emotional distress evolves into
  • 07:06suicidal ideations and then at that
  • 07:09point individuals were thinking
  • 07:11about suicide again.
  • 07:12Some kind of mechanisms or processes occur.
  • 07:16Different theories, again,
  • 07:17are proposing different mechanisms,
  • 07:19and then individuals transition
  • 07:21to suicidal behaviors.
  • 07:22So in essence,
  • 07:24they act upon the suicidal thoughts
  • 07:26and then another sort of typical
  • 07:30assumption about.
  • 07:31The sort of progression from ideations
  • 07:34to action is that relatively soon
  • 07:37before a suicide attempt occurs,
  • 07:39there are theoretically some observable
  • 07:42changes are observable variables that
  • 07:45we refer to as warning signs that should,
  • 07:48in theory signal that a suicide
  • 07:51attempt is about to occur.
  • 07:54So I would say despite in many ways
  • 07:58this serving is kind of a foundational.
  • 08:02Assumption about how we think about suicide.
  • 08:05As I noted on the previous slide,
  • 08:08we're not really good at preventing suicide,
  • 08:10at least not in the United States,
  • 08:13and so it's really prompted myself
  • 08:15and a number of other researchers
  • 08:18to question a lot of our long held
  • 08:21assumptions about suicide and to start
  • 08:24thinking about this in different ways.
  • 08:26And so I just want to kind of
  • 08:30talk today about some of my own.
  • 08:32Halfway my own thinking over the
  • 08:35past several years and kind of talk
  • 08:38about how different experiences
  • 08:40different studies have led us to to
  • 08:43really approach our understanding
  • 08:44of this emergent process of moving
  • 08:46twords suicidal behavior that we
  • 08:48think can potentially advance our
  • 08:51ability to prevent suicide.
  • 08:52And in many ways,
  • 08:54sort of.
  • 08:55I would kind of tie the
  • 08:57origins of my thinking
  • 08:59about this to some conversations
  • 09:01that I had with a colleague of mine.
  • 09:04In Utah. You know the first arrived.
  • 09:08There's faculty, especially the
  • 09:10conversations I had with John Buckner,
  • 09:13whose social quantitative psychologist his
  • 09:15expertise in complex dynamical systems.
  • 09:17And we were having coffee one day on campus,
  • 09:21just kind of chatting about our respective
  • 09:24programs, getting to know each other,
  • 09:27and it so happened that each of us had
  • 09:30been funded by NASA to do some work,
  • 09:34and in his project in his NASA.
  • 09:37On the project is kind of interesting.
  • 09:40He was being asked to model budget
  • 09:42overages and what NASA's concern was,
  • 09:45hey, you know we have all these grants.
  • 09:48We've all these contracts and
  • 09:50projects that we're funding.
  • 09:51You know, the vast majority of them.
  • 09:54They spend money as expected.
  • 09:56They perform on track,
  • 09:57so they're not really issues for us.
  • 10:00We obviously don't worry about
  • 10:02projects that achieve their
  • 10:04objectives on time and on budget.
  • 10:06And then there's another pool of
  • 10:08projects where the projects are
  • 10:10fairly consistently over budget.
  • 10:12They're not dramatically over budget,
  • 10:14but they are always spending more
  • 10:16than what was initially projected,
  • 10:19but there still achieving their
  • 10:21milestones and an absolute told him,
  • 10:23you know,
  • 10:24we're not really worried
  • 10:25about those projects either,
  • 10:27because they're predictably over budget.
  • 10:29We know exactly how much
  • 10:31they're going to spend.
  • 10:33And probably it's just we we
  • 10:35probably just under estimated the
  • 10:37resources needed for those projects,
  • 10:39but they're doing OK,
  • 10:40But there's this third group of projects.
  • 10:43That are really, really problematic for NASA,
  • 10:47and it's because.
  • 10:48They initially start on track there,
  • 10:51performing well they're spending as expected,
  • 10:54and then all of a sudden
  • 10:56they're massively over budget,
  • 10:58and then once they seem to
  • 11:00depart from being on track,
  • 11:02they have one period where their massively
  • 11:05over budget things just get worse and worse.
  • 11:08They keep coming up over budget,
  • 11:11over budget over budget dramatically,
  • 11:13and so this creates this massive
  • 11:15problem for the organization because it
  • 11:18shuts down other projects it creates.
  • 11:20Budget strain for everybody else.
  • 11:22All of the projects depending
  • 11:24on that one particular project,
  • 11:25it slowed down and so NASA
  • 11:28had actually a term for it.
  • 11:30They called it,
  • 11:31performance escapes,
  • 11:31and so they wanted John to help
  • 11:34figure out how do we know which of
  • 11:37our seemingly on track OK projects
  • 11:39are going to suddenly out of the blue
  • 11:42turn into project escapes and and so
  • 11:44John was modeling this mathematically.
  • 11:46This is one of the simulations
  • 11:48that he ran using their data and
  • 11:51you see here there's just like 1
  • 11:53project that sort of taking off.
  • 11:56This is a performance escape as compared
  • 11:58to all the other projects that are sort
  • 12:00of clustered on their spending there.
  • 12:02They're kind of on track,
  • 12:03and as we're talking about
  • 12:05these performance escapes.
  • 12:07He said,
  • 12:07you know,
  • 12:08that's interesting that what you're
  • 12:10doing there because you know I'm a
  • 12:13suicide researcher and one of our
  • 12:15big problems that we run into him.
  • 12:17Clinicians. We have some family members,
  • 12:19lots of people is that
  • 12:21suicide seems unpredictable.
  • 12:22Patients will seem to be doing OK in
  • 12:24treatment and then suddenly they're dead.
  • 12:27Family members say everything
  • 12:28seemed to be OK.
  • 12:30They seem to be fine.
  • 12:31And then all of a sudden
  • 12:33they had attempted suicide,
  • 12:35and so there is this sort of subgroup.
  • 12:38Of suicide cases that seem to align with
  • 12:41this notion of a performance escape,
  • 12:44and we also have known for
  • 12:46several decades if there's a,
  • 12:48there's a significant percentage in some
  • 12:50studies up to half of those who attempt
  • 12:53suicide who deny suicidal planning.
  • 12:56Severe suicidal ideations,
  • 12:57where they seem to like skip these
  • 13:00intermediate steps that we have long held
  • 13:02to be a necessary precondition for suicide.
  • 13:05The assumption that many of us have.
  • 13:08Worked on for many years is that
  • 13:10will you have to think about
  • 13:12suicide in order to attempt suicide.
  • 13:14But for whatever reason,
  • 13:16there's a significant percentage of suicide
  • 13:18attempt survivors who are saying no.
  • 13:20Actually,
  • 13:20I didn't do that.
  • 13:21I skipped that step and
  • 13:23so we were really there.
  • 13:25Seems to be a lot of parallels
  • 13:27between these sort of budget overages.
  • 13:30And suicide.
  • 13:30And so this is in many ways that
  • 13:33sort of the pre genitor of a lot of
  • 13:36our collaboration together to start
  • 13:38thinking about suicide in unique ways.
  • 13:42And this is where I started to become
  • 13:45familiar with dynamical systems theory
  • 13:47and one of the key concepts within
  • 13:50that mathematical sort of branches.
  • 13:52This notion of emergence and emergence
  • 13:55just in general kind of refers to the
  • 13:58process of coming to be how something.
  • 14:01That was not there before
  • 14:03eventually comes into being,
  • 14:04and this sort of mathematical
  • 14:06concept of emergence is largely
  • 14:08founded upon four key assumptions,
  • 14:10the first of which is that change
  • 14:13within a system is constant.
  • 14:15The second is that the whole is
  • 14:17greater than the sum of its parts.
  • 14:20The third is that each component
  • 14:22of a system depends upon the other
  • 14:25components of that system to function,
  • 14:27and then 4th that complex systems
  • 14:30behave in non proportional ways.
  • 14:32And as John and I started
  • 14:34working together over the years,
  • 14:36we started to apply these principles
  • 14:38of emergence to our study of
  • 14:40suicide and what we're finding
  • 14:42is that it seems to be a very,
  • 14:44very useful way to conceptualize and
  • 14:46think about suicide risk in general.
  • 14:49And as we'll talk about later
  • 14:51on in the presentation,
  • 14:52and I think it is a lot of patients for
  • 14:55clinical practice as well as suicide
  • 14:57prevention strategies in general.
  • 14:59And so I'd like to do is just.
  • 15:02Spent some time talking about how
  • 15:05these four concepts seem to apply to
  • 15:08suicide and how this might change,
  • 15:10or at least advance or upgrade our
  • 15:13thinking about preventing suicide.
  • 15:15And so first,
  • 15:16let's talk about the first
  • 15:18assumption that change is constant.
  • 15:20So this is this notion that
  • 15:22suicide risk is a dynamic concept
  • 15:25that it's always ever changing
  • 15:28really isn't necessarily
  • 15:29a new idea. I mean this.
  • 15:32This is certainly been written
  • 15:34about for many, many decades.
  • 15:36Anyone who's a clinician knows this to be
  • 15:39true that patients are low risk sometimes,
  • 15:41but then they in turn to a high risk state
  • 15:44and then they return to a little risk stage.
  • 15:48But it's only been within the past
  • 15:50decade or so that I think technological
  • 15:52advances and data analytic methods have
  • 15:55gotten to a point where we're able to
  • 15:57quantify it and describe these change
  • 15:59processes with sufficient detail.
  • 16:01And so, for instance, this is a.
  • 16:03This is some of the results from.
  • 16:06Study that Evan Kleiman at Ruckers did
  • 16:09using ecological momentary assessment or MA
  • 16:11where they were basically assessing suicidal
  • 16:14individuals four to six times per day.
  • 16:16They would assess multiple variables,
  • 16:18but this one on the screen right now.
  • 16:21Ask them about several aspects of suicidal
  • 16:24ideations and so they were able to map
  • 16:27out fluctuations in suicidal ideations
  • 16:29over the course of several weeks,
  • 16:31and what you see here.
  • 16:33This is all of the participants
  • 16:35in that study.
  • 16:37Aggregated together in a few of the
  • 16:39cases are kind of drawn out and put into
  • 16:43color to help demonstrate the concepts.
  • 16:45What you see is that variability.
  • 16:48Is the rule not the exception an one of the
  • 16:52more striking findings from climate studies,
  • 16:55at about 90% of participants who were
  • 16:58suicidal experience to 1 standard deviation
  • 17:00or larger shift in their suicidal thinking
  • 17:03within the span of just a few hours.
  • 17:06And so you can see that here in some
  • 17:09places where there are these sudden
  • 17:11dramatic increases in suicide risk,
  • 17:14and then sometimes it's the reverse.
  • 17:16There's a sudden and dramatic
  • 17:18drop in suicide risk.
  • 17:20And every so often there's even these,
  • 17:22like very brief peaks that seem to
  • 17:25correspond with an acute suicidal crisis.
  • 17:27And so this this we're seeing
  • 17:29this pattern over and over again,
  • 17:32not only with the MA studies with some of
  • 17:35the work that I've done modeling change
  • 17:38in suicidal ideations on a weekly basis.
  • 17:40And now a daily basis.
  • 17:42You know,
  • 17:43an intensive outpatient programs,
  • 17:45inpatient settings,
  • 17:46or even outpatient therapy
  • 17:47settings that these fluctuations
  • 17:49and suicide risk are the rule.
  • 17:51Now why this is sort of important
  • 17:53for us to think about from a clinical
  • 17:56perspective is that it provides an
  • 17:59explanation for why suicide risk screening
  • 18:02and assessment is so notoriously unreliable,
  • 18:04and so here on the screen.
  • 18:07Now I have two hypothetical patients,
  • 18:09one depicted in Black,
  • 18:11one depicted in red.
  • 18:12You see that they are experiencing
  • 18:15fluctuations in suicide risk
  • 18:17that are comparable to what we
  • 18:19saw in the previous screen.
  • 18:21Now, if these two patients were to say,
  • 18:24come into an emergency Department
  • 18:26on the same day,
  • 18:27and that day correspond to the arrow
  • 18:29on the left, and we assess them,
  • 18:32perhaps whatever scale that you use with pH,
  • 18:34D 9, the ask the easy save the Columbia,
  • 18:37whatever it is,
  • 18:38what we would conclude on that day
  • 18:41is that the person in black
  • 18:43is higher risk for suicide.
  • 18:45Been the person in red, and so we might.
  • 18:48Perhaps we would consider
  • 18:49hospitalizing the person in black,
  • 18:51but may be discharged.
  • 18:52Person in red to home.
  • 18:54Now if we slide to the right of
  • 18:56this graph to that second arrow,
  • 18:58if those same two individuals had come
  • 19:00into the emergency Department at a
  • 19:02different point in their time series,
  • 19:04we would conclude the exact reverse
  • 19:06person in red is the higher
  • 19:08risk than the person in black.
  • 19:10And so maybe we would hospital as a
  • 19:12person in red instead of the person in black.
  • 19:15Now if we look at the entire
  • 19:17T of the time series, however,
  • 19:19what you would see is that
  • 19:21they actually do not really.
  • 19:22Differentiate from each other.
  • 19:24There's not a whole way a real
  • 19:26way to kind of say that one person
  • 19:28is higher risk than the other.
  • 19:30They're both experiencing these
  • 19:32Upson Downs and this is,
  • 19:33I think a critical new.
  • 19:35Idea that many of us are starting
  • 19:38to find in our studies over and
  • 19:40over again is that suicide risk,
  • 19:43especially suicidal ideations,
  • 19:44isn't really all that useful for
  • 19:47distinguishing between patients
  • 19:48who are higher and lower risk.
  • 19:50And that's largely because
  • 19:51suicidal ideations suicide risk
  • 19:52is a within person phenomenon.
  • 19:54People have good days and bad days,
  • 19:57and so we perhaps need to start shifting.
  • 20:00I'm thinking away from some of
  • 20:02our research models of clinical
  • 20:04decision rubrics that are trying
  • 20:07to distinguish which patients are
  • 20:09going to attempt suicide in which
  • 20:11are not and move towards a thinking
  • 20:13of when is a given individual
  • 20:16vulnerable or in high probability
  • 20:18state to engage in suicidal behaviors
  • 20:21and so along this line of sort of
  • 20:23within person change processes.
  • 20:25We started to now conduct several
  • 20:28studies to really map out and
  • 20:31mathematically model these
  • 20:32fluctuations in suicide risk overtime,
  • 20:35under the assumption that maybe
  • 20:37there are certain patterns of change
  • 20:40within individuals that might signal
  • 20:42the near term emergence of suicidal
  • 20:45behavior such that the concept
  • 20:47of a warning sign, for instance,
  • 20:50isn't necessarily the occurrence or
  • 20:53the existence of a given variable or a given.
  • 20:57Behavior,
  • 20:57but perhaps the warning sign is a
  • 20:59change process that departs from in
  • 21:02individual standard baseline change process.
  • 21:04So on the screen.
  • 21:06Here I give an example.
  • 21:08This on the left hand side of this time
  • 21:11series we see fluctuations and suicide risk.
  • 21:14Then at about the midpoint
  • 21:16of the time series,
  • 21:18what you see is those fluctuations in
  • 21:21suicide risk become more and more prominent.
  • 21:24The amplitude of the Upson Downs increase.
  • 21:27In magnitude,
  • 21:27this is actually a pattern that
  • 21:30we've seen down to separate
  • 21:32studies that have been published,
  • 21:34that it serves as an indicator that
  • 21:37suicidal behavior is coming where in essence,
  • 21:40from a mathematical perspective.
  • 21:42What this signals is a loss
  • 21:44of self regulation.
  • 21:45The system is breaking down in the
  • 21:48system is no longer able to contain
  • 21:51and preserve its homeostatic balance,
  • 21:53and so it's getting
  • 21:55agitated which signals and.
  • 21:57Upcoming emergent shift in states we've
  • 21:59also found in some of our research
  • 22:02that a different but related pattern
  • 22:04relates to the frequency of fluctuation.
  • 22:07So again, here on the left hand side
  • 22:09of the time series we see these
  • 22:12periodic spikes that occur in suicide
  • 22:15risk and then at the midpoint those
  • 22:18spikes in suicide risk occur much,
  • 22:20much more often.
  • 22:22This is a second dimension of
  • 22:24dysregulation such that perhaps
  • 22:26sometimes we see an increase in.
  • 22:28Amplitude of fluctuations.
  • 22:29Sometimes we'll see an increasing
  • 22:31frequency of amplitudes,
  • 22:33and we might also see a combination
  • 22:36of the two.
  • 22:37So if we were to then apply
  • 22:40this to clinical practice,
  • 22:42what this would indicate is
  • 22:44that repeated assessments of
  • 22:46suicide risk overtime state,
  • 22:48like every single therapy session,
  • 22:50every single Med monitoring
  • 22:52session might be able to detect
  • 22:54within person change processes,
  • 22:56which could then signal hey,
  • 22:59this is a departure from the norm.
  • 23:02This is not a typical change pattern
  • 23:04for this particular individual.
  • 23:06They're becoming more dysregulated and so
  • 23:08perhaps that would prompt an intervention.
  • 23:11We're actually working now.
  • 23:12We have a narrow one.
  • 23:14This is kind of the key idea behind
  • 23:17the work that we're currently
  • 23:19doing on a grant to look at.
  • 23:22Could you potentially develop novel
  • 23:24detection and alert systems that
  • 23:26could be implemented in clinical
  • 23:28practice that now takes into
  • 23:30account multiple time points in
  • 23:32applies new mathematical models?
  • 23:34To look at within person, change to signal.
  • 23:36Hey,
  • 23:36this patient is now at a higher
  • 23:39probability risk state and we
  • 23:41should probably intervene with them.
  • 23:44Now the 2nd two concepts of emergency.
  • 23:46I want to talk about here is the
  • 23:48whole is greater than the sum of its
  • 23:51parts in each component of a system.
  • 23:54Depends on the other components to function,
  • 23:56and so this is.
  • 23:57These are sort of two related ideas
  • 23:59that I think is also highly relevant
  • 24:01to our work and suicide prevention.
  • 24:04And to demonstrate these two concepts,
  • 24:06I'm actually going to share with you
  • 24:08a video that I found very sort of
  • 24:11informative in my own thinking about
  • 24:13systems theory and dynamical systems.
  • 24:15So in this video I want to say maybe
  • 24:18like a minute and a half long.
  • 24:21It's sort of outlines the process
  • 24:23of emergence for hurricane and so
  • 24:25hurricanes go through several.
  • 24:27We've sort of classified different stages,
  • 24:29whether that's tropical depression to
  • 24:30a tropical storm to a full blown hurricane,
  • 24:33and then we have different
  • 24:35categories of hurricanes.
  • 24:36But even though there are these unique
  • 24:39sort of stages that we have just sort
  • 24:42of arbitrarily come up with as humans.
  • 24:45There are these sort of continuous
  • 24:47change processes that lead you know
  • 24:49weather patterns to kind of move through
  • 24:51these different arbitrary stages.
  • 24:53So as we watch this video,
  • 24:55the thing I wanted to just pay attention
  • 24:58to is again this notion of complexity.
  • 25:01How different variables are
  • 25:02interacting with each other and
  • 25:04also pay attention to it will
  • 25:06show different arrows moving in
  • 25:08different directions to to
  • 25:10convey some these concepts.
  • 25:14Depends upon the following conditions
  • 25:17coinciding Alarge still and warm ocean
  • 25:19area with the surface temperature
  • 25:22that exceeds 26.5 degrees
  • 25:23Celsius over an extended period.
  • 25:25This allows a body of warm air to
  • 25:30develop above the ocean surface.
  • 25:33Low altitude winds are also
  • 25:35needed to form a tropical cyclone.
  • 25:38As air warms over the ocean,
  • 25:40it expands, becomes lighter and rises.
  • 25:43Other local winds blow in to
  • 25:45replace the air that has risen.
  • 25:47Then this air is also warmed and rises.
  • 25:52The rising air contains huge amounts of
  • 25:54moisture evaporated from the ocean surface.
  • 25:57As it rises, it cools,
  • 25:59condensing to form huge clouds about
  • 26:0210 kilometers up in the troposphere.
  • 26:05More warm air rushes in and
  • 26:07rises drawn by the draft above.
  • 26:10The amazing drafts are there carrying
  • 26:13moisture high into the atmosphere
  • 26:15so that these clouds eventually
  • 26:17become very thick and heavy.
  • 26:20Condensation then releases the latent
  • 26:22heat energy stored in the water vapor,
  • 26:25providing the cyclone with more power.
  • 26:27This creates a self sustaining heat cycle.
  • 26:31Drawn further upwards by the
  • 26:33new release of Energy,
  • 26:35the clouds can grow to 12
  • 26:38to 15 kilometers high.
  • 26:40The force created by the Earth's
  • 26:42rotation on a tilted axis,
  • 26:44the Coriolis effect,
  • 26:46causes rising currents of air to spiral
  • 26:49around the center of the tropical cyclone.
  • 26:52It is at this stage that the cyclone matures
  • 26:55and the eye of the storm is created.
  • 26:58As the air rises and cools.
  • 27:00Some of this dense air descends
  • 27:02to form the clear.
  • 27:04Still,
  • 27:04I as the cyclone rages around it.
  • 27:10So a couple of key ideas in here is that
  • 27:15as the hurricane is a cyclone emerges.
  • 27:19It's not simply that you have to
  • 27:21have wind speed at a certain level
  • 27:24temperature at a certain level have
  • 27:27to be over a suitable body of water.
  • 27:30There's a lot of conditional
  • 27:31interdependence amongst these variables.
  • 27:33It's the right wind speed at the
  • 27:36right temperature in the right place,
  • 27:38under the right conditions,
  • 27:40and if any of these variables shift
  • 27:43or are they off just a little bit,
  • 27:45it can actually dissolve the
  • 27:47entire system when aspect about.
  • 27:49Hurricane formation that I learned
  • 27:51about in the past few years doing
  • 27:54this work is that wind speeds
  • 27:56Tradewinds at very high altitude.
  • 27:58Have to actually be in this
  • 28:00very narrow band of velocity.
  • 28:02If the trade winds are not fast enough,
  • 28:05it does not provide enough momentum
  • 28:07for the hurricane to actually move in.
  • 28:10Things break apart.
  • 28:11Conversely, if the wind,
  • 28:13the trade wind speeds are too
  • 28:15high of a velocity,
  • 28:17it doesn't allow enough time for all
  • 28:19of these other processes to congeal.
  • 28:22And for man,
  • 28:23it basically breaks up the system
  • 28:25and So what we have here then.
  • 28:28Is a system where lots of variables
  • 28:31are interacting with each other
  • 28:33in those variables are changing in
  • 28:35very key ways that we start to see
  • 28:38feedback loops and if those feedback
  • 28:40loops in those change processes
  • 28:42aren't in the right configuration,
  • 28:44then in essence the hurricane doesn't
  • 28:46emerge and so this seems to be
  • 28:49something comperable to suicide that
  • 28:51the conceptual theoretical model that
  • 28:53we use in a lot of our research is
  • 28:56referred to as the suicidal mode,
  • 28:58and this is a graphic depiction of that.
  • 29:02And so a lot of people have
  • 29:04sometimes commented clinicians like,
  • 29:06well,
  • 29:06they're like all these sort of
  • 29:08arrows pointing to different pieces.
  • 29:10You know,
  • 29:10it's sort of like a complicated
  • 29:12model is compared to some of the more
  • 29:15classic like cognitive models or
  • 29:17the biomedical biobehavioral model,
  • 29:18so I've seen.
  • 29:19But what this really captures in
  • 29:21the suicidal motives,
  • 29:22this notion of a network that
  • 29:24lots of different variables are
  • 29:26influencing each other,
  • 29:27and they have to influence each other
  • 29:30in the right way at the right time.
  • 29:33In order for the conditions to be
  • 29:35right for suicidal crisis to emerge,
  • 29:38and then critically for suicidal
  • 29:40behaviors to emerge,
  • 29:41and some of the newer research
  • 29:43that's being done right now,
  • 29:45particularly IMA researchers that are
  • 29:47using network computational models,
  • 29:49is showing us that there actually
  • 29:51are multiple pathways suicide.
  • 29:53One of the limitations of a lot
  • 29:56of our traditional thinking about
  • 29:58suicide is that we focus on.
  • 30:00One or two risk variables or factors.
  • 30:03Hopelessness, perceived,
  • 30:04burdensomeness thwarted belonging.
  • 30:06This fearlessness about death or
  • 30:08some of the ones that are more
  • 30:11popular that are more well known.
  • 30:13But what we're seeing now using these
  • 30:17more complex dynamic models is that,
  • 30:19you know,
  • 30:20hopeless hopelessness is a primary
  • 30:22factor in firmly some suicidal people.
  • 30:25Perceived Burdensomeness is an
  • 30:27important variable for a different group
  • 30:29of individuals, but in essence.
  • 30:31The reason may be that we haven't
  • 30:33been better at preventing suicide
  • 30:35as we create these monolithic
  • 30:37models that don't have sufficient
  • 30:39complexity and therefore don't allow
  • 30:41us to fully understand the different
  • 30:43processes by which suicide might be
  • 30:45a common endpoint of another way of
  • 30:47saying this is sort of like there.
  • 30:49There might be multiple roads to Rome,
  • 30:52but because we assume there's
  • 30:53only one Rd to Rome,
  • 30:55we're not doing a good job setting
  • 30:57up the barriers on all of the
  • 31:00many different roads to block
  • 31:01their pathway to get to room.
  • 31:06I'll say one other point about
  • 31:08that that I didn't mention is that
  • 31:11this notion of interdependence,
  • 31:13where all of these many different factors
  • 31:15and variables influence each other,
  • 31:17and so you know depression.
  • 31:19Hopelessness, for instance,
  • 31:21if you experience an increase
  • 31:22in your hopelessness,
  • 31:24we might reasonably expect
  • 31:25an increase in depression.
  • 31:27Conversely, an increase in depression
  • 31:29might reasonably be expected
  • 31:30to increase once hopelessness,
  • 31:32and so we really can't sort
  • 31:34of parse out and separate.
  • 31:37The notion of depression and hopelessness
  • 31:39and other risk variables because
  • 31:41they all depend upon each other and
  • 31:43we have a study under review right
  • 31:45now or what we did is we use the
  • 31:47best scale for suicidal ideations.
  • 31:49And instead of modeling the scale for
  • 31:51suicidal ideations as a single total score,
  • 31:53which is how most of us are traditionally
  • 31:56used it in looking at that change process.
  • 31:58Overtime,
  • 31:58what we've done is we actually
  • 32:00took each of the individual items
  • 32:02and we said all of the items.
  • 32:04In a sense they can change
  • 32:06in any way they want.
  • 32:08Independence of each other and let's
  • 32:10see what happens in what we found
  • 32:13was that can sort of consistent with
  • 32:15the notion of emergency complexity
  • 32:18be different asset aspects or facets
  • 32:20of suicidal ideations under some
  • 32:23conditions were tightly correlated
  • 32:25with each other such that an increase
  • 32:27in the wish to die corresponded with
  • 32:29an increase in suicidal intent.
  • 32:31But there are other cases in
  • 32:34other circumstances where there
  • 32:35was no relationship between these
  • 32:37two otherwise supposedly.
  • 32:39Correlated facets of suicidal ideations,
  • 32:41and So what we're starting to see,
  • 32:43then,
  • 32:44is that there might be these sort
  • 32:46of different networks and Inter
  • 32:48related webs of risk variables and
  • 32:51processes that have implications.
  • 32:53For prevention is from a network perspective.
  • 32:56We can identify some of the key
  • 32:58variables that are most tightly
  • 33:00influential and interconnected
  • 33:02with other aspects of the network.
  • 33:05What we could start doing, then is targeting.
  • 33:08This is the one thing that if we
  • 33:11can change this one variable,
  • 33:13it has this downstream effect that
  • 33:16ripples out and starts to impact
  • 33:18other risk factors for suicide,
  • 33:20leading to a more efficient
  • 33:22reduction in suicidal crises.
  • 33:24Conversely,
  • 33:24if we're not targeting the
  • 33:26right piece of the network,
  • 33:28we might have interventions that are really,
  • 33:31really good at changing a
  • 33:33particular risk variable,
  • 33:34but that change isn't sufficiently
  • 33:36connected to other members of the network,
  • 33:39and so we don't see that downstream
  • 33:43effect in reduction in suicide risk.
  • 33:46So the 4th concept that I wanted to
  • 33:49talk about is that complex systems
  • 33:51behave in non proportional ways and
  • 33:53what this refers to is that you can.
  • 33:56You can sometimes put a whole lot
  • 33:58of stuff into a system to change
  • 34:01that system and in the system just
  • 34:04sort of ignores all of the input.
  • 34:06It doesn't really change how it behaves.
  • 34:09Conversely you see the opposite
  • 34:11where we don't really do a whole lot
  • 34:14in terms of input or intervention.
  • 34:16And then the the system will
  • 34:18dramatically change in how
  • 34:20it behaves, and so a little bit
  • 34:23and sometimes paying big dividends.
  • 34:26Is a whole lot of input doesn't
  • 34:28really seem to have any effect.
  • 34:30This is like one of those really
  • 34:32frustrating aspects of clinical practice
  • 34:33where some of our patients that come in.
  • 34:35We just really hit them with a lot of
  • 34:38interventions. We really are group,
  • 34:39you know they do therapy.
  • 34:41They do meds.
  • 34:42Did you all sorts of stuff and
  • 34:43then we don't really see a change,
  • 34:46whereas other patients it
  • 34:47seems like they come in.
  • 34:48We have clinicians or like I didn't
  • 34:50really even do anything and now all of
  • 34:52a sudden there dramatically better and
  • 34:54we really don't understand why there
  • 34:56are these differential response patterns.
  • 34:57In one of the possibilities of that well,
  • 35:00complex systems behave
  • 35:01in non proportional ways.
  • 35:03There isn't any guarantee that what you
  • 35:05put in will have a proportional output,
  • 35:08and so I have another video to kind of
  • 35:11display and highlight some of this.
  • 35:13This is a gymnastics example
  • 35:15before I introduces further.
  • 35:16I'll encourage you might want to adjust
  • 35:18your volume a little bit down on this one.
  • 35:22The volume on this video tends to
  • 35:24be a lot louder than the last one,
  • 35:27and so perhaps we can avert some
  • 35:29panic attacks here if we.
  • 35:31Give you a little bit of warning.
  • 35:33This is a 30 second video.
  • 35:35This is Simone Biles.
  • 35:37I'm sure everybody recognizes her
  • 35:39and she's doing one of her routines
  • 35:41and I found that as I was really
  • 35:43trying to understand the concepts of
  • 35:45non proportionality that gymnastics
  • 35:47was like this.
  • 35:48Really really great way of kind
  • 35:50of capturing it.
  • 35:51Embodying this notion of non proportionality.
  • 35:53So let's let's watch your 32nd routine here.
  • 36:19That's I warned a second Trisha
  • 36:21that usually this video and
  • 36:23practice runs works beautifully.
  • 36:25And then during the live
  • 36:27presentation this happens.
  • 36:28Let me try an alternative approach.
  • 36:30Going to switch to you.
  • 36:33Another video here.
  • 36:35Let's see if this works better.
  • 36:45Share this instead.
  • 36:49The same video I'm just going to use
  • 36:52the raw video and maybe that will.
  • 36:54Correct the problem, right? Let's try now.
  • 37:32Right, so I shared that video because
  • 37:36I think it really helps to capture
  • 37:40that notion of that non proportionality
  • 37:43where here we have this undisputed,
  • 37:47very accomplished.
  • 37:48Athlete who's very good at what she does.
  • 37:52Ann is 1 lots of medals and is very very
  • 37:56accomplished and is doing quite well on her.
  • 38:00Routine for the first 29 1/2 seconds
  • 38:02and then she does a split an she lands
  • 38:05on the beam and up until that moment is
  • 38:08sort of like we had absolutely no way of
  • 38:11knowing that she was actually going to fall.
  • 38:14And from the time that we see her
  • 38:17legs shooting up for her trying to
  • 38:19touch her balance to her feet being
  • 38:21on the ground with the fall,
  • 38:23it's about half of a second
  • 38:25so it's very dramatic.
  • 38:27Very rapid,
  • 38:27seemed to come out of the blue despite.
  • 38:30All of this good performance
  • 38:32up until that point.
  • 38:34And so this really sort of aligns
  • 38:36with that notion of the projects
  • 38:39that John was working on with NASA,
  • 38:41as well as some of what we
  • 38:44know to be true with suicide.
  • 38:46That it seems to sometimes come out of
  • 38:49the blue without much advanced notice.
  • 38:52Well that in essence this fall
  • 38:54here that we saw in gymnastics.
  • 38:57It's sort of like we don't really
  • 39:00know what contributed to it.
  • 39:01We can probably infer that maybe she
  • 39:04just landed in a way where her center
  • 39:07of gravity was perhaps just a little
  • 39:09bit off from where she wanted it to be.
  • 39:12Maybe her,
  • 39:13maybe her foot was just twisted in a slightly
  • 39:16different way than what she was expecting.
  • 39:18We really don't know, but a very,
  • 39:21very slight shift in the mechanics of
  • 39:23the move led to this dramatic outcome,
  • 39:25and suicide seems to function
  • 39:27in the same way.
  • 39:29So mathematically,
  • 39:29this is referred to as a catastrophic
  • 39:33change where the system suddenly
  • 39:35shifts from its prior behavior to a
  • 39:38completely different behavioral pattern,
  • 39:40and we've started to think about
  • 39:43catastrophic change processes and apply
  • 39:46it to our work with suicidal individuals.
  • 39:49And we're actually finding it.
  • 39:51It's a very useful model,
  • 39:53and the reason we think this is
  • 39:57the case is that.
  • 39:59Again, from assistance.
  • 40:00Perspective to have these sort of
  • 40:02sudden catastrophic changes,
  • 40:04there's another inherent assumption
  • 40:06embedded within non proportionality,
  • 40:07which is the notion of a tipping
  • 40:10point where there exists two distinct
  • 40:12distinct and discrete States and in
  • 40:15between these two separate states of
  • 40:18being is this tipping point such that
  • 40:20if you fall just shy of the tipping point,
  • 40:24you stay in one particular state.
  • 40:27If however you just barely move
  • 40:29over the cusp of that tipping point.
  • 40:32You can shift into a new state,
  • 40:35and indeed there's now an accumulation
  • 40:37of evidence indicating that this is
  • 40:39actually how suicide risk works.
  • 40:41So this is a study here, but Tracy Witty.
  • 40:44They've done.
  • 40:45Taxa,
  • 40:45metric analysis with large sample of
  • 40:47suicidal individuals and found that
  • 40:49there was a subgroup of suicidal
  • 40:51individuals who
  • 40:52had very, very high risk for suicide,
  • 40:54and they were categorically
  • 40:56distinct from everybody else.
  • 40:57These results were actually replicated by
  • 40:59team down at Baylor College of Medicine,
  • 41:02with psychiatric inpatients,
  • 41:03and found that.
  • 41:04Among psychiatric inpatients,
  • 41:05there's a small group that was really,
  • 41:07really high risk for suicide and then
  • 41:09everybody else was in a lower risk state.
  • 41:12We've seen this as well.
  • 41:13An epidemiological study.
  • 41:14This is a. Project done better.
  • 41:17Army Stars research team led by Ron Kessler.
  • 41:20What they did was they took a large sample
  • 41:22of soldiers who had been discharged
  • 41:25from psychiatric inpatient care,
  • 41:26used machine learning to split
  • 41:28them up into vent tiles.
  • 41:30And then they looked at they rank
  • 41:32ordered the vent tiles in terms of
  • 41:35overall risk level and then mapped
  • 41:37onto who and where are the suicide
  • 41:40deaths occur and what they found
  • 41:42was that over half of the suicide
  • 41:44deaths occur in the first subgroup.
  • 41:46The 1st event tile.
  • 41:48This is on the left hand side
  • 41:50of the screen and you see the
  • 41:52rest of the 19 vent tiles.
  • 41:54There's not as much difference between
  • 41:56them is a little bit of variability.
  • 41:58Groups two and three or a little
  • 42:00bit higher risk than the others,
  • 42:02but not dramatically so.
  • 42:04So this also suggests that there's
  • 42:06this sort of tipping point.
  • 42:07There's one small subgroup that accounts
  • 42:09for more than half of the suicides,
  • 42:11and then there's everybody else
  • 42:13but the rest of the 95% of the
  • 42:15population now on the right hand side.
  • 42:17This is not hypothetical.
  • 42:19Model where if suicide risk was a continuum,
  • 42:22this sort of unidimensional gradual change
  • 42:25process moving from lower to higher risk.
  • 42:27The graph on the right hand side
  • 42:30is what Kessler should have found.
  • 42:32There should have been the sort of
  • 42:35incremental changes in the probability
  • 42:37of suicide deaths within each ventile,
  • 42:39but that's of course not what they found.
  • 42:43We've since mathematically modeled
  • 42:44suicidal Ideations on a session by
  • 42:47session basis within psychiatric
  • 42:49outpatient psychiatric outpatients,
  • 42:51and what we found was that patients
  • 42:53who had a history of multiple suicide
  • 42:57attempts at the start of treatment,
  • 43:00they had a unique change process that
  • 43:03was characterized by sudden dramatic
  • 43:05Upson Downs in suicidal ideations
  • 43:07on a session to session basis.
  • 43:10It was like a sawtooth pattern.
  • 43:13In that that sawtooth pattern
  • 43:15is consistent with the existence
  • 43:17of two discrete states of risk,
  • 43:18where in essence the patients are
  • 43:21bouncing back and forth between
  • 43:22low and high risk that they're
  • 43:24not doing it in a gross, gradual,
  • 43:26slow manner because there's a tipping point,
  • 43:28and so they can be.
  • 43:30It's sort of like a binary or
  • 43:32a one or a zero.
  • 43:34There is no in between,
  • 43:35and there is a distinct subgroup of patients
  • 43:38who seemed to conform to this change process,
  • 43:40and So what that means is,
  • 43:42I think.
  • 43:43Our traditional ways of thinking
  • 43:45about suicide risk in this sort
  • 43:48of unit dimensional hierarchy,
  • 43:49where at the bottom we have
  • 43:51things like I don't
  • 43:53want to live anymore.
  • 43:55I don't want to be alive,
  • 43:57moving to active suicidal ideations,
  • 43:59moving to planning,
  • 44:00moving to preparatory behavior,
  • 44:02and then at the highest level,
  • 44:04suicidal behavior where there's at least
  • 44:07a significant subgroup of individuals
  • 44:09who do not conform to this pathway.
  • 44:11In this hierarchical model,
  • 44:13some patients do seem to follow
  • 44:15this gradual change process.
  • 44:17But there's another substantial minority.
  • 44:19At the very least,
  • 44:20that don't follow this,
  • 44:22and So what this means is we
  • 44:24probably need to change how we
  • 44:26conceptualize suicide risk.
  • 44:28And so we've now informed
  • 44:30by the notion of emergence,
  • 44:32move towards a 3 dimensional model of suicide
  • 44:35risk and and this figure on the screen.
  • 44:38With this sort of curved claim of
  • 44:40suicide risk is what's referred
  • 44:42to as a cusp catastrophe model.
  • 44:45So this provides us with.
  • 44:47A way to understand why is it
  • 44:49that some individuals seem to
  • 44:51have this gradual change process.
  • 44:53They sort of slowly move through these
  • 44:56incremental stages towards suicide,
  • 44:57whereas other individuals seem to
  • 44:59very suddenly shift from a low
  • 45:02risk state to a high risk state,
  • 45:04and so let me demonstrate several of
  • 45:07the kind of a key change processes here.
  • 45:10This first pathways pathway a.
  • 45:12This is sort of the classic
  • 45:14way of thinking about suicide,
  • 45:16it's unidimensional.
  • 45:16As you move up the surface plane that
  • 45:19corresponds to increasing suicide risk,
  • 45:21and these individuals then follow back down.
  • 45:23They kind of move up and down Hill,
  • 45:26and the way that this typically
  • 45:28manifests is those fluctuations in
  • 45:29suicide rates that have inclined in,
  • 45:31as noted in that,
  • 45:32like very first one of the very first
  • 45:35slides that I've showed you today.
  • 45:37And so we do see that people
  • 45:39have good days and bad days that
  • 45:42they have these fluctuations,
  • 45:43but they tend to be somewhat
  • 45:46smooth and gradual.
  • 45:47These individuals often come
  • 45:48in to mental health care.
  • 45:50We typically diagnosed him with
  • 45:52like mood disorders and this is,
  • 45:54I think the sort of preponderance of
  • 45:56the people that we have traditionally
  • 45:58thought about when it comes to suicide.
  • 46:01We didn't have a different group of
  • 46:03individuals who are located in a
  • 46:05different place on this behavior surface.
  • 46:08They also move up and down Hill,
  • 46:10but because they are located in a
  • 46:12place of the this curved plane that
  • 46:15corresponds to a steeper slope,
  • 46:17they experience much more rapid
  • 46:19increases in rapid decreases in suicide risk.
  • 46:22It's no different than if we were
  • 46:24to kind of like roll a ball up
  • 46:27and down a really steep Hill.
  • 46:29The ball would start to roll up very quickly,
  • 46:32stop, and then come back down very rapidly.
  • 46:35In the steeper the slope,
  • 46:37the faster that return process would be.
  • 46:40Now this this sort of sub group of
  • 46:43individuals seems to characterize
  • 46:45those who are diagnosed with
  • 46:47borderline personality disorder and
  • 46:49have other related conditions that
  • 46:51are characterized by deficient self
  • 46:53regulatory processes where we're
  • 46:55more likely to see a sawtooth pattern
  • 46:58and then one of the key aspects of
  • 47:02this is that that up and down that
  • 47:05dramatic up and down actually increases
  • 47:07their vulnerability for suddenly
  • 47:09shifting from a low risk state.
  • 47:12To a very high risk state from the bottom
  • 47:14of the surface to the top of the surface,
  • 47:17and that was actually what David wrote,
  • 47:19and I found in some of our initial
  • 47:22research with multiple attempters
  • 47:23and we now have a new study we just
  • 47:26published in behaviour research and
  • 47:27therapy in the past year where we
  • 47:30took patient psychiatric out patients
  • 47:31who had attempted 1 one time in their
  • 47:33life and then we mapped out their
  • 47:36fluctuations in suicidal ideations over
  • 47:37therapy and what we found was that the
  • 47:40patients who had larger fluctuations
  • 47:41on a session by session basis.
  • 47:44Or significantly more likely to
  • 47:46attempt suicide during treatment
  • 47:48or soon after treatment.
  • 47:50And so these individuals also,
  • 47:52I think,
  • 47:53are more likely to come in for
  • 47:55mental health treatment.
  • 47:57And so this is where a lot of our
  • 47:59work that Seth mentioned in his
  • 48:02introduction on cognitive behavioral
  • 48:04therapy for suicide prevention,
  • 48:06I think has been really impactful.
  • 48:08I think other treatments in addition
  • 48:10to BTI but also dialectical behavior
  • 48:12therapy has a lot of implications here.
  • 48:15What we were finding is that these
  • 48:17treatments seem to sort of calm the storm
  • 48:20where the fluctuations in suicidal ideations.
  • 48:23Over the course of treatment tend
  • 48:25to settle down so that there is a
  • 48:28decreased probability that a person
  • 48:29will suddenly shift to a high risk
  • 48:32state where suicidal behavior
  • 48:34is more likely to occur.
  • 48:35The key aspect about some of our
  • 48:38findings with BTI is that we see
  • 48:40treatment effects very quickly,
  • 48:42typically within three months
  • 48:43of starting treatment,
  • 48:44and so not only is it a long lasting
  • 48:47effect up to two years post baseline,
  • 48:50but we tend to see pretty early
  • 48:53separation between treatment groups.
  • 48:55Another related intervention
  • 48:56that that we've worked on a lot
  • 48:59as crisis response planning,
  • 49:00which serves as sort of like the kind
  • 49:03of a precursor to safety planning,
  • 49:06which a lot of people are familiar with.
  • 49:09Crisis response planning was,
  • 49:10apart of BTE,
  • 49:11and then we subsequently extracted
  • 49:13this procedure and tested it as a
  • 49:16standalone intervention and emergency
  • 49:17department's as well as walk in clinics
  • 49:20for UNE scheduled crisis evaluations.
  • 49:22Whereas what we do is we hand
  • 49:25write on an index card what?
  • 49:27Ultimately,
  • 49:28amounts to sort of a self regulation plan.
  • 49:30How to know when you're getting upset,
  • 49:33how to know when you're heading uphill.
  • 49:35And then.
  • 49:36Here's a list of things that you can
  • 49:39do to stop yourself from progressing
  • 49:41towards a high risk state into self
  • 49:44regulate to kind of calm yourself back down.
  • 49:46We completed a randomized controlled
  • 49:48trial that we published in 2017
  • 49:51comparing crisis response planning
  • 49:52to treatment as usual and found 76%
  • 49:54reduction in suicide attempts over
  • 49:56the six month follow up period.
  • 49:58And so this.
  • 50:00Very simple.
  • 50:00Ateji that definitely takes 30 or 60 minutes.
  • 50:04We found this actually quite potent
  • 50:06for averting suicidal behaviors.
  • 50:08But then there's one last group that my
  • 50:11research programs increasingly focused on.
  • 50:13This is this pathway.
  • 50:14See, this is the.
  • 50:16This is the performance escape,
  • 50:18the catastrophic change group,
  • 50:19for whom suicidal behaviors
  • 50:21seemed to come out of the blue.
  • 50:23And you can see their position
  • 50:25on the behavior surface here.
  • 50:27What happens with this group is when they
  • 50:30experience an increase in suicide risk.
  • 50:32It's very gradual.
  • 50:33It's not very pronounced,
  • 50:35so maybe they only get to a point where
  • 50:38they're wishing that they weren't.
  • 50:40Around anymore they would.
  • 50:42They think they'd be better off dead,
  • 50:44but in some cases they're not
  • 50:46manifesting active forms of suicidal
  • 50:48ideations or even planning.
  • 50:50But then what happens is they hit
  • 50:52this curve in the behavior surface
  • 50:55and they sort of like teleport up
  • 50:58to the top where they see this very
  • 51:00sudden shift in the probability
  • 51:02for suicidal behavior in these
  • 51:04individuals are much less likely
  • 51:06to come to mental health care,
  • 51:08and it's because for the most part
  • 51:11they're existing in a low risk state.
  • 51:14And so when we say get help,
  • 51:16watch out for these warning signs
  • 51:18of suicide that largely focus
  • 51:19on active suicidal ideations.
  • 51:21It doesn't apply to these individuals.
  • 51:23This isn't the nature of their
  • 51:25subjective experience of suicide risk,
  • 51:26and so we end up missing this group,
  • 51:29and we keep encouraging.
  • 51:30Go get help to get help to get help,
  • 51:33but this is a group for whom that is
  • 51:36a message that just isn't hitting the
  • 51:38target in the way that we needed to.
  • 51:41So we know that about 40 to 50% of those
  • 51:44who attempt suicide sort of skipped.
  • 51:47The planning stage and we have
  • 51:49some newer data coming out.
  • 51:51Showing that manifestations of suicide
  • 51:53risk do not necessarily involve active
  • 51:56thinking about suicide where we are
  • 51:58now able to capture using a scale
  • 52:01called the Suicide cognition scale.
  • 52:03Things like I can't take this anymore.
  • 52:06I deserve to die.
  • 52:07No one can help me solve my problems.
  • 52:11These are perhaps thought processes
  • 52:13that signal this is someone who's
  • 52:16at elevated risk for suicide,
  • 52:18but they aren't necessarily experiencing
  • 52:20suicidal thoughts in the way that we
  • 52:23have traditionally conceptualized them.
  • 52:25Traditionally asked about that,
  • 52:27and so we're effectively missing
  • 52:30a large group of people.
  • 52:33And so this brings me into the kind of
  • 52:35the last idea of what we're working on,
  • 52:38which is the importance of means restriction,
  • 52:41especially with firearm owners.
  • 52:42So now a big portion of my research
  • 52:45is really focused on understanding.
  • 52:47Suicide risk amongst firearm owners
  • 52:49and what we're finding is that there
  • 52:52are really high risk subgroups of
  • 52:54gun owners who they typically own.
  • 52:57Guns for self protection purposes,
  • 52:59but they are much,
  • 53:00much more likely to engage in suicidal
  • 53:03behaviors because we're starting to
  • 53:05see now is that they have a number of
  • 53:09biobehavioral vulnerabilities to suicide.
  • 53:11They view.
  • 53:12The world is a dangerous place.
  • 53:14They perceive hostility and others
  • 53:17actions even with no hostility.
  • 53:19Exists in this seems to degrade
  • 53:21cognitive control processes,
  • 53:22which increases the propensity for them to
  • 53:24engage in these dysregulated behaviors,
  • 53:26and if they have a loaded weapon during
  • 53:28moments of acute distress when they suddenly
  • 53:31shift to a high probability high risk state,
  • 53:34it weaponizes their emotional States
  • 53:36and they die very quickly and we do
  • 53:39not have a way to intervene with them,
  • 53:41and so we do have now a new study.
  • 53:44It's impressed with the American
  • 53:46Journal of Public Health,
  • 53:47that's hopefully will be coming out
  • 53:49in the next few months, showing that.
  • 53:51And we brought gun owners into our lab
  • 53:55and we sat down with them and we talked
  • 53:59about safe gun storage and using gun safes,
  • 54:02trigger locks,
  • 54:03things like that and what we found
  • 54:05was that very simple conversations
  • 54:08guided by motivational interviewing
  • 54:10principles significantly increased
  • 54:11the likelihood of gun owners actually
  • 54:14using safe storage methods which we
  • 54:16know from epidemiological data are
  • 54:18correlated with dramatic reductions
  • 54:20in suicide mortality.
  • 54:22We now have an R61 that we're about
  • 54:24to start enrollment on to further
  • 54:26understand a lot of these processes,
  • 54:29but I think this is in many ways,
  • 54:31one of the most important directions in
  • 54:34suicide prevention is to move outside
  • 54:36of the healthcare system because there
  • 54:38is a subgroup who don't have access,
  • 54:40are not going to seek out mental
  • 54:43health treatment,
  • 54:44and during those moments of a
  • 54:45sudden onset of acute despair,
  • 54:47we just don't have time to find
  • 54:49them in to intervene with them.
  • 54:52So we need to change the environment.
  • 54:55So that we suicide proof their lives in,
  • 54:58decrease the probability of a fatal outcome.
  • 55:03It's all,
  • 55:04so that's that's kind of where we're at.
  • 55:06With the research a lot more
  • 55:09to be done understandably,
  • 55:10but I'm happy to take any questions that
  • 55:13anyone might have in the remaining time,
  • 55:15and likewise,
  • 55:16if you have to drop off,
  • 55:18totally understandable and by all
  • 55:20means feel free to shoot me an email
  • 55:23afterwards with any questions or
  • 55:24follow up thoughts that you might have.
  • 55:27So thank you.
  • 55:30Thanks Greg, if there any questions
  • 55:33folks wanna put them into the chat
  • 55:36chat room that would be great.
  • 55:38Two things I'd never say.
  • 55:39Seeing you give this talk before
  • 55:41Craig so you know two things I
  • 55:43thought were really interesting.
  • 55:44One was on tipping points. You know,
  • 55:46when you think about the medical model of.
  • 55:50Of of that, you presented around suicidality.
  • 55:52This is true, I think,
  • 55:54across many different medical diseases.
  • 55:56So kidney function,
  • 55:57for instance, right?
  • 55:58We learn in medical school that
  • 56:00your kidney function declines
  • 56:01gradually throughout your lifetime,
  • 56:03and then boom,
  • 56:04all of a sudden something happens
  • 56:06and you cross a tipping point,
  • 56:08and then all of a sudden you're
  • 56:11creating starts going up.
  • 56:12You know you're you're essentially
  • 56:14entering kidney failure even
  • 56:15though you've been in decline.
  • 56:17There's been these issues all along.
  • 56:19There's a tipping point.
  • 56:20And same with coronary artery disease
  • 56:23where your things are going on in the
  • 56:25background for an extended period,
  • 56:26sometimes something can happen early.
  • 56:28Maybe you're taking a drug abuse.
  • 56:30Maybe there's another disease
  • 56:31process going on,
  • 56:32but then boom,
  • 56:33all of a sudden there's a myocardial
  • 56:35infarction because something
  • 56:36is occurring in the vessel that
  • 56:38was either an it's similar to
  • 56:39the 3D model you presented,
  • 56:41where it can be gradual or all
  • 56:43of a sudden something can happen
  • 56:45and you just jump up the curve.
  • 56:47So I really like that,
  • 56:48right?
  • 56:50Yeah, it's the notion of tipping
  • 56:52points is yeah, it's widely used.
  • 56:55In other words not widely used,
  • 56:58but it's sort of applicable
  • 57:00and well understood,
  • 57:01and other branches of medicine but
  • 57:04also other Sciences within biology,
  • 57:06chemistry, physics.
  • 57:07All of these processes are quite
  • 57:10widely used in even the notion of
  • 57:13catastrophic change processes.
  • 57:14And so I think there's a lot of potential
  • 57:18value and thinking about change.
  • 57:21In these more complicated ways and
  • 57:22the good news is that we do have a
  • 57:25lot of precedent and a lot of examples
  • 57:27from other scientific disciplines,
  • 57:29and so it's really now just sort of
  • 57:31a matter of applying the methods
  • 57:32in the computational concepts
  • 57:34to the work that we do.
  • 57:37Then there's a question from Tammy.
  • 57:41Say I'll skip over the very nice words
  • 57:43at the beginning of the question,
  • 57:45although everyone can read it,
  • 57:47any data you're aware of Crag that's
  • 57:49been collected with micro longitudinal
  • 57:51methods like EMA or experience
  • 57:52sampling with firearm owners.
  • 57:56So not yet, but that's actually so.
  • 57:58There are 61 that I just received.
  • 58:00We are going to do that this year,
  • 58:03so January is when we're
  • 58:04hoping to start enrollment.
  • 58:05What we'll do is the design of our study will
  • 58:08be bringing gun owners and non gun owners.
  • 58:11And then we're looking at two
  • 58:13different subgroups of gun owners,
  • 58:14those who carry a firearm on a
  • 58:17regular basis and those who do not.
  • 58:19Maybe they have a gun in their house,
  • 58:21but they don't necessarily carry
  • 58:23it with them on a regular basis.
  • 58:26And yeah, we'll be hanging them six
  • 58:29times a day for four weeks to see.
  • 58:32Like what are they doing?
  • 58:33Who are they with will be asked about.
  • 58:36Mood will also be think
  • 58:38about suicidal ideations.
  • 58:39So Needless to say we have a pretty
  • 58:41robust risk management protocol in
  • 58:43place because consultation with
  • 58:45other suicide researchers using MA.
  • 58:47Some of the comments I've heard is,
  • 58:50like you know,
  • 58:51we all know intuitively that probably some
  • 58:53of our participants are carrying a weapon,
  • 58:56but.
  • 58:56This is the first time that you actually
  • 58:59know they'll be carrying a weapon when
  • 59:01they're suicidal and that sort of
  • 59:03changes the scariness of the project,
  • 59:06so we have a pretty robust risk management
  • 59:08protocol in one of the hypothesis we have it.
  • 59:11We will be asking the mini may are
  • 59:14you carrying a firearm right now,
  • 59:16or is there a firearm within?
  • 59:19I can't remember the wording.
  • 59:21Basically,
  • 59:21is there something firearm
  • 59:23within reach something like that?
  • 59:26And one of our hypothesis is
  • 59:28that when someone is in physical
  • 59:30possession of a firearm,
  • 59:32that might increase their
  • 59:35emotional liability.
  • 59:36Their reactivity to stressful events,
  • 59:37so my hope is that maybe maybe
  • 59:40in a couple of years, yeah,
  • 59:42we'll be able to have some of
  • 59:44those those data available.
  • 59:46You know on the group this
  • 59:48is just a question for me.
  • 59:50It's not in the chat box.
  • 59:51And please if anyone has questions,
  • 59:53feel free to jot them down in the box.
  • 59:55But I'm thinking about that
  • 59:57third group on your 3D model.
  • 59:59I've done some work with health.
  • 01:00:00And where they're looking for signals
  • 01:00:02and in non behavioral health claims
  • 01:00:04data so people are not feeling well,
  • 01:00:05they're going to primary care.
  • 01:00:06They are finding flags that seem to be
  • 01:00:08predictive of eventual suicide attempts,
  • 01:00:10even though the people are actually
  • 01:00:11not in any sort of mental health
  • 01:00:13care based on their claims data.
  • 01:00:14Do you have any sense of whether or
  • 01:00:16not I mean have you looked at that?
  • 01:00:18Is that something that's come up
  • 01:00:20as to are there going to be signals
  • 01:00:22that we can acquire to reach out?
  • 01:00:25The so the the one project we did
  • 01:00:28that was not tide to like medical
  • 01:00:31record data was social media project
  • 01:00:34is actually funded by Department
  • 01:00:37of Defense several years ago and we
  • 01:00:41basically content coded a years worth of.
  • 01:00:44It was mostly Facebook posts.
  • 01:00:47Amongst users personnel who died by
  • 01:00:49suicide and then a control group,
  • 01:00:52they died of other non suicide causes
  • 01:00:54and we looked at we kind of compared
  • 01:00:57what was on their social media
  • 01:01:00accounts and one of the findings was,
  • 01:01:02you know we did find a statistically
  • 01:01:05significant higher rate of,
  • 01:01:07you know post that were hopeless
  • 01:01:09and despondent and depressed.
  • 01:01:10And had those types of themes.
  • 01:01:13They were particularly huge
  • 01:01:15effect sizes once we applied.
  • 01:01:17These sort of dynamical systems
  • 01:01:19models what we found was that yeah,
  • 01:01:21that was like a game changer in one of
  • 01:01:24the key findings was that we that was
  • 01:01:27where we started to see the increase
  • 01:01:30in instability in certain variables
  • 01:01:32was a very very clear indicator.
  • 01:01:34And what was interesting is as we got
  • 01:01:37closer and closer to the date of death,
  • 01:01:40those change processes got
  • 01:01:42more and more pronounced.
  • 01:01:43The second key finding was that we
  • 01:01:46found that it was certain sequences.
  • 01:01:48Of variables, so if you had,
  • 01:01:51say variable in variable,
  • 01:01:52feed if variable a consistently
  • 01:01:54happened right before variable
  • 01:01:56B that signaled suicide.
  • 01:01:57But if variable B came
  • 01:01:59right before variable A,
  • 01:02:01it did not signal suicide.
  • 01:02:03So that sort of notion of
  • 01:02:05sequencing ended up being really,
  • 01:02:07really key and then an independent
  • 01:02:09team led by Glenn Coppersmith and
  • 01:02:11they actually used the technique
  • 01:02:13faceoffs presented at a conference.
  • 01:02:15They went back to use it on Twitter data,
  • 01:02:19and they called it Micro patterns,
  • 01:02:21and they found that.
  • 01:02:22These micro patterns of sequencing in
  • 01:02:25certain orders doubled the amount of
  • 01:02:27information and predictive ability
  • 01:02:29to identify not only suicide but
  • 01:02:31also like schizophrenia, anxiety,
  • 01:02:33depression.
  • 01:02:33So we have
  • 01:02:35two questions. Actually,
  • 01:02:36I think related to your EPI scenario,
  • 01:02:38so I'll jump to those real quick.
  • 01:02:42Alec was asking about the dependent variable.
  • 01:02:45Do any of the conclusions you have
  • 01:02:47presented applied differently according
  • 01:02:48to whether we're looking at attempts
  • 01:02:50that could have ended in death?
  • 01:02:54Yeah, so we've not gotten to
  • 01:02:56that point yet because our
  • 01:02:58samples haven't been big enough.
  • 01:03:00We do have some studies
  • 01:03:02where death was the outcome.
  • 01:03:04We have studies where attempts were
  • 01:03:07the outcomes and so the general
  • 01:03:09concept seemed to be the same,
  • 01:03:12but I think the question is well
  • 01:03:14placed in it is something that I've
  • 01:03:16wondered about is are are there
  • 01:03:19differences between highly lethal and
  • 01:03:21less lethal suicidal behaviors that?
  • 01:03:24Yeah, sort of those.
  • 01:03:26Those sudden shifts to high risk
  • 01:03:28states we see at least are thinking
  • 01:03:31right now is perhaps more relevant
  • 01:03:34for things like firearm suicide
  • 01:03:37and other highly lethal methods.
  • 01:03:39Whereas pathways A&B,
  • 01:03:41those are individuals who tend to select
  • 01:03:44less lethal methods like medications,
  • 01:03:46and so there may actually be
  • 01:03:49different change processes that signal
  • 01:03:51lethal versus non lethal outcomes.
  • 01:03:53I would love to.
  • 01:03:55Hopefully get a data set
  • 01:03:57large enough where we can
  • 01:03:59test that out more.
  • 01:04:00Yeah, one of the things I loved about
  • 01:04:03your work is to focus on attempts,
  • 01:04:06which is like heart attacks, right?
  • 01:04:08It potentially lethal,
  • 01:04:09and it relates to Alex,
  • 01:04:10question around that.
  • 01:04:11And then Sally asked about alcohol
  • 01:04:14consumption and whether or not that
  • 01:04:16teleports someone to severe risk.
  • 01:04:19Yeah, yeah, that's that's a great question,
  • 01:04:22and that's that's something
  • 01:04:24that we wondered about as well.
  • 01:04:27We don't know for sure, but you know,
  • 01:04:31so there's some evidence outside of
  • 01:04:33the suicide field that acute alcohol
  • 01:04:37intoxication perhaps attenuates.
  • 01:04:38Fluctuations in suicide risk owns a
  • 01:04:40person down and oftentimes a sort of
  • 01:04:42the motive for alcohol consumption.
  • 01:04:43As I'm up and down,
  • 01:04:45I kind of feel out of control.
  • 01:04:48So I drink alcohol and it numbs
  • 01:04:50and sort of pegs these Upson
  • 01:04:52downs that they're less extreme.
  • 01:04:54But then there are other individuals
  • 01:04:56for whom alcohol consumption sort of.
  • 01:04:58Yeah, sort of ramps them up and make some
  • 01:05:01more vulnerable to these sudden shifts.
  • 01:05:03So we will be we.
  • 01:05:05That's one of the other things
  • 01:05:07we are going to look at.
  • 01:05:09Some of our PMA studies is how does
  • 01:05:11acute alcohol consumption intoxication
  • 01:05:13perhaps alter change processes?
  • 01:05:15My suspicion what I really think
  • 01:05:17we'll probably find is that
  • 01:05:19there is going to be sort of a.
  • 01:05:22A conditional finding where it
  • 01:05:24will be some for some people.
  • 01:05:28Alcohol consumption and those sort of
  • 01:05:30calm them down and then for others
  • 01:05:33it increases their reactivity,
  • 01:05:35which I think would be really helpful
  • 01:05:38because that might help us to more
  • 01:05:41accurately target different subgroups
  • 01:05:42of patients and individuals for alcohol
  • 01:05:45consumption within their treatment plans.
  • 01:05:50There's another question that
  • 01:05:51came in between those two,
  • 01:05:53but it wasn't as related to what
  • 01:05:55you had said, so I hold it back,
  • 01:05:58which is the international
  • 01:06:00variability safe to countries.
  • 01:06:01I think she meant the country Australia.
  • 01:06:04Where they ban access to firearms
  • 01:06:06in a more stringent way?
  • 01:06:08Can you comment on that?
  • 01:06:10Yeah, so there certainly are.
  • 01:06:13You know, lower suicide rates and.
  • 01:06:17In countries with more
  • 01:06:18stringent access to firearms,
  • 01:06:20what one of the things that related
  • 01:06:22to this when we look at just sort of
  • 01:06:25means restriction in general is that
  • 01:06:27there are sort of international and
  • 01:06:29cultural variability regarding this,
  • 01:06:30and so one of the key assumptions about
  • 01:06:33means restriction in general that we
  • 01:06:35talk about is that the method that
  • 01:06:37you're going to restrict needs to be
  • 01:06:40sufficiently lethal to have an effect,
  • 01:06:42so it has it has to kill people with
  • 01:06:44a high enough rate that restricting
  • 01:06:46access will actually make a difference.
  • 01:06:49And then Secondly,
  • 01:06:50the method has to be common enough
  • 01:06:53within the population that again
  • 01:06:55restricting it would potentially
  • 01:06:57reduce an observable notable,
  • 01:07:00meaningful proportion of the population.
  • 01:07:02And so yeah,
  • 01:07:03in many international spaces
  • 01:07:05in other nations,
  • 01:07:06firearms isn't really sort of initiative.
  • 01:07:09Are no firearm suicides are
  • 01:07:11very little because of their
  • 01:07:14limited access to that method,
  • 01:07:16but in those nations they typically look at.
  • 01:07:20Other methods in from a means
  • 01:07:22restriction perspective.
  • 01:07:23They would target something else so.
  • 01:07:26The most classic examples of this
  • 01:07:29historically was in Sri Lanka,
  • 01:07:31Indonesia or maybe it was Indonesia
  • 01:07:34and some other Pacific island nations.
  • 01:07:37It was poisoning with fertilizer.
  • 01:07:40Someo farmers would drink there.
  • 01:07:42Basically fertilizer to kill themselves in.
  • 01:07:45So in these nations they restricted
  • 01:07:48basically the sale of pesticides and
  • 01:07:51fertilizers that that removed some of
  • 01:07:54the most toxic chemicals and agents and so.
  • 01:07:58In essence, former still had the tools
  • 01:08:01that they need to do their jobs,
  • 01:08:04but now they were less dangerous and
  • 01:08:07so if they drink pesticide or whatnot,
  • 01:08:10they were less likely to die as a result.
  • 01:08:14In UK it was carbon monoxide
  • 01:08:17being sent to homes to fuel ovens,
  • 01:08:20things like that,
  • 01:08:21and some of the Scandinavian
  • 01:08:23nations is restricting paracetamol.
  • 01:08:26And so,
  • 01:08:27so in the United States,
  • 01:08:28it's guns and I don't think will ever
  • 01:08:31been the curve on suicide until we
  • 01:08:34start really thinking about firearms as
  • 01:08:36a key central aspect of a conference.
  • 01:08:39Is suicide prevention plan.
  • 01:08:43Um, we have a very long statement
  • 01:08:45in question from Shelly.
  • 01:08:47I don't know if she wants to
  • 01:08:50actually go on audio and say it.
  • 01:08:53I mean, I think.
  • 01:08:55Well, that's thought about just
  • 01:08:57sort of a question comment crag
  • 01:08:59because when I think about some of
  • 01:09:01the questions in the talking about
  • 01:09:03because 'cause this shell is question
  • 01:09:05comment was really around a bunch
  • 01:09:07of linear variables and then some
  • 01:09:08unpredictable variable or exponential.
  • 01:09:10I think you know comes in from
  • 01:09:12some direction and then pushes
  • 01:09:14everything over the tipping point.
  • 01:09:15But I think on the treatment side
  • 01:09:17or intervention side one of the
  • 01:09:19things that I find interesting.
  • 01:09:21If you take time to really think about.
  • 01:09:24Crisis response planning.
  • 01:09:26CBT in your work.
  • 01:09:28In some ways,
  • 01:09:29it helps neutralize the unpredictability
  • 01:09:31of what those things are.
  • 01:09:34Because you're identifying
  • 01:09:35specific things that will either
  • 01:09:38distract or soothe the storm.
  • 01:09:40You know that are not 100% likely,
  • 01:09:42but at least are highly likely,
  • 01:09:44and so some of that is in the crisis
  • 01:09:46response plan some more of it is in
  • 01:09:48the CBT in terms of better identifying
  • 01:09:50when those storms are brewing.
  • 01:09:52But can you comment on that?
  • 01:09:54Like why is it so effective?
  • 01:09:57Yeah, I think it's you.
  • 01:10:01So it doesn't sort of like provides
  • 01:10:03this sort of buffer zone between these
  • 01:10:05two distinct States and so in essence,
  • 01:10:08the first piece of it is how to
  • 01:10:10know when you're heading towards
  • 01:10:12a higher state and being able to
  • 01:10:15recognize your own personal indicators
  • 01:10:17or warning signs of that process,
  • 01:10:19and then so now that you're aware of,
  • 01:10:22like Now you know that you're
  • 01:10:24heading towards that space,
  • 01:10:25then the rest of the plan is all about.
  • 01:10:29Here's a whole bunch of
  • 01:10:30different things that you could.
  • 01:10:32Do to report back up to reverse the
  • 01:10:36process to take an offering up to.
  • 01:10:38In essence,
  • 01:10:39lots of lots of different ways to avert
  • 01:10:42getting into that high risk space,
  • 01:10:45and so in some cases,
  • 01:10:47and it really is sort of riding the wave,
  • 01:10:51but it does.
  • 01:10:52It's something that I think really hits
  • 01:10:54that notion of non proportionality
  • 01:10:56is something very simple,
  • 01:10:58like I was shocked to be perfectly
  • 01:11:01honest when we did that CRP study.
  • 01:11:05I was like, you know, this.
  • 01:11:06We're writing a bunch of stuff
  • 01:11:08done on an index card.
  • 01:11:09In about 30 minutes,
  • 01:11:10you know this is going to help in
  • 01:11:13the next few weeks, but you know,
  • 01:11:15we really need to get someone into
  • 01:11:17like treatment like the CBT or
  • 01:11:19something like that to have a longer
  • 01:11:21term effect and what we ended up
  • 01:11:23finding was that the effects tended to
  • 01:11:25get bigger and bigger as more time passed,
  • 01:11:28and so I was like wow,
  • 01:11:29there's something happening here and
  • 01:11:31it appears to help individuals to
  • 01:11:33know where their tipping point is.
  • 01:11:35Into in essence,
  • 01:11:37a stay away from that,
  • 01:11:39and it seems to work very,
  • 01:11:41very effectively as result.
  • 01:11:44So I think we're how we have 60 seconds left,
  • 01:11:47so just given all this data,
  • 01:11:49what do you think? Why?
  • 01:11:51Why do we still have these
  • 01:11:53issues in the military,
  • 01:11:54the VA and in our country?
  • 01:11:56What's preventing these interventions from?
  • 01:11:59And be part of it is.
  • 01:12:01I think we still buy and larger than the
  • 01:12:04middle health disciplines conceptualize
  • 01:12:06suicide as a symptom of mental illness.
  • 01:12:09And so by and large treatment, as usual,
  • 01:12:12is largely treat the depression.
  • 01:12:14Treat the PTS di treat anxiety,
  • 01:12:16whatever the you know,
  • 01:12:18whatever the diagnosis is,
  • 01:12:20and then theoretically suicide
  • 01:12:21risk should resolve after that.
  • 01:12:23But now I think we increasingly recognize
  • 01:12:26that suicide is transdiagnostic
  • 01:12:27that it's better to look at.
  • 01:12:30Suicide risk is.
  • 01:12:31Independent of psychiatric diagnosis
  • 01:12:34and so treatments like bteen crisis
  • 01:12:37response planning are going directly at.
  • 01:12:40Several mechanisms that seem
  • 01:12:42to give rise to suicide risk,
  • 01:12:44and so it's a more direct targeting
  • 01:12:46of the problem as opposed to an
  • 01:12:49indirect targeting through these
  • 01:12:51other reported mental health.
  • 01:12:53Channels the last thing that I think
  • 01:12:55is really key that another aspect of
  • 01:12:58my thinking is really moving out of
  • 01:13:01this sort of mental health model of suicide,
  • 01:13:04and we're now looking more and
  • 01:13:06more at more like environmental.
  • 01:13:09Injury prevention models where
  • 01:13:10you know we don't reduce traffic
  • 01:13:12fatalities by saying everyone needs to.
  • 01:13:14Lookout for the warning signs of
  • 01:13:16a car accident and then if you see
  • 01:13:19those warning signs you should go
  • 01:13:21and get enroll in a drivers Ed class.
  • 01:13:24I mean,
  • 01:13:25that's basically how we approach
  • 01:13:26suicide prevention right now.
  • 01:13:28What we do is we build safer
  • 01:13:30roads and we build safer cars and
  • 01:13:33we require people to buckle up.
  • 01:13:35And we criminalize conditions like
  • 01:13:37drinking and driving that increase
  • 01:13:39the probability of fatal outcomes.
  • 01:13:40And so,
  • 01:13:41how do we now take a similar
  • 01:13:44thought process with suicide,
  • 01:13:46where we change the environment in a
  • 01:13:49way that reduces the likelihood of a
  • 01:13:52person if they do shift to a high risk state,
  • 01:13:56they have a seat belt now and they
  • 01:13:59are significantly more likely
  • 01:14:01to survive that unforeseen,
  • 01:14:03unpredictable conditions.
  • 01:14:04So
  • 01:14:05can you one quick thing on that
  • 01:14:07that issue of of the systemic?
  • 01:14:10Approach and associating
  • 01:14:11it with mental illness.
  • 01:14:13There's varying degrees in different
  • 01:14:15studies about how many people actually
  • 01:14:17even have an independent mental health
  • 01:14:19condition from suicidality, right?
  • 01:14:21Just like heart attacks like some
  • 01:14:23people just don't have another
  • 01:14:25cardiac condition that we're aware of.
  • 01:14:28So what is your best guestimate of the
  • 01:14:30people who attempt suicide and actually
  • 01:14:33don't have some other standalone
  • 01:14:35health condition?
  • 01:14:36Yeah, I would say so.
  • 01:14:38It's a bigger percentage than we think.
  • 01:14:41Or at least that we've traditionally assumed.
  • 01:14:43So if you look at CDC data,
  • 01:14:4555% of suicide decedents in the US
  • 01:14:47do not have a known mental illness.
  • 01:14:49Now the typical worst retort to that is,
  • 01:14:51well, they didn't go in to meet
  • 01:14:53with the therapist or psychiatrist,
  • 01:14:55and so they were never diagnosed,
  • 01:14:57so we just didn't know that they had
  • 01:14:59the condition that we assume is there.
  • 01:15:02But if you look at some
  • 01:15:03other lines of data and ones,
  • 01:15:05I think that is really informative.
  • 01:15:07This Joe Franklin's meta
  • 01:15:08analysis from a few years ago.
  • 01:15:10Looking at 50 years of
  • 01:15:12suicide risk factor research,
  • 01:15:13if you kind of do some reverse engineering
  • 01:15:16of some of the statistics he reported,
  • 01:15:18in essence,
  • 01:15:19what his data would suggest is that.
  • 01:15:21Somewhere around 40% of those
  • 01:15:24who attempt suicide or die by
  • 01:15:26suicide have a mental health
  • 01:15:29diagnosis or elevated symptoms.
  • 01:15:30Things like that.
  • 01:15:34I think the third line of evidence
  • 01:15:36comes from psych autopsy studies,
  • 01:15:38which are often used to support that.
  • 01:15:40This is where the 90% status
  • 01:15:42obtained from 90% of suicide
  • 01:15:44decisions have a mental illness.
  • 01:15:46It comes from cycle autopsy studies.
  • 01:15:50The second party method is
  • 01:15:52highly vulnerable to bias,
  • 01:15:54and if you look at case controlled
  • 01:15:56cycle top studies where you can see
  • 01:16:00is that they generally overestimate
  • 01:16:02rates of psychiatric illness in
  • 01:16:04control cases by double and so you
  • 01:16:07can reasonably infer that maybe
  • 01:16:09psych autopsies are doubling their
  • 01:16:12biased towards over diagnosing mental
  • 01:16:14health conditions by a factor of two,
  • 01:16:17in which case 90% of.
  • 01:16:20Which is their finding would convert to
  • 01:16:22about a 45% and estimate the correction,
  • 01:16:25so to speak.
  • 01:16:26So this is sort of like these several
  • 01:16:29lines of evidence that I would say
  • 01:16:32maybe around 40 to 50% of suicide.
  • 01:16:35Decedents Oregon suicide
  • 01:16:37attempters probably do not have.
  • 01:16:39A diagnosable mental condition.
  • 01:16:41They might be really stressed out.
  • 01:16:43It might be really upset
  • 01:16:44under pressure for sure,
  • 01:16:46but come on like being upset
  • 01:16:48when your partner tells you that
  • 01:16:50they're going to leave you there.
  • 01:16:52Having an affair is not a mental illness.
  • 01:16:54That is a normative emotional
  • 01:16:56reaction to a life stressor,
  • 01:16:58and so I think we've blurred
  • 01:17:00the lines between.
  • 01:17:01Between these experiences and as a result,
  • 01:17:04we put all of our eggs into
  • 01:17:06the Git mental health.
  • 01:17:08Basket mental health treatment basket
  • 01:17:10and so we're ignoring a lot of other
  • 01:17:14potentially useful and impactful
  • 01:17:17suicide prevention strategies.
  • 01:17:21Some very positive feedback in the chat.
  • 01:17:22I'll take some screenshots so
  • 01:17:24that you can show it to your loved
  • 01:17:27ones and family so that they know
  • 01:17:29what a great job you did crag.
  • 01:17:31Any other questions or comments for Crag?
  • 01:17:35Before we let him get on
  • 01:17:37to his weekend I guess.
  • 01:17:41Hi this is Shelly I guess I I apologize
  • 01:17:43for the length the set up there so I've
  • 01:17:45read Malcolm Gladwell's tipping point.
  • 01:17:47Understand I've done a lot of work in the
  • 01:17:49chemistry lab with pH buffering and it's
  • 01:17:51very very simple to overshoot because
  • 01:17:52you're on an exponential pathway right?
  • 01:17:54So what I was thinking is it sounds like
  • 01:17:56you have a lot of linear factors and then
  • 01:17:59you have an exponential factor that may not
  • 01:18:01be the same exponential factor every time.
  • 01:18:03So very very quick to overshoot
  • 01:18:05at that moment of suicidality.
  • 01:18:06Most of the work I do is
  • 01:18:08with veterans at this point.
  • 01:18:10And most of the veterans are very,
  • 01:18:12very familiar with weapons.
  • 01:18:13The conversations I start out early I
  • 01:18:15start out often talking about guns.
  • 01:18:16I talked extensively about it
  • 01:18:17in the interview,
  • 01:18:18find out how they're stored,
  • 01:18:20what they do with somehow they carry it.
  • 01:18:22You know,
  • 01:18:22there's a fairly lengthy conversation
  • 01:18:24and a lot of times sometimes they
  • 01:18:25back off and sometimes it it goes to.
  • 01:18:27You know, I just say listen,
  • 01:18:29you're probably not anything
  • 01:18:30to worry about right now,
  • 01:18:31but if the time ever comes in the future,
  • 01:18:34I want to, you know,
  • 01:18:35I have a comfort level talking
  • 01:18:37with you about this,
  • 01:18:38and we've laid out the groundwork for that.
  • 01:18:40Now I don't have any data.
  • 01:18:42I don't know if it helps us.
  • 01:18:44Don't know if it doesn't,
  • 01:18:45but one of the frustrations I have
  • 01:18:47is with my fellow clinician who
  • 01:18:49sometimes in all best efforts.
  • 01:18:51Seem like that conversation
  • 01:18:52is incredibly minimal,
  • 01:18:54is sort of the richness that
  • 01:18:56it could be done.
  • 01:18:57Yeah,
  • 01:18:58so I guess I'm wondering what we
  • 01:19:00can do to train the clinician to
  • 01:19:03inquire in a way that's useful
  • 01:19:05to the patient in those times.
  • 01:19:08So we yeah,
  • 01:19:09so we trained clinicians pretty
  • 01:19:10often in mean safety counseling,
  • 01:19:13but now the training workshop that I do,
  • 01:19:16I just call firearm safety counseling.
  • 01:19:18I'm like, yeah,
  • 01:19:19'cause I find clinicians are usually
  • 01:19:21reasonably comfortable talking about like.
  • 01:19:23Limiting access to medications
  • 01:19:25and things like that,
  • 01:19:26but it is a firearm issue that often is much,
  • 01:19:30much more challenging, and so we do.
  • 01:19:33So we do have data.
  • 01:19:35Their forthcoming that if you do this,
  • 01:19:38if you have these conversations in
  • 01:19:40a very sort of non judgmental way.
  • 01:19:43But again we used the motivational
  • 01:19:45interviewing based approach we found.
  • 01:19:47There was incredibly highly
  • 01:19:49acceptable in our study with military
  • 01:19:52personnel out of 200 year old 130.
  • 01:19:54200% of them said I would recommend
  • 01:19:56this conversation for a friend,
  • 01:19:58which is sort of like OK then.
  • 01:20:01That's some St cred there.
  • 01:20:02This is evidence of acceptability.
  • 01:20:06Reached their likelihood of locking up their
  • 01:20:08guns and we found that the vast majority,
  • 01:20:11even though there was there,
  • 01:20:13was a small number who kind of still had,
  • 01:20:15you know, screw you guys at
  • 01:20:17the end of the conversation,
  • 01:20:19all of those during the follow up.
  • 01:20:21They said, you know,
  • 01:20:22I felt respected and listened to you guys
  • 01:20:25didn't push me and so we had a few who
  • 01:20:28at least we're left with a good impression,
  • 01:20:30even though perhaps we hadn't yet convinced
  • 01:20:32them that it was in their best interest.
  • 01:20:35But one of those things that we
  • 01:20:37have found really helpful working.
  • 01:20:39Gun owners in general.
  • 01:20:40Is using the metaphor of a designated
  • 01:20:42drivers in wearing seatbelts and
  • 01:20:44we say do you when you drive?
  • 01:20:46Do you only wear seat belt on the day that
  • 01:20:49you expect to get into a car accident?
  • 01:20:51And they're like, well, no,
  • 01:20:52you can't predict it is like right?
  • 01:20:54And we never know when we're going to
  • 01:20:56have those really, really bad days.
  • 01:20:58And so that's why we wear a
  • 01:21:00seat belt just in case.
  • 01:21:02And so what's going to be
  • 01:21:03the seat belt for your gun?
  • 01:21:05And they find that we were finding
  • 01:21:07that oftentimes helps him to think.
  • 01:21:09OK,
  • 01:21:09maybe a gun safe and unlock something
  • 01:21:11like that is sort of like a seat belt.
  • 01:21:14And that might be the difference
  • 01:21:15between life and death in the same way
  • 01:21:18that wearing your seat belt can be.
  • 01:21:19Those difference between life and death
  • 01:21:21in a car accident and then the designated
  • 01:21:23driver model is really resonated a lot.
  • 01:21:25Where,
  • 01:21:26like you know,
  • 01:21:26if you're worried about a friend,
  • 01:21:28we we target this sort of community rather
  • 01:21:30than this like a high risk individual,
  • 01:21:32but will say you know when
  • 01:21:34your friend has been drinking,
  • 01:21:35you take away their parties because
  • 01:21:37it's dangerous and it doesn't mean they
  • 01:21:39never get to drive again in their life.
  • 01:21:41It's just that you have to wait
  • 01:21:43until they're sober so it's
  • 01:21:45safer and then they can.
  • 01:21:46You know,
  • 01:21:47have a car keys back again and so
  • 01:21:49it works the same with the gun,
  • 01:21:51shouldn't it will ask you should
  • 01:21:53suicidal people have access to
  • 01:21:54loaded weapons and everyone's like,
  • 01:21:56no, that's a terrible idea,
  • 01:21:57like right,
  • 01:21:58and so if you're if you know we have a friend
  • 01:22:01is going through a hard time like that.
  • 01:22:04Let's think about this like a
  • 01:22:05designated driver and let's maybe
  • 01:22:07offer to temporarily hold onto
  • 01:22:08their weapons until they're in
  • 01:22:10a different state of mountain.
  • 01:22:11And then perhaps it would be
  • 01:22:13safer for them to have access.
  • 01:22:15And we're finding those messages
  • 01:22:17are very positively received.
  • 01:22:18But the hard part is,
  • 01:22:20yeah,
  • 01:22:21training clinicians.
  • 01:22:21My sense is that clinicians are often
  • 01:22:25biased towards complete removal.
  • 01:22:27Of the fire room.
  • 01:22:28From the households which makes
  • 01:22:30a lot of sense,
  • 01:22:31that is definitely a probably
  • 01:22:33the safest option,
  • 01:22:34but we're really taking more of a
  • 01:22:37harm reduction approach and saying,
  • 01:22:39all things being equal,
  • 01:22:40if someone has multiple loaded weapons
  • 01:22:43that are readily available in the home
  • 01:22:45and we move them to a state where now
  • 01:22:48those weapons are all in a gun safe,
  • 01:22:51or they're all locked up with a trigger lock,
  • 01:22:54even though they're still in the home.
  • 01:22:58They are in a safer space,
  • 01:23:00relatively relatively speaking
  • 01:23:01than they were before,
  • 01:23:03and I see that as a valuable
  • 01:23:06movement that's worth us talking
  • 01:23:08with this clinicians.
  • 01:23:09While I see Mike and Alec and Howard here,
  • 01:23:13you have three, you know,
  • 01:23:15preeminent forensic psychiatrists.
  • 01:23:16And I heard what you said about
  • 01:23:19absolute removal of the gun,
  • 01:23:21which my suspicion is done out of fear of
  • 01:23:25liability for not insisting on it versus.
  • 01:23:28Google clinical evidence 'cause
  • 01:23:30your middle ground is so
  • 01:23:31intuitive and makes so much sense.
  • 01:23:33You know my suspicion,
  • 01:23:34and I am always have a little
  • 01:23:36little touch of cynicism,
  • 01:23:37and I guess my perspective on things
  • 01:23:39is that it's because we don't go for
  • 01:23:41the absolute removal were afraid.
  • 01:23:42If something happens,
  • 01:23:43there's a liability risk.
  • 01:23:44It's black or white.
  • 01:23:46And I remember the first time
  • 01:23:48I I saw you speak live,
  • 01:23:50you put up a little table of like the disk,
  • 01:23:53disjoint between the misalignment
  • 01:23:55between what does the clinician want
  • 01:23:57when they meet with the suicidal patient?
  • 01:23:59And what does the patient want?
  • 01:24:01Do you remember that slide?
  • 01:24:03That was quite awhile ago?
  • 01:24:05Comment on that because I thought it
  • 01:24:07was really incredibly elegant, yeah?
  • 01:24:11Yeah, it's sort of a key point in a
  • 01:24:13lot of the trainings I do is yeah
  • 01:24:15this that the clinicians goals are
  • 01:24:18to prevent the patient from dying,
  • 01:24:20and then I also tongue in cheek,
  • 01:24:22say and to avoid getting sued.
  • 01:24:24And then I like to say well in
  • 01:24:26most cases let's be honest,
  • 01:24:28we don't want to get sued and we know
  • 01:24:30the way to not get sued is to not have
  • 01:24:33a patient die something like that.
  • 01:24:36Whereas the patient's goals are
  • 01:24:37to alleviate their suffering
  • 01:24:38and solve their problem,
  • 01:24:40and So what ends up happening is we.
  • 01:24:43Sometimes gets so focused on preventing
  • 01:24:45death that we forget that in another
  • 01:24:48important part of suicide prevention is
  • 01:24:51creating lives that are worth living,
  • 01:24:53and so we may succeed in the short term in
  • 01:24:56keeping someone breathing biologically alive.
  • 01:24:59But if we don't do something
  • 01:25:02that changes their environment,
  • 01:25:04their psychology,
  • 01:25:04things like that,
  • 01:25:06and probably they're going to get
  • 01:25:08into the state again,
  • 01:25:10and so we need to look at quality
  • 01:25:13of life in addition to simply just.
  • 01:25:17Like restraining people from dying,
  • 01:25:19and so when it comes to the issues
  • 01:25:21surrounding like means restriction,
  • 01:25:23I think of you know I've written
  • 01:25:26about this a few times.
  • 01:25:27I think one of the downsides we
  • 01:25:29recommended as a suicide prevention
  • 01:25:31intervention for clinicians.
  • 01:25:33But as was Shelley,
  • 01:25:34was alluding to we don't really
  • 01:25:37do any training at all on how
  • 01:25:39to do this effectively,
  • 01:25:40so we almost set ourselves up for failure,
  • 01:25:43and we think I think in terms of
  • 01:25:46these sort of binary extremes.
  • 01:25:49Where I don't know.
  • 01:25:50We'll see how things emerge overtime.
  • 01:25:52You know I do some consulting on
  • 01:25:54legal cases like negligence cases,
  • 01:25:56things like this and it's a topic
  • 01:25:58that is very key to a lot of those
  • 01:26:01is at what point do you say that a
  • 01:26:04clinician have been reasonable and
  • 01:26:06done enough 'cause we can always
  • 01:26:08find something more that a person
  • 01:26:11could have theoretically done?
  • 01:26:13And it I found it tough with the gun issue,
  • 01:26:16but they give some cases now.
  • 01:26:18Clinicians that actually did
  • 01:26:20needs restriction.
  • 01:26:21Yeah,
  • 01:26:21they didn't need description and
  • 01:26:23then the patient didn't tell them
  • 01:26:25they still had a secret gun.
  • 01:26:27I mean it's good news.
  • 01:26:29Craig is that unlike drinking and driving
  • 01:26:31gun ownership is not not all politicized.
  • 01:26:34So since drinking and driving and
  • 01:26:36designated driver that just simple,
  • 01:26:38it's just simple problem.
  • 01:26:40The gun issue I'm sure will
  • 01:26:43be even easier to tackle.
  • 01:26:45Yeah, well thank you, anything else?
  • 01:26:48I just wonder what you think
  • 01:26:50the APA is considering having
  • 01:26:53suicidal behaviors, a diagnosis.
  • 01:26:56And given the fact that yeah don't have,
  • 01:26:59I just wonder what you think about that.
  • 01:27:03Yeah, I'm sort of ambivalent about it.
  • 01:27:06On the one hand.
  • 01:27:08I think some of the arguments being
  • 01:27:11made for why we want to do it,
  • 01:27:14I think makes sense.
  • 01:27:16You know to recognize it as being discrete
  • 01:27:19from other psychiatric conditions diagnosis,
  • 01:27:22I think probably there might be what?
  • 01:27:27Joyner is called the closet with an acute
  • 01:27:30suicidal syndrome or something like that,
  • 01:27:32where maybe there is kind of a
  • 01:27:34unique mental state associated
  • 01:27:36with near term risk for suicide.
  • 01:27:38Now the downside kind of my hesitation
  • 01:27:41about it as I kind of feel like begin
  • 01:27:44were now now with a diagnosis as
  • 01:27:47opposed to a manner of death. And.
  • 01:27:50You know, I don't know that we have like,
  • 01:27:53yeah, Heart Attack syndrome.
  • 01:27:54We don't.
  • 01:27:54You know, it's sort of.
  • 01:27:56I kind of feel like it's our continuing trend
  • 01:27:59to conceptualize and think about suicide.
  • 01:28:02From the nearly exclusive
  • 01:28:03lens of psychiatric illness.
  • 01:28:05And so I think that will slow
  • 01:28:07us down from in my opinion.
  • 01:28:09I think we need to be a little bit
  • 01:28:12more divergent and are thinking
  • 01:28:14about suicide to consider it from
  • 01:28:16these different perspectives and
  • 01:28:18angles as opposed to continuing to
  • 01:28:20kind of narrow us down more and
  • 01:28:23more into the DSM and the domain of.
  • 01:28:26You know one particular health
  • 01:28:28care discipline so.
  • 01:28:32And no one is asking me,
  • 01:28:33but I happen to the heart attack
  • 01:28:35thing to me or an arrhythmia is
  • 01:28:37in fact a parallel in my mind.
  • 01:28:39Anyone can have.
  • 01:28:40I honestly believe anyone can
  • 01:28:41become suicidal with the right.
  • 01:28:43External variables,
  • 01:28:44and certainly if other conditions
  • 01:28:45occur as they go through life,
  • 01:28:47just like in a heart attack,
  • 01:28:49anyone can get Melanoma.
  • 01:28:51They are like important,
  • 01:28:52they had outcomes,
  • 01:28:53and I wouldn't worry about it, Craig.
  • 01:28:55Yeah, well and I think yeah,
  • 01:28:57I've thought about this a lot in
  • 01:28:59the past year when they were getting
  • 01:29:02close to a year of a pandemic.
  • 01:29:04And so I'm constantly asked
  • 01:29:06about how the pandemic influence
  • 01:29:08suicide risk and how do we improve
  • 01:29:10access to mental health care.
  • 01:29:11And I think the point that.
  • 01:29:14We often overlook is like, yeah,
  • 01:29:16you know, therapy medications might
  • 01:29:17help to reduce a person's distress,
  • 01:29:20but it doesn't inherently change the
  • 01:29:22fact that the environment right now is
  • 01:29:25placing a lot of strain on individual,
  • 01:29:27so you can't therapy your way out of
  • 01:29:30COVID-19 you know you can't therapy
  • 01:29:32your way out of a minimum wage job.
  • 01:29:35You can't therapy your way out of an abusive,
  • 01:29:38toxic work environment.
  • 01:29:40And and so I think this is.
  • 01:29:43This is one of the main reasons why
  • 01:29:46I think we're so bad that suicide
  • 01:29:49prevention as there are things in
  • 01:29:52life that contribute to suicide risk.
  • 01:29:55But we we largely think about suicide
  • 01:29:57as being within the individual.
  • 01:29:59The thing that we.
  • 01:30:00Just to sort of root out and get rid of
  • 01:30:04when in reality there are conditions
  • 01:30:06all around us that contribute to that,
  • 01:30:09and we would probably actually
  • 01:30:11see way better results if.
  • 01:30:13If we increase the minimum wage,
  • 01:30:15if we expanded access to health care,
  • 01:30:18if we targeted racism and discrimination
  • 01:30:20against vulnerable populations,
  • 01:30:21and there's not much that I'm going to
  • 01:30:23be able to do as a health care provider.
  • 01:30:26Yeah,
  • 01:30:27to improve the financial stability
  • 01:30:28of my patients that they're working
  • 01:30:303 minimum wage jobs.
  • 01:30:32It's it would be way more impactful to
  • 01:30:34just increase their salary in resolve.
  • 01:30:36A lot of the stress that they're coming in.
  • 01:30:39Complaining to me about.
  • 01:30:41But of course,
  • 01:30:42it's really not coming to complain.
  • 01:30:44About that because I can't afford it, right?
  • 01:30:47So anyway.
  • 01:30:51Craig, thank you.