Skip to Main Content

Yale Psychiatry Grand Rounds - March 5, 2021

March 05, 2021
  • 00:00Because my introduction is much less
  • 00:02important than Doctor Calipari's talk,
  • 00:04which will be eagerly awaiting.
  • 00:07So it's it's my absolute pleasure
  • 00:10to introduce Doctor Erin Calipari
  • 00:13who's today's grand round speaker.
  • 00:16She is someone who has really been
  • 00:19a pioneer and incredibly prolific
  • 00:22scientists already in the area of
  • 00:25addiction and using a rodent models to
  • 00:30understand the neurobiological basis
  • 00:32of important constructs underlying
  • 00:35addiction that are relevant to.
  • 00:38Human subjects she did her
  • 00:40undergraduate degree at the University
  • 00:42of Massachusetts in Amherst,
  • 00:44where she has got her BS in both psychology
  • 00:47and biology and already began working
  • 00:50with Doctor Gerald Meyer using rodent models.
  • 00:53And then she did her graduate work at Wake
  • 00:57Forest School of Medicine with Sarah Jones,
  • 01:00where she began to.
  • 01:03It really immerse herself and neurochemistry,
  • 01:06an other aspects of in vivo manipulations
  • 01:09and measurements related to behavior.
  • 01:12She then went on as a postdoctoral fellow
  • 01:15and then a instructor at the Mount Sinai's
  • 01:19Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai,
  • 01:23where she worked with Eric Nestler.
  • 01:26And there she began to develop
  • 01:28a number of lines of research,
  • 01:31including examination of.
  • 01:33Particularly molecular consequences
  • 01:35of drug addiction,
  • 01:37and particularly intracellular
  • 01:39signaling as a consequence of drug
  • 01:43addiction that may maintain long term
  • 01:46structural and behavioral adaptations
  • 01:49to drugs of abuse and also was really
  • 01:52key in a number of important papers
  • 01:55on sex specific effects of addictive,
  • 01:58addictive substances like cocaine and
  • 02:01those experiments are particularly
  • 02:04notable because they.
  • 02:05Involved many, many levels of evaluation,
  • 02:07not only in rodents but also in
  • 02:10human subjects.
  • 02:11She has won a number of awards already.
  • 02:14I want to note a few notable ones.
  • 02:17In particular she is an awardee of the DP.
  • 02:21One Avenir Award in genetics,
  • 02:23and epigenetics from the National
  • 02:25Institute on Drug Abuse,
  • 02:27and that's someone who the director
  • 02:29of Naida pulls out as having research
  • 02:32that is extremely innovative and
  • 02:34at the edge of the.
  • 02:36Molecular basis of addiction.
  • 02:38She's an associate member of the AC NP.
  • 02:41Oh I should have noted that she's
  • 02:43also an assistant professor in
  • 02:45the department's particularly in
  • 02:46the Department of Pharmacology
  • 02:48at Vanderbilt University,
  • 02:50but also has appointments in
  • 02:52the departments of Psychiatry
  • 02:53and Behavioral Sciences.
  • 02:55Of course relevant to our Department
  • 02:57and the Department of Molecular
  • 02:59Physiology and Biophysics.
  • 03:00So again, even with her appointments,
  • 03:03you can see how her work spans areas of
  • 03:06investigation from the very molecular
  • 03:08and cellular through the pharmacological too.
  • 03:11The area of drug addiction
  • 03:13relevant to psychiatric illness.
  • 03:15So back to her awards.
  • 03:17Just because they're so notable she is,
  • 03:20has been awarded a Whitehall Foundation
  • 03:23research grant and are said Young
  • 03:25Investigator Award and a K99 to
  • 03:28R00 pathway to Independence Award,
  • 03:30and even back in her days
  • 03:33as a graduate student.
  • 03:34The Knighted Director's Award,
  • 03:36which acknowledged her innovative
  • 03:38work from the beginning,
  • 03:40an addiction.
  • 03:41She's an editorial boards relevant
  • 03:43to this Department.
  • 03:44Both the editorial board of
  • 03:46Neuropsychopharmacology and
  • 03:46the Journal of Neuroscience,
  • 03:48and she's been incredibly
  • 03:49prolific at every stage of her
  • 03:52career, showing how which I love that
  • 03:54not only does she have great ideas,
  • 03:57but she carries them through all the way
  • 03:59to really publishing beautiful papers,
  • 04:02and it's been exciting to watch
  • 04:04the evolution of the work.
  • 04:06Do you want to say something that
  • 04:08is not exactly science related,
  • 04:10as she was recounting during?
  • 04:12The first discussion she does have one area
  • 04:15that she's interested in outside of science,
  • 04:17or that she's able to devote time
  • 04:19to as an assistant professor,
  • 04:21and that is she is a powerlifter and
  • 04:24I have seen a video of her doing.
  • 04:27Plus, which I'm very, very envious of,
  • 04:29but I do want to highlight that her most
  • 04:32recent achievement in powerlifting is
  • 04:34that she has just deadlifted 300 pounds.
  • 04:36Now if that isn't an achievement,
  • 04:38I don't know what is.
  • 04:39So thank you very much for being
  • 04:42with us here.
  • 04:42Erin Dr.
  • 04:43Calipari,
  • 04:43it is a pleasure to welcome you
  • 04:45to the Department and I'm looking
  • 04:47forward to hearing we're all looking
  • 04:49forward to hearing your talk.
  • 04:51Thank you so
  • 04:52much. That was so nice.
  • 04:53And yeah, I know I tried it.
  • 04:55You know the pandemic has
  • 04:57made me start to try to find.
  • 04:59Things to occupy my time that aren't
  • 05:01just sitting at home and working and
  • 05:03so that's that's what I've been doing.
  • 05:06But I'm I'm actually really excited
  • 05:07to present for a number of reasons.
  • 05:09One, because I get to see some people
  • 05:12outside of my immediate family in my lab.
  • 05:14But to this, this work is some kind
  • 05:17of work that's been developing in
  • 05:19my lab of the last couple of years.
  • 05:22That focuses on kind of what
  • 05:24dopamine is doing,
  • 05:25and so I think you know for somebody who
  • 05:28studies addiction and psychiatric disease.
  • 05:31The reason this is so important
  • 05:33is because domains at the core
  • 05:35of a lot of these disorders,
  • 05:37and you know specifically addiction
  • 05:39where you see deficits in dopamine,
  • 05:41and I think it's really important
  • 05:43to understand for people what
  • 05:45those deficits mean.
  • 05:46You know if dopamine is encoding
  • 05:48reward and reduction in dopamine
  • 05:50may mean something very different.
  • 05:52Then if it don't mean is encoding
  • 05:54some other aspect of learned behavior,
  • 05:56which I'm probably going to show you today.
  • 06:00So you know,
  • 06:00I'll kind of show some stuff in the weeds,
  • 06:03but also kind of tide into big
  • 06:05picture and so please like stop
  • 06:07me if things aren't clear or you
  • 06:10have thoughts or comments as we go.
  • 06:12OK,
  • 06:13so the focus of my lab is really
  • 06:15understanding if you can see my slides right.
  • 06:18OK,
  • 06:18good is understanding how neural
  • 06:20experience or how neural circuits
  • 06:22integrate experiences to drive
  • 06:23behavior and so you know in life
  • 06:26or in animals you know we have
  • 06:28experiences that have negative outcomes.
  • 06:30Things that have positive outcomes
  • 06:32and what happens is that these
  • 06:34experiences change the way our brain
  • 06:36response to stimuli in the future to
  • 06:38increase the probability of behaviors
  • 06:40that result in good outcomes.
  • 06:41And decrease the probability of
  • 06:43behaviors that result in negative outcomes.
  • 06:46And the reason I'm so interested
  • 06:49in this is because this is kind of,
  • 06:52you know,
  • 06:53the fundamental core of how we
  • 06:55make decisions.
  • 06:56But it's also dysregulated,
  • 06:57and almost every psychiatric disease state,
  • 07:00and so you know for somebody
  • 07:02who studies addiction,
  • 07:04you know drug associated stimuli,
  • 07:06or overvalued relative to
  • 07:07negative consequences.
  • 07:08An alternative reinforcers depression.
  • 07:10You have reduced motivation.
  • 07:11Valuation of rewards.
  • 07:12Reward learning or things like
  • 07:14anxiety and stress disorders where
  • 07:16these negative outcomes may be
  • 07:18overgeneralized to neutral cues and contexts,
  • 07:20which is,
  • 07:21you know things like PTSD,
  • 07:23and so you know this kind of
  • 07:25fundamental process by which we
  • 07:27attribute value to things that our
  • 07:29environment is really a core of
  • 07:31how we should be thinking about
  • 07:33treating people with disorders
  • 07:35where this is dysregulated.
  • 07:37So you know, I'm, you know,
  • 07:39going way back to the simple,
  • 07:41you know half of my lab studies.
  • 07:43You know how to drug change the brain,
  • 07:45but the other half of my
  • 07:47labs that he's just
  • 07:48kind of. How do these same systems
  • 07:50work in a normal situation?
  • 07:51And so the first thing is kind of,
  • 07:54you know, how do we learn to
  • 07:56make these adaptive decisions?
  • 07:57And so we use things in our
  • 07:59Virat environment to do this.
  • 08:01So maybe there's contextual cues
  • 08:02that help you figure out when things
  • 08:05are dangerous and when they aren't,
  • 08:06and so if you have something like the
  • 08:09sound of a helicopter in a hometown
  • 08:11may be very different than the cell of
  • 08:14sound of a helicopter in a war zone.
  • 08:16We have things like discrete cues,
  • 08:18which would be the sound of a
  • 08:19helicopter itself with drug addiction.
  • 08:21It's these cues that are associated
  • 08:23with the drug taking experience,
  • 08:25and so we learn to associate those.
  • 08:27And then there's also, you know.
  • 08:29What we do in response to these stimuli,
  • 08:31and so you know my background is
  • 08:33really focused on reinforcement
  • 08:34learning and how these stimuli in
  • 08:36the environment drive animals to
  • 08:37make decisions in different context.
  • 08:39So what are they going to do?
  • 08:41Are they going to, you know,
  • 08:43are they reinforce?
  • 08:43Are they going to do something or
  • 08:45they going to avoid something?
  • 08:47And how we can understand the neural
  • 08:50circuitry that underlies this
  • 08:51decision to kind of seek out or avoid
  • 08:54different things in the environment?
  • 08:56And so you know,
  • 08:57I think this is a really important thing,
  • 08:59right?
  • 09:00Is that we learn to make predictions and
  • 09:02so our actions have some sort of outcome.
  • 09:05It changes the state of our
  • 09:07environment and basically what we
  • 09:09do is we learn to do something and
  • 09:11so this is kind of guiding these.
  • 09:13These associations aren't just there,
  • 09:14they're guiding how we navigate
  • 09:16an environment. And so.
  • 09:18How do we do this?
  • 09:21Well,
  • 09:21we need to encode the value or
  • 09:24salience salience is kind of like how
  • 09:26attention grabbing something is of
  • 09:28unexpected outcomes and so you get ice cream.
  • 09:31That's great.
  • 09:32It's awesome.
  • 09:33We need to know whether it's good
  • 09:35or bad and how good or bad it is.
  • 09:38How attention, yes, are you advancing slides?
  • 09:40Yes, sorry,
  • 09:41this has happened to me before and
  • 09:44I have no idea why it does this.
  • 09:47Let me try this again.
  • 09:49It's OK if you guys followed them.
  • 09:51Yeah, I thought the intro was all very clear,
  • 09:54but I thought maybe you were dancing.
  • 09:56We didn't know. Yeah, this is
  • 09:57this is happened to me before. I have no.
  • 10:01Idea when or why this does this?
  • 10:04Let me try this one more time.
  • 10:06So where I made it 'cause it's on,
  • 10:09zoom on the shorter side because
  • 10:11I don't think people love watching
  • 10:13zoom for two hours. So OK.
  • 10:15So now if I move the slides they move OK.
  • 10:19Well, there were pictures you guys
  • 10:21have experience with all of this stuff,
  • 10:23so that's fine.
  • 10:24So now we're into the important
  • 10:25bit so it's good you saw this. OK,
  • 10:28so nothing like this is like pandemic level.
  • 10:30Like everything,
  • 10:30something has to go wrong every time.
  • 10:32Otherwise, like you know, it's not.
  • 10:34It's not real, so OK,
  • 10:35so you have to encode some information.
  • 10:37We need to know whether it's good
  • 10:39or bad and how intense it is.
  • 10:41Is this something we should
  • 10:43really pay attention to?
  • 10:44Or is this something that's not as important?
  • 10:46We need to make predictions
  • 10:48about when that's going to occur.
  • 10:50And so you know you have an ice cream truck.
  • 10:52You predict whether the ice
  • 10:54cream will be there or not.
  • 10:55But not only do we need to make predictions,
  • 10:58we need to be able to update
  • 11:00these when they change.
  • 11:01So when something no longer is associated,
  • 11:03we need to be able to adapt.
  • 11:05If the update this so that we can change
  • 11:07our behavior when the environment is
  • 11:09not the same as we learned previously,
  • 11:11and so this is a really, really critical
  • 11:14aspect of learning and behavior.
  • 11:15So I'm going to kind of go.
  • 11:17There's going to be some computation in here,
  • 11:19but what I'll tell you is most of
  • 11:21it is is more of a framework for
  • 11:23how people think about how these
  • 11:25these computations are being done,
  • 11:27and if you don't care about the computation,
  • 11:29which I've met, many people who say,
  • 11:31oh,
  • 11:31whatever we've used these to
  • 11:32design experiments,
  • 11:33and so it's not like you
  • 11:34need to know the math.
  • 11:36It's more of a kind of framework
  • 11:38for how we designed experiments.
  • 11:40So this kind of prediction
  • 11:42based learning was formalized.
  • 11:43You know,
  • 11:44originally by Rescorla Wagner in 1972,
  • 11:46and there's been a bunch of kind
  • 11:48of adaptations of this and allow
  • 11:51the model to do other things.
  • 11:53But really,
  • 11:53what this is is it's a mathematical
  • 11:56model that allows us to kind
  • 11:58of formalize how animals learn,
  • 12:00and So what happens in this model
  • 12:02is that if you have something
  • 12:04like an unexpected outcome,
  • 12:06that is an error in prediction,
  • 12:08you predicted nothing, something was there.
  • 12:10You made an error and what happens
  • 12:13overtime is your prediction gets
  • 12:15better and then there's less error.
  • 12:17So essentially what happens is the
  • 12:20associative strength or how well
  • 12:22you how well something predicts
  • 12:24something goes up and the error in
  • 12:26that prediction goes down and so
  • 12:28basically the way the model works is
  • 12:31that as you learn the prediction of
  • 12:34that Q and the outcome increases.
  • 12:36But any errors you make go down
  • 12:39and so essentially
  • 12:40you get this. Increase in the
  • 12:42predictive response and a decrease in
  • 12:44the error or the mistakes from that,
  • 12:46and so this is kind of how animals learn.
  • 12:49It can map learning rates in
  • 12:51a lot of different contexts.
  • 12:53You know learning about
  • 12:54Accuen award extinction.
  • 12:55All of these and so people have really
  • 12:58been searching for what is a circuit
  • 13:01in the brain that does this math.
  • 13:03And that's been a kind of really big
  • 13:06focus of specifically the dopamine field.
  • 13:09And other fields too.
  • 13:10I think a lot of people are
  • 13:11starting to see that these kinds of
  • 13:13computations are done in a variety
  • 13:15of circuits across the brain.
  • 13:17So the dopamine system is important
  • 13:19for any a lot of reasons.
  • 13:21These neurons you know that we focus on
  • 13:24originate in the ventral tegmental area,
  • 13:27so we're focusing on more reward
  • 13:29associated circuits rather than things
  • 13:31that are associated with motor.
  • 13:33So we're looking in for this
  • 13:35particular project.
  • 13:36The nucleus accumbens,
  • 13:37the core region,
  • 13:38and So what these dopamine neurons
  • 13:41are really important for survival.
  • 13:43Lesioning them present prevents
  • 13:44this kind of associative learning.
  • 13:46An also reinforcement learning.
  • 13:49And the thing people been kind of really
  • 13:52focus on is that this domain neurons
  • 13:54respond in a fashion that mimics this.
  • 13:57This mathematical model I just showed you,
  • 13:59and so essentially this kind of
  • 14:01originated and within a lots of other
  • 14:04people have shown these patterns.
  • 14:05So this originative with Wolfram Schultz
  • 14:07and I'm just showing the the original.
  • 14:10But people within the domain
  • 14:12field have done this with all
  • 14:14kinds of other approaches as well.
  • 14:16But essentially what they see
  • 14:18is this kind of same math.
  • 14:20We went hoping that this didn't
  • 14:23just move because OK,
  • 14:24the slides are still advancing, right?
  • 14:27Yeah, OK, OK.
  • 14:28So essentially what happens is
  • 14:29you have an unexpected reward.
  • 14:32Dopamine firing goes up.
  • 14:34You predict that reward dopamine firing
  • 14:37now goes up to the queue that predicts it,
  • 14:40but not the reward,
  • 14:41because the prediction of that
  • 14:43reward is basically perfect.
  • 14:44And now if the reward is omitted,
  • 14:47what happens is the dopamine
  • 14:49response to the queue goes up,
  • 14:51but there is now a decrease in that
  • 14:54domain response when it's omitted,
  • 14:56signaling the negative error that,
  • 14:58uh, from that prediction.
  • 14:59And so this is they.
  • 15:02You know, originally this,
  • 15:03this first paper.
  • 15:04They said, wow,
  • 15:05that looks a lot like reward prediction,
  • 15:07error learning,
  • 15:08and so this is kind of formed the
  • 15:10basis of the domain field dopamine
  • 15:13does reward prediction learning.
  • 15:15So here's the kind of maybe issue with that.
  • 15:19If you do stress work,
  • 15:21anything else you know the domain
  • 15:23does not only respond to rewards
  • 15:26and reward predictions,
  • 15:28it's involved in things like punishment,
  • 15:30which is an aversive learning parameter.
  • 15:33Motivation, fear, safety transitions,
  • 15:35aversive learning.
  • 15:37All kinds of there's been a
  • 15:38lot of Association,
  • 15:39aversive learning and these
  • 15:40fields have been kind of.
  • 15:42It was a separate,
  • 15:43but there's kind of the reward.
  • 15:44Prediction people and then the
  • 15:46people who studied anxiety,
  • 15:47depression looking at Microdialysis,
  • 15:48showing that dopamine does
  • 15:49go up to aversive stimuli,
  • 15:50and so these kind of have been a
  • 15:52little bit at odds with each other.
  • 15:55But they kind of are,
  • 15:56you know, different fields,
  • 15:57so people haven't really looked
  • 15:59at them in the same context.
  • 16:01And so essentially,
  • 16:02I think some of the disconnect also
  • 16:04comes from this kind of fundamental
  • 16:06process of about domain neurons.
  • 16:08That's actually my favorite
  • 16:09part of GOP neurons.
  • 16:11Many studies have looked
  • 16:12at VTA cell body firing.
  • 16:14They use electrophysiology.
  • 16:15They say we don't need it goes up.
  • 16:18It goes down.
  • 16:19And there's this inference that
  • 16:21that means dopamine release.
  • 16:23That is,
  • 16:23projection targets is going to be the
  • 16:26same as what the firing looks like.
  • 16:29But dopamine terminals are so cool
  • 16:31because they're regulated at the
  • 16:33terminal level by **** synaptic.
  • 16:34So things that are regulated by Domi
  • 16:37itself but also header, synaptic,
  • 16:39regulators things like glutamate,
  • 16:40GABA.
  • 16:41A favorite of this Department,
  • 16:43acetylcholine and so these.
  • 16:44These things actually can elicit
  • 16:46dopamine release from the terminals,
  • 16:47independent of cymatic firing.
  • 16:49And so if you want to understand
  • 16:51what dopamine release another
  • 16:52projection target is doing,
  • 16:54you need to actually record
  • 16:56dopamine and the ultimate wrists.
  • 16:57You have a few of those as well,
  • 17:00have been doing this for a really long time,
  • 17:03but there's a lot of kind of
  • 17:06limitations to voltammetry and we'll
  • 17:08kind of talk about those as we go,
  • 17:10but our goal was really too.
  • 17:13Record dopamine,
  • 17:13but be able to dissociate
  • 17:15these kind of things.
  • 17:16People have seen in the aversive
  • 17:18field with the things people
  • 17:20have seen in the reward fields.
  • 17:22Why is dopamine look like it's doing
  • 17:24both of these at the same time?
  • 17:27So my background isn't reinforcement
  • 17:29learning and what we did is we we
  • 17:31like to develop behavioral tasks to
  • 17:33parse the things that we're interested.
  • 17:35So we developed this task,
  • 17:37which is not really the task
  • 17:38itself is an innovative behavioral
  • 17:40pharmacology and reinforcement learning.
  • 17:42People have been doing this for years.
  • 17:45Essentially what we do is we
  • 17:46have a queue that comes on.
  • 17:48In one phase,
  • 17:49that tells animals if they know
  • 17:50spoke during this Q and in
  • 17:52this example is white noise.
  • 17:54But we counterbalance and change
  • 17:55that they will get sucrose.
  • 17:57This is like normal positive reinforcement.
  • 17:59You know you treat your teacher dog,
  • 18:01that's it. They get a reward.
  • 18:03What we taught the animals in
  • 18:05the other face is that a separate
  • 18:07queue comes on and they have
  • 18:09the same behavioral response.
  • 18:10They know spoke,
  • 18:11but they know spoke to prevent a
  • 18:13series of shocks from being delivered,
  • 18:15so it's called negative reinforcement.
  • 18:18The reason that using these is so cool
  • 18:20is because they have the exact same action.
  • 18:23So if dopamine just encodes
  • 18:24the motivated response,
  • 18:25these will look the same.
  • 18:28They have the same outcome value.
  • 18:30The outcome is positive.
  • 18:31Avoiding something negative is positive.
  • 18:32Getting something positive is positive,
  • 18:34but there's different stimuli maintaining
  • 18:36these behavioral events and so
  • 18:38essentially what we've associated here is
  • 18:40the kind of motivated action from this
  • 18:42stimulus value in the outcome value,
  • 18:43and the question is in this sounds
  • 18:46more complicated than it is,
  • 18:47and I'm going to tell you the story
  • 18:50is that we're going to be able to see
  • 18:52if dopamine responds to just rewards
  • 18:55if it's just involved in motivation,
  • 18:57or if it's doing something maybe slightly.
  • 19:00It's a more complicated,
  • 19:01but it's actually simpler.
  • 19:03We need a way to record dopamine
  • 19:06during this task.
  • 19:07Aversive foot shocks are electrical signals.
  • 19:09All of the previous domain recording
  • 19:11techniques on fast time scales
  • 19:13were used on electrical systems,
  • 19:14and so the problem with this is all
  • 19:17the voltammetry techniques people
  • 19:18use before you couldn't record
  • 19:20responses to foot shocks,
  • 19:22and So what we've been using is
  • 19:25a fluorescent dopamine sensor.
  • 19:26This one is called delight.
  • 19:28It was developed at UC Davis by
  • 19:30Lindsay Angela and what this is is
  • 19:33it's a modified D1 dopamine receptor
  • 19:35that when it binds to dopamine it.
  • 19:38Laura says.
  • 19:38And so this fluorescent sensor is
  • 19:40really great because we can inject
  • 19:42it in with a virus into the brain.
  • 19:44We build fiber, photometry, systems.
  • 19:46I know a lot of people are using these,
  • 19:49but what it allows us to do is in awake
  • 19:51and behaving animals during these
  • 19:53discrete aspects of this behavioral task,
  • 19:55is record fluctuations in joking that
  • 19:57happened through this kind of fluorescent
  • 19:59response that isn't interfering?
  • 20:00Electrical signals and also the
  • 20:02great thing about these optical
  • 20:03sensors is they have really great
  • 20:05signal to noise and so you can get
  • 20:07single trial responses which with
  • 20:08a lot of voltammetry in the past,
  • 20:10which is my backgrounds you didn't get,
  • 20:12you had to average responses and
  • 20:14what I'll show you is a lot of what
  • 20:16we see is these really rapid changes
  • 20:18in dopamine that are happening on
  • 20:19the trial by trial basis that we
  • 20:21think are really critical for
  • 20:23this behavioral response.
  • 20:26So we started with kind
  • 20:27of what everyone is done.
  • 20:29Before this you know it's always good
  • 20:30when you start with like new tools to
  • 20:33make sure you see what everybody else's.
  • 20:35And So what we did is we recorded domain
  • 20:37responses during the pre training session.
  • 20:39The first time the animals had been
  • 20:41in these operating chambers and post
  • 20:42training after the animals had learned
  • 20:44and so not surprising animals learn to
  • 20:46know spoke during a queue for sucrose we
  • 20:48can change the length of the queue to
  • 20:50make the task more or less difficult.
  • 20:52We kind of did this so we had some
  • 20:55dynamic range of whether they did the.
  • 20:57Miss trials or not,
  • 20:58and so we did some machine learning.
  • 20:59I won't show you then.
  • 21:01This was actually a really
  • 21:02great tool for that,
  • 21:03but what we do is we see kind of the
  • 21:05same thing everyone else is seen.
  • 21:07Early on,
  • 21:08when the animals go into the sucrose port,
  • 21:11so red in here this is more
  • 21:13domain response over trials.
  • 21:14When they go into the Super sport,
  • 21:16you get this robust domain
  • 21:18response to the sucrose.
  • 21:20Overtraining this signal
  • 21:21moves back to the cube.
  • 21:23That's very predictive.
  • 21:23So now you get this really robust
  • 21:26domain response to the queue,
  • 21:27but not as much of a domain
  • 21:30response to the sucrose.
  • 21:32Great, it looks just like that equation.
  • 21:34I showed you.
  • 21:35Dopamine goes up to the Q goes
  • 21:37down to the error signal.
  • 21:40All is well in the reward domain
  • 21:43does reward based learning field.
  • 21:45But then we moved on to this
  • 21:47other behavioral task.
  • 21:48So again,
  • 21:49the animals can know spoke
  • 21:50during this Q and they do it to
  • 21:53avoid a series of foot shocks.
  • 21:54So if they don't press during
  • 21:56the Q they get shocked.
  • 21:58There's a series of shocks they
  • 21:59can press anytime during this
  • 22:01series to terminate the shocks,
  • 22:02and so we did the same thing
  • 22:04we recorded early in Leanne.
  • 22:06Learning animals actually learn.
  • 22:07When I started my lab a bunch of behavior.
  • 22:10People told me that animals
  • 22:11will never do this.
  • 22:12Mice do this really great.
  • 22:14They'll learn really rapidly
  • 22:15to press on the nose,
  • 22:17poke that active know spoke
  • 22:18to avoid the shocks.
  • 22:19And they actually at the end
  • 22:21of these trials are doing this,
  • 22:23that they are not getting shots at all,
  • 22:25and so they learn this very fast.
  • 22:27And it's actually really robust task
  • 22:29for generating motivated behavior.
  • 22:30Um?
  • 22:31OK,
  • 22:31so the first thing we saw which goes
  • 22:34in the face of dopamine encoding
  • 22:36rewards is that you get this really
  • 22:39robust positive response to a foot shock.
  • 22:43So dopamine goes up when
  • 22:45aversive stimuli are encountered.
  • 22:47Um,
  • 22:48other people have seen this,
  • 22:49but what's really interesting is I'll
  • 22:51remind you of what that model looked like.
  • 22:53If dopamine is doing reward based learning.
  • 22:56We didn't get this robust
  • 22:58response to the Q like we did with
  • 23:02sucrose overlearning.
  • 23:03We did get a decrease in the response.
  • 23:05We have the safety queue
  • 23:07that came on when the
  • 23:08animals avoided the negative consequences.
  • 23:10So at the end of the trial.
  • 23:13It did go down over learning like
  • 23:15you'd expect of an error signal,
  • 23:17but here's the problem.
  • 23:18The safety queue domain response
  • 23:19was the biggest on the first
  • 23:21trial they ever encountered it
  • 23:22before they could know its value,
  • 23:24and so we were kind of a little
  • 23:26bit hesitant about that.
  • 23:27But we said OK, but maybe this looks kind of.
  • 23:30Maybe it fits.
  • 23:31But then what we found was that Adobe
  • 23:34response to the foot shot during
  • 23:36these trials was not only positive,
  • 23:38it actually increases.
  • 23:40Animals got better at the task and so.
  • 23:43We've looked and we said,
  • 23:45OK, this doesn't really fit.
  • 23:46People had seen this safety Q response
  • 23:48before and said look doping doesn't
  • 23:50work of RP in aversive contexts.
  • 23:52But we looked at the other parameters
  • 23:55that this doesn't really make sense.
  • 23:57So now we have this situation
  • 24:00where dopamine responses in the
  • 24:02nucleus accumbens track these
  • 24:05prediction error based computations.
  • 24:07But only in contexts that are reward
  • 24:10based and so everyone has really kind
  • 24:13of design these experiments to parse
  • 24:15weather domain does RP not does all
  • 24:18of what doping does fit these computations.
  • 24:21So we had a problem.
  • 24:24This reward based Association model
  • 24:26was just too simplistic to account
  • 24:28for what domain was doing in the same
  • 24:31animals in his behavioral tasks.
  • 24:33And what we started looking through
  • 24:35the literature is a lot of people.
  • 24:37What they did is once we have this RP
  • 24:39hypothesis reward prediction error.
  • 24:41Apophysis people started saying OK,
  • 24:42well reward fish based dictionary does.
  • 24:44This. Does dopamine look like this?
  • 24:46And the issue with that is that
  • 24:48dopamine does a lot of stuff that
  • 24:50reward prediction error cannot do,
  • 24:52and so we ended up doing all
  • 24:54this broad prediction error.
  • 24:55Modeling those that math cannot
  • 24:56do negative reinforcement.
  • 24:57So we ended up at this problem where
  • 24:59if we wanted to understand what domain
  • 25:01was doing from a computational model.
  • 25:03These models didn't even make
  • 25:06the computations we needed.
  • 25:07So.
  • 25:08We decided to I have a
  • 25:10postdoc who's fantastic,
  • 25:12who's a computational psychologist.
  • 25:13I will not take credit for
  • 25:15developing the model.
  • 25:16This is not my backgrounds,
  • 25:18but we've had a really great
  • 25:20synergistic relationship.
  • 25:21And So what we did is we created a
  • 25:23complex model of learning and memory.
  • 25:26You have the theoretical components of
  • 25:28this model that are developed from site.
  • 25:30Many years of psychology research
  • 25:32and then what we can do is we can
  • 25:35record domain responses and many many
  • 25:37Contacts and we can map the domain responses.
  • 25:40On to the parameters of this model
  • 25:42that we know is capable of modeling
  • 25:45the behavioral outputs we have.
  • 25:47So I'm not going to go into
  • 25:49details like super details,
  • 25:50but I'm really happy for people if
  • 25:52they have questions to talk more
  • 25:54essentially with the model does is
  • 25:56it models the behavioral responses,
  • 25:58the outcomes,
  • 25:59the predictions like people have done before.
  • 26:01We actually have those prediction
  • 26:02based learning algorithms.
  • 26:03But one thing it has that's actually
  • 26:05was been has been found over years.
  • 26:08So you really critical component of learning.
  • 26:10Is this perceived salience term?
  • 26:11And so this is kind of you know how
  • 26:14attention grabbing something is and
  • 26:15so it's really highly influenced by
  • 26:17things in the environment like novelty.
  • 26:20The first time you experience an unexpected
  • 26:22aversive foot shock or something,
  • 26:23it's it's more attention grabbing
  • 26:25the 10th time you present it,
  • 26:26and so this that with this term does
  • 26:28is it influences how we learn based
  • 26:31on these other factors that are
  • 26:33not included in these other models
  • 26:34and what it does is it's able to
  • 26:37make really accurate predictions of
  • 26:39what animals will do in the future.
  • 26:42Again, we use this model to figure out
  • 26:44what experiments would dissociate these
  • 26:45different factors from one another.
  • 26:47So after I show you this,
  • 26:48you can ignore the model stuff
  • 26:50if you want and just look at the
  • 26:52experiments we run to parse.
  • 26:54Kind of what's going on.
  • 26:56The really important thing about this
  • 26:58model is the simulations from the model.
  • 27:00The behavioral output
  • 27:01simulations are in grey,
  • 27:03and the behavioral data itself
  • 27:04is in blue and you can see it can
  • 27:07start to model these things that
  • 27:09couldn't be modeled before,
  • 27:11so things like the animals to train
  • 27:13to know spoke for sucrose and then
  • 27:15we have we introduce an aversive
  • 27:17foot shock all the sudden.
  • 27:19The animals responding goes down.
  • 27:20That's what the model suggests.
  • 27:22We can model how animals will learn
  • 27:24and negative reinforcement Contacts
  • 27:26the removal of an aversive stimulus.
  • 27:28And so we're now able to model the
  • 27:31behavioral outcomes of these more
  • 27:33complex behaviors but use these same
  • 27:35kind of computations to not punch to me so.
  • 27:38What I'm going to show you is that
  • 27:40dopamine does not track reward
  • 27:42prediction error.
  • 27:42It tracks perceived salience,
  • 27:44which is just kind of like how
  • 27:46attention grabbing a stimulus is.
  • 27:48Perceived salience is the perception
  • 27:51of housing question.
  • 27:52Yes,
  • 27:53just a quick question.
  • 27:55How different is this model from
  • 27:58Pierce Hall Macintosh model of?
  • 28:02It's it's actually very similar,
  • 28:04so it includes the.
  • 28:05So the thing about the Pearsall Macintosh
  • 28:07models for the people who don't know is
  • 28:09they include these attentional terms,
  • 28:11which are really actually important
  • 28:12for things like Leighton ambition,
  • 28:14things that these other models can't do.
  • 28:16It includes the same kind of
  • 28:18computational terms, and this perceived
  • 28:19salience term is essentially kind
  • 28:21of what was added to that model.
  • 28:23It's based on a neural net model,
  • 28:25so it's a little bit different in the math,
  • 28:27but it's the same kind of idea,
  • 28:30and that's it that the
  • 28:31attentional value of things.
  • 28:32Are going to influence the associative
  • 28:34strength and how animals learn,
  • 28:35and so that's that's what we're adding
  • 28:38that allows us to do these things.
  • 28:40And then on top of that, we've added these.
  • 28:42They operate probabilistic responses because
  • 28:44pavlovi and learning is not probabilistic,
  • 28:45and so we've added that to that.
  • 28:47To that kind of framework.
  • 28:49So it's very, very similar to that.
  • 28:51Thank you for that question.
  • 28:52I don't go into too much detail 'cause
  • 28:54people don't always know about these models,
  • 28:57but that's actually kind of the
  • 28:58framework by which we're using it.
  • 29:00More appears Hall that the Macintosh,
  • 29:02though.
  • 29:04OK, so we can model the behavior we need,
  • 29:07so so again,
  • 29:08this perceived salience is kind of
  • 29:10like a driving animals attention
  • 29:11towards States and so it's really
  • 29:13highly affected by the in physical
  • 29:15intensity of a stimulus and the
  • 29:17novelty and environment that
  • 29:18changes how you attend to stimulate.
  • 29:21And So what we did is we started.
  • 29:24We said OK,
  • 29:25if it's tracking just the saliency,
  • 29:27it should increase with physical
  • 29:29intensity of a stimulus.
  • 29:30Whether it's positive or negative.
  • 29:32And so this is a increasing
  • 29:34series of foot shocks on the left.
  • 29:37That is,
  • 29:38the simulations from that perceived
  • 29:40salience term of our model,
  • 29:41and on the right is the actual
  • 29:44dopamine recorded responses to
  • 29:46increasing intensities of foot shocks
  • 29:47and what you see is that dopamine
  • 29:50increases with foot shock intensity.
  • 29:52It also increases when we
  • 29:55increase the volume of sucrose.
  • 29:57So a better than expected appetitive.
  • 30:00Reward or the volume of quiet Night,
  • 30:02which is a bitter taste scent,
  • 30:04and so everyone always asks.
  • 30:06And it's a great question.
  • 30:07Foot shocks are kind of
  • 30:09weird aversive stimuli,
  • 30:10so for many of these tasks we have
  • 30:12worked in other things that are aversive.
  • 30:15But not, you know,
  • 30:16painful.
  • 30:16We can argue all day about whether
  • 30:18foot shock is pain or something else,
  • 30:20but we see the same pattern with
  • 30:22other types of aversive stimulation,
  • 30:24and so this is ruling out simple,
  • 30:26rewarding coding by dopamine,
  • 30:28because dopamine in the incumbents
  • 30:29is going up to both appetitive
  • 30:31and aversive stimuli.
  • 30:32So it cannot be just rewards.
  • 30:36So the other thing that influences
  • 30:39perceived salience is novelty.
  • 30:40So how much experience
  • 30:42you have with something.
  • 30:43So what we did is we took foot
  • 30:46shocks of the same intensity.
  • 30:49Intensity is not changed and we repeated
  • 30:51them in a series on a fixed interval,
  • 30:54so every every 60 seconds
  • 30:56the animals got a foot shot.
  • 30:59What we found is that doping goes down
  • 31:02to the foot shots even though the
  • 31:04intensity of the foot shock stays.
  • 31:07Constant, so it's also not just
  • 31:10encoding only the intensity,
  • 31:12it's encoding other aspects
  • 31:14like novelty as well,
  • 31:16and so you get decreased domain
  • 31:19responses to repeated exposures
  • 31:22of stimulate both aversive.
  • 31:24And neutral simulate,
  • 31:25and so this is an auditory tone,
  • 31:28so we've done this with tones,
  • 31:30lights, and white noise.
  • 31:32This is an example from white noise,
  • 31:34but what you see which is
  • 31:37important is the first exposure
  • 31:39of a stimulus that is neutral,
  • 31:41elicits dopamine and repeated exposures
  • 31:44of that stimulus go down overtime.
  • 31:47So you're getting decreased dopamine
  • 31:49when animals are exposed repeatedly
  • 31:51to stimuli in the environment,
  • 31:53regardless of whether they have
  • 31:55positive value, negative value,
  • 31:57or are what we think of as neutral.
  • 32:01I'm.
  • 32:02So another aspect that I think,
  • 32:05and this is really important experiment
  • 32:07because it really rules out this
  • 32:09reward prediction error based learning.
  • 32:11So what we did is we trained animals to
  • 32:13know spoke during a discriminative cue.
  • 32:16So auditory cue comes on.
  • 32:17If they respond they get sucrose.
  • 32:21Without any signal,
  • 32:22we now switch it to the same auditory cue.
  • 32:25If they press,
  • 32:27they get shocked,
  • 32:28so we're switching that the Q is
  • 32:30the same and it now represents
  • 32:33a worse than expected outcome.
  • 32:36And so.
  • 32:36What should happen in a task
  • 32:38like this is that the animals
  • 32:41in this grezar simulation,
  • 32:42the animal should reduce
  • 32:44their behavioral responding.
  • 32:46They do not surprisingly,
  • 32:47this is a traditional kind
  • 32:48of punishment task.
  • 32:49Animals will reduce their behavior.
  • 32:52But what our model predicts,
  • 32:54this perceived salience model.
  • 32:55Is that because there's unexpected
  • 32:57information and it's novel?
  • 32:59There should be increase
  • 33:01in dopamine to this Q.
  • 33:03Prediction error responding with
  • 33:05say it should be decreased because
  • 33:07it's a worse than expected outcome.
  • 33:10A reward prediction error, excuse me.
  • 33:12So what we're going to look
  • 33:14at here is this Q,
  • 33:16which is the last Q that predicted sucrose,
  • 33:18so this is before the animals
  • 33:20got gotten shocked.
  • 33:21So this Q still has that predict sucrose,
  • 33:24and then we're going to look at the next
  • 33:27Q right after the first foot shock.
  • 33:30And what we find is first,
  • 33:33this foot shock causes a
  • 33:35positive domain response.
  • 33:36So it doesn't matter what kind of task,
  • 33:40but shocks are being presented and
  • 33:42they're always words resulting
  • 33:44in positive domain responses.
  • 33:46What the dopamine response to this
  • 33:48discriminative cue actually goes up,
  • 33:50even though it's it represents a
  • 33:53worse than expected outcomes and
  • 33:55so dopamine is increasing anytime
  • 33:58information is novel or salient
  • 34:00to the animal.
  • 34:01And it's increasing even if
  • 34:03the outcome is worse than
  • 34:05expected or better than expected,
  • 34:07and one of the key aspects of this
  • 34:10experiment is dopamine is going up,
  • 34:12even though the animal's
  • 34:14behavior is going down,
  • 34:15so increases in dopamine don't just
  • 34:17mean motivated behavior or approach
  • 34:19because we're getting increases in
  • 34:21dopamine here that correlate with animals
  • 34:24inhibiting a behavioral response,
  • 34:25and so this kind of saliency.
  • 34:27What it'll do, is it helps animals make
  • 34:31adaptive updating of responses were.
  • 34:33List of what the context of those responses
  • 34:36are or the behavioral response necessary.
  • 34:39So I'm gonna show I think
  • 34:41I've one more experiment,
  • 34:43so this experiment is
  • 34:45actually really important,
  • 34:46because it's kind of shows how much these
  • 34:49kind of novel salients events that don't
  • 34:53necessarily acquire value R to animals.
  • 34:56So what we did is we did an experiment
  • 34:59where we associated ECU with a foot shock,
  • 35:02so just fear conditioning.
  • 35:04But what we did is on some of
  • 35:07the trials we just put a random
  • 35:10irrelevant house light on.
  • 35:12And what the model predicts is that because
  • 35:14there is novelty in the environment,
  • 35:16there will be an increase in dopamine
  • 35:18response on these trials where novel
  • 35:20information is added even though previous
  • 35:23work has shown that novel irrelevant
  • 35:25information will not acquire value.
  • 35:27So the Q that we're adding this random
  • 35:29irrelevant light won't acquire value
  • 35:31because the animals already associated
  • 35:34the previous Q with the foot shot.
  • 35:36And So what we find is on trials
  • 35:38where we add this novel light.
  • 35:41There is a very large increase
  • 35:43in the domain response,
  • 35:44even though that novel light
  • 35:46won't acquire value itself.
  • 35:47And So what this does is it rules
  • 35:50out the simple Attribution of some
  • 35:52sort of balance to a queue or
  • 35:55associative strength of that Q.
  • 35:57It means that you're getting
  • 35:59increases in dopamine responses.
  • 36:00They don't necessarily correspond with
  • 36:02the animal making Association between
  • 36:05that queue and the outcome in some cases.
  • 36:11The last thing, which again for
  • 36:12people who are are really deep
  • 36:14in the prediction based field.
  • 36:16One thing that people can
  • 36:17say at this point is, well,
  • 36:19maybe doping is doing prediction error
  • 36:21but not reward prediction error.
  • 36:22So it's going up every time
  • 36:24there is an error in prediction.
  • 36:26This is actually really good good thought
  • 36:28and we thought this too and we said OK.
  • 36:30Well let's let's see if that is the
  • 36:33case and we were kind of Gnostic here.
  • 36:35We were saying, OK,
  • 36:36let's just figure out what it does.
  • 36:38We're not trying to.
  • 36:40Pusha theory we're saying, well,
  • 36:41how does the data fit together?
  • 36:44So saliency or perceived salience?
  • 36:46What it would suggest is that when
  • 36:48you have a stimulus like a foot shock,
  • 36:52you should have the biggest opening response,
  • 36:55because when the stimulus is
  • 36:56present and there it's the most
  • 36:59salient 'cause most intense.
  • 37:00But if you have a prediction of
  • 37:03that during extinction or omission,
  • 37:05there should still be a positive
  • 37:08doping response,
  • 37:08but it should be lower than when the
  • 37:11stimulus is physically there a prediction.
  • 37:14Error hypothesis would be that
  • 37:16when you have an omission,
  • 37:17this response should be higher
  • 37:19than when the stimulus is there
  • 37:21because it signals an error.
  • 37:25We did this experiment.
  • 37:27What we found was that there's a
  • 37:29positive domain response at the time
  • 37:31of the predicted foot shock when
  • 37:33it's omitted, so it's not there,
  • 37:35but the response of the foot shock
  • 37:38itself is higher than when it's omitted.
  • 37:40And so this also rules out
  • 37:43other competing theories,
  • 37:44which is that domain does
  • 37:45prediction error learning,
  • 37:46but it's not reward based.
  • 37:50So. And I showed you a lot of stuff and,
  • 37:54well, I kind of rule kind of come
  • 37:56back and say like why should you care?
  • 37:59So essentially what we did is we did
  • 38:01a number of experiments to rule out
  • 38:03these kind of competing factors of what
  • 38:05dopamine is doing in learning and memory.
  • 38:07Don't mean release is doing.
  • 38:08I'm not saying the VTA cell
  • 38:10bodies don't do this.
  • 38:11Maybe they do,
  • 38:11but there's integration of information
  • 38:13at the level of the terminal that
  • 38:15dictates how doing is actually
  • 38:16releasing these brain regions and
  • 38:17what I am saying is that dopamine
  • 38:19release in the nucleus accumbens,
  • 38:21core Maps on true perceived.
  • 38:22Salience not prediction error or value.
  • 38:27Our models are modeled behavior,
  • 38:28we just use it to generate experiments
  • 38:30we should run to parse different
  • 38:32aspects of domain encoding,
  • 38:33and So what you can do is use
  • 38:35these kind of predictions to make
  • 38:37experiments with other circuits as well,
  • 38:39which I think is kind of an interesting
  • 38:42way to approach the question.
  • 38:45But what I'm showing you is that
  • 38:47even in reinforcement context,
  • 38:48pavlovi in context,
  • 38:49this isn't a value based prediction signal,
  • 38:51and these same signals are
  • 38:53there in punishment tasks in
  • 38:54negative reinforcement tasks,
  • 38:55and so it's actually really
  • 38:57interesting that you're seeing
  • 38:58this kind of dopamine signal.
  • 39:00It's very critical in driving behaviors,
  • 39:02just not in the way that I
  • 39:04think we predicted before.
  • 39:07So why should we care?
  • 39:08I think understanding what domain is
  • 39:11doing is really important for disease,
  • 39:13and so if you want to understand what
  • 39:15dopamine is doing and what deficits in
  • 39:18dopamine in a patient mean for that patient,
  • 39:21it really requires a kind of
  • 39:23holistic understanding.
  • 39:24What domains doing across contexts and
  • 39:26internal States and things like that,
  • 39:28and so you know when you have a model and
  • 39:32you say doesken dopamine fit this model,
  • 39:35the answer might be yes.
  • 39:37But it kind of leaves out that aspect of.
  • 39:39But what is domain doing in other
  • 39:42contexts of the model can't fit and
  • 39:44so understanding the components that
  • 39:46dry of these behaviors is really
  • 39:49critical to understanding this.
  • 39:51But I think that maybe more
  • 39:53important thing for for kind of
  • 39:55human health is is from, you know,
  • 39:57my primary field which is
  • 39:59addiction an it's understand.
  • 40:00The difference between a dopamine
  • 40:02signal that signals were worn
  • 40:04and what a salience signal does.
  • 40:05So a reward signal.
  • 40:07If you have a deficits in a reward signal,
  • 40:09you may say you know we don't
  • 40:11want to increase those and people
  • 40:13suffering from substance use disorder
  • 40:15because if we do that may increase
  • 40:17the rewarding value of stimuli
  • 40:19in the environment like drugs.
  • 40:21But the issue is with the salience signal.
  • 40:24If you have deficits,
  • 40:26it's going to slow the rate of learning
  • 40:29for everything in the environment,
  • 40:31so it could explain why people
  • 40:34are compulsive because they don't
  • 40:36respond to negative outcomes.
  • 40:38It would explain why they have trouble
  • 40:41learning. The adaptive alternatives.
  • 40:42Are there an?
  • 40:43It would explain why extinguishing
  • 40:46drug associations is much slower,
  • 40:48and so in if it's a saliency signal,
  • 40:51we may want to.
  • 40:52Increased opening so that people can
  • 40:54learn adaptively in all of these contexts.
  • 40:57And so again,
  • 40:58I'm not saying that like you know,
  • 41:00this is the end all be all dopamine is
  • 41:02in lots of projection targets and it
  • 41:04does lots of things in different areas.
  • 41:07And we're in one single area,
  • 41:08but I think kind of taking a step
  • 41:10back and thinking about what these
  • 41:12domains signatures really mean
  • 41:14and what those deficits would
  • 41:15mean to a behaving individual,
  • 41:17as I think it's really important
  • 41:19component of conceptualizing
  • 41:20what these you know psychiatric
  • 41:22deficits mean to people and how
  • 41:24to best treat them.
  • 41:25Anyway, so with that I'll
  • 41:27end with thanking my lab,
  • 41:29so Ganesh clue his background
  • 41:30is in computational psychology,
  • 41:31so he's like the modeler and he's
  • 41:34really like driven this you know.
  • 41:35Together he's a Pavlovian guy.
  • 41:37I was a reinforcement person.
  • 41:39I think this was like one of
  • 41:40those projects that was this great
  • 41:42synergism between two people who
  • 41:44just we're really excited about.
  • 41:46Kind of figuring out what's going on.
  • 41:48Jennifer, Zachary and Patrick and
  • 41:50Stephanie were are grad students
  • 41:52that were working on this project
  • 41:54and put a lot of time into it.
  • 41:56Cody Siciliano and Lindsay Ann Lynn
  • 41:58was was really nice and was helping us
  • 42:01get the delight these optical sensors
  • 42:03up and running in the lab fairly
  • 42:06early in Cody's at optical engineer.
  • 42:07So he helps a lot at Vanderbilt with
  • 42:10getting these working correctly,
  • 42:12validating them an my funding and I
  • 42:14can take any questions you may have.
  • 42:17So thank you so much.
  • 42:22Thank you so much Aaron.
  • 42:23If anybody doesn't want to pipe
  • 42:25up with the just asking questions,
  • 42:27please put them in the chat and
  • 42:30I can read them out for Aaron.
  • 42:33Like can I start with one?
  • 42:35Go for it. So in terms of those
  • 42:38projections you were discussing
  • 42:40and you're looking in the core,
  • 42:42do you think that just with predictions
  • 42:44of what the core versus shell of the
  • 42:47incumbents does in activations of the
  • 42:49core versus Shell does to behavior?
  • 42:51Do you think that one might be
  • 42:54more important than the other in
  • 42:56the prediction error?
  • 42:57This is actually really
  • 42:59great question an I should
  • 43:00have pasted this.
  • 43:01Actually I can do it now.
  • 43:04I we have shell data so so.
  • 43:06The answer is probably yes in some contexts,
  • 43:09although when we we started to
  • 43:12look through the shell data.
  • 43:15So I just wanted to side when
  • 43:17we started to look through the
  • 43:20new data or the shell data.
  • 43:22It did not look like what
  • 43:24I would expect either.
  • 43:28Basically. It still doesn't
  • 43:31look like prediction error.
  • 43:33So we still get a positive
  • 43:36domain response to the shock.
  • 43:38We get some scaling with stimulus intensity.
  • 43:41Not quite as much.
  • 43:44People have shown decreases in dopamine
  • 43:46and to aversive stimuli and we have
  • 43:49been working out why that would be when
  • 43:51we aren't seeing them and we think.
  • 43:53And so basically we did show
  • 43:55decreases in domain in some contexts.
  • 43:57Dopamine goes down when animals don't
  • 44:00have to do anything or they have to wait.
  • 44:02So what we did is we design
  • 44:05this other experiment that I'm
  • 44:06like really excited about.
  • 44:08What we did is we trained animals to know
  • 44:11smoker sucrose and then we switched the
  • 44:13contingency so that they had to with.
  • 44:16Hold a response and wait to get sucrose.
  • 44:18So this is kind of like the same reciprocal
  • 44:20thing to fear conditioning right?
  • 44:22You have a queue.
  • 44:23The animal just waits to get shocked.
  • 44:25There's nothing they can
  • 44:26do during that period.
  • 44:27They wait,
  • 44:28we see decreases in dopamine to
  • 44:30fear conditioning Q and two AQ,
  • 44:31where the animal gets sucrose at the end,
  • 44:34but they have to wait to do it.
  • 44:37And So what we think is happening is a
  • 44:39lot of these decreases in domain people
  • 44:42have seen or not necessarily just value.
  • 44:45They have to do with what animals
  • 44:47are doing and what novelty in
  • 44:49salience do in an environment
  • 44:51is they increase exploration.
  • 44:53So if you need to decrease exploration
  • 44:55and just wait for something to happen,
  • 44:58domain goes down.
  • 44:59So we get doping reductions even when
  • 45:02the outcome is positive when the task
  • 45:04design mimics that of the aversive
  • 45:06tasks where people have seen reductions.
  • 45:09And a lot of the like you know,
  • 45:11there's a lot of great work from
  • 45:13like Mitchell White Men looking at
  • 45:15aversive like wine in the mouth.
  • 45:17It's unavoidable.
  • 45:17The animals are just waiting there as well.
  • 45:20And so I think there's also
  • 45:21differences in relative,
  • 45:22you know,
  • 45:23in his designs he has positive and
  • 45:25negative stimuli in the same task,
  • 45:27which are a little bit different
  • 45:28than having an animal behaves.
  • 45:30So obviously there's there is some
  • 45:31sort of value based computation,
  • 45:33but we think they're done and
  • 45:35really specific context.
  • 45:38Thank you.
  • 45:39I think the other question go ahead.
  • 45:44But they are zoom etiquette.
  • 45:46Go ahead, I'll jump in after you.
  • 45:49OK, thanks for bringing talker
  • 45:50and that was really really cool.
  • 45:52So one thing that you can do with
  • 45:55these models is sort of see if they
  • 45:57can predict particular phenomena
  • 45:58and one of the ideas I think that's
  • 46:01becoming more and more prevalent
  • 46:02about a sort of teleological idea
  • 46:04about what the dopamine system
  • 46:06might be for is not rewards,
  • 46:08not punishments as you've argued,
  • 46:09but actually the causal
  • 46:10structure of the world.
  • 46:12Yeah, and so does your model
  • 46:14predict things like sensory
  • 46:15preconditioning where?
  • 46:16There was
  • 46:16no value at all.
  • 46:18Initially you use that information
  • 46:19later to imbue or impede value.
  • 46:21Yes, yes, so actually this is one
  • 46:23of the other powers of this model is
  • 46:25it can do sensory preconditioning,
  • 46:28Layton addition,
  • 46:28so these are two things that that even
  • 46:31the temporal difference models cannot do,
  • 46:33and the problem is dopamine does them.
  • 46:36So if dopamine does these
  • 46:37in a computational model,
  • 46:39can't that cannot be the
  • 46:41computation domain is doing,
  • 46:42and so we we have our next at
  • 46:45once we get this out the door.
  • 46:47I'm trying to find my.
  • 46:49We haven't started doing sensory
  • 46:50preconditioning yet because of the fact that
  • 46:53it's a little bit more of a pain in mice,
  • 46:56and I think we're going to
  • 46:58have to switch to rats.
  • 47:00Mice aren't like the best set
  • 47:02like attending to things so,
  • 47:04so lame.
  • 47:05Inhibition is something that
  • 47:06our model does do.
  • 47:08So late inhibition is actually
  • 47:10this really interesting novelty
  • 47:11based learning constructs were
  • 47:13essentially pre exposed stimuli,
  • 47:14acquire values slower than simulated.
  • 47:16Have not been pre exposed so familiar
  • 47:18stimuli take longer to because you're
  • 47:20basically unlearning the no Association.
  • 47:23So a lot of these different models
  • 47:25you know we brought this up earlier.
  • 47:28I was asked about Pierce Hall Macintosh.
  • 47:30Like all of these models have
  • 47:33added these components to do this
  • 47:35and our model does this and the
  • 47:37sensory preconditioning.
  • 47:39I showed you the dobine goes
  • 47:41down to repeated shocks.
  • 47:42Um?
  • 47:42We can get pre exposed stimuli to
  • 47:45have less associative value and
  • 47:47what's really interesting is that
  • 47:50the dopamine response to these pre
  • 47:53exposed stimuli is much lower and it
  • 47:56also tracks over the pre exposure period.
  • 47:59So these these kind of non value based
  • 48:01learning constructs were previous
  • 48:03experience is changing the way that
  • 48:05stimuli can drive future behavior
  • 48:07or sensory preconditioning were two
  • 48:10irrelevant simulate form associations
  • 48:12that can then be associated later.
  • 48:14Our model does it,
  • 48:15and dopamine still Maps onto that
  • 48:17perceived salience term in those contexts.
  • 48:19And this is actually why we were so set on.
  • 48:21You know,
  • 48:22the first experiments really well
  • 48:23it could be other things too,
  • 48:25and then we started going
  • 48:26into these latent addition and
  • 48:27sensory preconditioning ideas,
  • 48:29because those really can't be other things.
  • 48:31I mean,
  • 48:31it could be there's other components of it,
  • 48:34but I think it is more strong
  • 48:35with the other stuff.
  • 48:37It does too, that that's what it's doing.
  • 48:39But it's that's a great, I think.
  • 48:41Those are like the killer like knife
  • 48:43in the coffin experiments, right?
  • 48:44Because they just.
  • 48:45Those other models cannot do them,
  • 48:47so yeah, that's a great great point.
  • 48:51I think we had a question,
  • 48:52thanks. Those are really great talk here
  • 48:54and thanks for taking us through all that.
  • 48:56And you have touched on my question
  • 48:58a little bit because you started.
  • 49:00I mean, even with the default
  • 49:02Pomeranians when he starts to
  • 49:03look at nucleus accumbens shell.
  • 49:04But one thing I was curious about with your
  • 49:07model is how how global do you think it is?
  • 49:10And you hinted as we talked about the
  • 49:12VTA component as well. So you know,
  • 49:14going back to some of the Bromberg,
  • 49:16Martin work about different VTA
  • 49:17neurons responding differently
  • 49:18to balance versus silence.
  • 49:19You think this Maps onto multiple regions
  • 49:21using this sub regions within the.
  • 49:23The common score is a global number
  • 49:25as a discrete to specific places.
  • 49:27Yeah, so this is a great question,
  • 49:29so I'm lucky to have married very well in
  • 49:32my life and my partner is a two photon guide.
  • 49:35When he does,
  • 49:36he does 2 photon imaging through grin lenses,
  • 49:39cranial windows, you name it and
  • 49:40so one thing we're working on now.
  • 49:43The really power of of these kind
  • 49:45of optical imaging approaches is
  • 49:47that you can record as small as
  • 49:49you want or as big as you want,
  • 49:51and so depending on your.
  • 49:53Microscope and resolution so
  • 49:54we're moving into these.
  • 49:55Either I love slice work.
  • 49:57This is like my background so
  • 49:59we're moving into these kind of.
  • 50:00In vivo in slice imaging approaches
  • 50:02to understand better domain
  • 50:04regulation across big and small areas.
  • 50:06Because the thing about domain
  • 50:07neurons that so you know,
  • 50:09like kind of weird about
  • 50:11them is their projection.
  • 50:12Like you know, Arborization is insane.
  • 50:14If you fill a single domain on the PTA
  • 50:17and look at the field that it populates,
  • 50:20it's like half the straight up.
  • 50:22But then if you look at these specific
  • 50:24release sites on these neurons,
  • 50:26it's not releasing dopamine
  • 50:27at everywhere every time.
  • 50:29It's depending on all these different things.
  • 50:31So this.
  • 50:32Release structure is so complicated and
  • 50:33I think part of the reason people have
  • 50:36been so like oh volume transmission
  • 50:38is our ability to really look at these
  • 50:40granularities between these components.
  • 50:42And so we're starting to go,
  • 50:44you know, start big.
  • 50:45We're just saying,
  • 50:46OK,
  • 50:46if we do image in a bigger field or
  • 50:49with multiple sites at the same time,
  • 50:51are we seeing differences?
  • 50:52We do see differences so I think there
  • 50:55are differences in these VTA neurons in
  • 50:57what what they're doing in different areas.
  • 50:59So I don't think like all dopamine is.
  • 51:02This I think don't mean to the core is this,
  • 51:05but it also makes sense that domain
  • 51:06in the core that's been tide more to
  • 51:08instrumental responding than like the shell.
  • 51:10That's like these acquisition.
  • 51:11And like Valeant space kind of learning
  • 51:13would look like a perceived salience term,
  • 51:15right?
  • 51:15That makes way more sense for something
  • 51:17that is involved in punishment,
  • 51:18negative reinforcement,
  • 51:19positive reinforcement,
  • 51:19which are the same motivated responses,
  • 51:21independent violence.
  • 51:22So we're doing some more stuff in the
  • 51:24shell. You know, I'm not sold that the
  • 51:26shell doesn't do value because I don't think
  • 51:28foot shocks are the best way to do stuff.
  • 51:30I think foot shocks are weird.
  • 51:32Stimulus that are really powerful initially,
  • 51:34but we didn't really evolve
  • 51:36to respond to foot shocks,
  • 51:37so we're starting to go in
  • 51:39more with things like Quy 9.
  • 51:41We've been developing.
  • 51:42You can make them liquor lic ometer hot,
  • 51:45so we've been doing like thermal,
  • 51:47like not pain, but thermal sensitivity
  • 51:49curves so that you can look at thermal
  • 51:51stimuli that reduced responding.
  • 51:53But without this, like foot shot component,
  • 51:55so we're trying to parse this out.
  • 51:57I'm not sold that,
  • 51:58it's just like every dopamine responses
  • 52:00that I think it's more complicated,
  • 52:02but I think we need better resolution
  • 52:05techniques to really parse that.
  • 52:07And hopefully over the next I don't know.
  • 52:09However long my career last
  • 52:10will see will start to get it.
  • 52:11Some of those questions and
  • 52:12other people are doing that too.
  • 52:14I mean,
  • 52:14there's some really great work
  • 52:15coming out where people are
  • 52:16using those like single synapse.
  • 52:18You know,
  • 52:18you know Uncaging and Eli and
  • 52:19all kinds of crazy stuff,
  • 52:21so I'm excited to see where the field goes.
  • 52:23Yeah,
  • 52:23that's really excited how
  • 52:24great you are looking
  • 52:25at. I'm glad you're looking
  • 52:26into it. Sounds like
  • 52:27you thought about it already. Definitely
  • 52:28I went a little bit. Now
  • 52:29the question is just like do we
  • 52:31have the tools and then the next
  • 52:33thing is do we have the month of
  • 52:34money and the people that do it?
  • 52:36And so it's like you know.
  • 52:38You you see what you can do and
  • 52:39with the resources you have so.
  • 52:44There is a question in the chat by
  • 52:47from Denise, Baghdad and Denise.
  • 52:48Do you want to read it out or
  • 52:50would you like me to ask it?
  • 52:53Good morning, great talk,
  • 52:56so I just wanted to understand something.
  • 53:00Maybe it's not a great question.
  • 53:03Let me just say it.
  • 53:06So nicotine reinforcement is
  • 53:09generally considered as positive
  • 53:11reinforcement enforcement.
  • 53:12However, it's also discussed
  • 53:14about like the weather.
  • 53:17Nicotine reinforcement is
  • 53:18negative reinforcement.
  • 53:20Becausw of nicotine withdrawal.
  • 53:22The compost.
  • 53:23Open intake and taking is actually
  • 53:27contributes to nicotine reinforcement,
  • 53:29so I am interested in whether your
  • 53:33model could dissect the positive or
  • 53:36negative reinforcement for the nicotine.
  • 53:39It's I know it's your.
  • 53:44Shock, but this is the one molecule.
  • 53:47You know, so could have both.
  • 53:49So how we put the fact
  • 53:51that's a great question.
  • 53:53So one of the things in the addiction
  • 53:55field is that there are all these
  • 53:57series of negative reinforcement, right?
  • 53:59Like opioid withdrawal alcohol,
  • 54:01withdrawal.
  • 54:01All of these are negative
  • 54:03reinforcement concepts,
  • 54:03but no one actually does
  • 54:05negative reinforcement.
  • 54:06We make the inference that is negative
  • 54:08reinforcement from the fact that it
  • 54:10causes withdrawn animals are taking
  • 54:12it with during the withdrawal period.
  • 54:15It's OK,
  • 54:15so it's a hard question.
  • 54:17I think the first step would
  • 54:19be to look at how you know if
  • 54:22negative reinforcement processes,
  • 54:23like for avoiding shocks,
  • 54:25are changed after nicotine.
  • 54:26So one of the things that we're
  • 54:28working with with Cody Siciliano is
  • 54:30looking at how alcohol changes animals
  • 54:32motivation for negative reinforcers,
  • 54:34and so that's like the first step.
  • 54:37I think this is like it's
  • 54:39a hard thing to parse.
  • 54:41Nicotine is also, I know it's like I do.
  • 54:45A cholinergic regulation
  • 54:46of dopamine terminals.
  • 54:47So nicotine is like in my mind,
  • 54:49but like we don't do nicotine
  • 54:51reinforcement stuff.
  • 54:51It's also this really interesting
  • 54:53molecule because it regulates
  • 54:54like how dopamine is released
  • 54:56in a really interesting way.
  • 54:57That's not just like up,
  • 54:59it's changing like phasic responses
  • 55:00to stimuli in the environment.
  • 55:02And so thinking about the
  • 55:04interaction between those,
  • 55:05it's like we're doing some work
  • 55:07with sex differences in that
  • 55:08system is like so much more
  • 55:10complicated than I want it to be.
  • 55:12Like with cocaine,
  • 55:13it's like it binds to the transporter.
  • 55:15Show me goes up.
  • 55:17Can we reduce that nicotine is like Oh
  • 55:19well in some cases domain goes down
  • 55:22some cases it goes up and so it's
  • 55:24just such a complicated question.
  • 55:25I think the behavioral stuff we do
  • 55:27can start to parse how processes and
  • 55:29animals are changing by exposure,
  • 55:31and I think that's the first step
  • 55:33and then the next step is trying.
  • 55:35We're trying to develop task
  • 55:36to figure out how to do this.
  • 55:38So one thing we've been thinking
  • 55:41about is doing.
  • 55:42Old school drag discrimination so
  • 55:44animals will actually press before
  • 55:46like to tell you an internal state
  • 55:48is X or Y and what we want to do
  • 55:50is we've been thinking about doing
  • 55:52this with optogenetics, right?
  • 55:53Does an optical stimulation of a
  • 55:55circuit substitute for X drug X
  • 55:57state and I think that some of these
  • 55:58withdrawal effects you could see if,
  • 56:00like nicotine withdrawal,
  • 56:01substituted for some of these other things,
  • 56:03and if that was a critical component
  • 56:05of reinforcement isn't hard question.
  • 56:07I think that that's it can
  • 56:09start answering that question,
  • 56:10but I've been thinking about this a lot
  • 56:12and I'm not sure how to specifically.
  • 56:14Parse when an animal is doing something
  • 56:17for two things at the same time.
  • 56:18What component is what I?
  • 56:20I wish I had better answer.
  • 56:22I'm excited about the question,
  • 56:23but I don't have the answer for you.
  • 56:29Is there time for one more Marina
  • 56:32or do we directly is that Beth?
  • 56:34No, it's less less sorry.
  • 56:37I had one too. OK first Liz then Jane.
  • 56:42So Aaron, that was such a beautiful
  • 56:43talk and I love all the different
  • 56:46behavioral experiments that
  • 56:47were inspired by your model.
  • 56:48And one of the powers of this
  • 56:50model is obviously you could
  • 56:52take that salience term out.
  • 56:53And guess how behavior would be
  • 56:55altered by it in the future.
  • 56:57So I'm curious whether you're going
  • 56:59to start looking at blocking these
  • 57:00signals and seeing whether they
  • 57:02match the expectations that the
  • 57:04model would make in particular,
  • 57:05that when you were showing the responses
  • 57:07to the light cue during conditioning,
  • 57:09which shouldn't be involving any learning
  • 57:11like what's the point of that signal?
  • 57:14Our behavior could come from it.
  • 57:15I'm
  • 57:16so excited to just ask me this one.
  • 57:18OK, so there's.
  • 57:21I'm lazy and I don't want to.
  • 57:22Maybe I'll do this why I
  • 57:24should have put these in here.
  • 57:25I didn't think people were going to have.
  • 57:27Not that I didn't think you
  • 57:28would have great questions,
  • 57:29but I didn't think you guys were to ask
  • 57:32questions that I had like specific data for.
  • 57:34OK, so two things. First thing first.
  • 57:37Wow, that looks terrible.
  • 57:39We did do experiments to
  • 57:40eliminate this signal.
  • 57:41You can see I'm like really crafty with this.
  • 57:44This is not my OK so we First things first.
  • 57:47Yes, I'll tell you what I think
  • 57:49that signal is doing and then two.
  • 57:51Well, two.
  • 57:52I'll show you the optic Jenner.
  • 57:54Other we're not.
  • 57:55We're almost done with the
  • 57:56Histology so take this with it.
  • 57:58This is preliminary preliminary ish.
  • 58:00We did two experiments where we
  • 58:02inhibited the signal using what our
  • 58:04model would predict as the condition
  • 58:06response that would dissociate
  • 58:07it from these other components.
  • 58:09Injected Halo rhodopsin in TH
  • 58:11positive neurons in the VTA and
  • 58:13then inhibited the terminal.
  • 58:14So we're only inhibiting
  • 58:15dopamine releasing terminals.
  • 58:16Any comments?
  • 58:18We either inhibited during a Q
  • 58:22predicting fear conditioning.
  • 58:25Or we inhibited or?
  • 58:27We know we stimulated during.
  • 58:28Sorry this is my fault.
  • 58:30We stimulated during these are
  • 58:32two different things.
  • 58:33We stimulated during a fear conditioning
  • 58:36Q or we stimulated channelrhodopsin
  • 58:38during an emitted but expected shock.
  • 58:41If you stimulate and this gets
  • 58:43your questions during a Q,
  • 58:45that's a fair condition.
  • 58:46Q You actually get less freezing,
  • 58:49so this is the opposite of what you
  • 58:51would expect from associative strength,
  • 58:53but it kind of person.
  • 58:55We basically points to this question when
  • 58:58there's novel stimuli in the environment,
  • 59:00you increase exploration.
  • 59:01All of our data,
  • 59:03like the novel Q.
  • 59:04All of our data show that
  • 59:06when you add novelty,
  • 59:08you increase dopamine and the increased
  • 59:10dopamine is associated with more exploration.
  • 59:12And less freezing.
  • 59:13And so these novelty terms,
  • 59:15what they're doing is they're
  • 59:17helping animals to adaptively
  • 59:18learn by increasing exploration.
  • 59:20And like here, decreasing freezing.
  • 59:22So in the same animal,
  • 59:23the queue that wasn't stimulated has just
  • 59:26as much freezing is the wifey group,
  • 59:28and when it's stimulated they freeze less
  • 59:31and that's what our model would predict.
  • 59:33The other thing we did is we show
  • 59:36that we can prevent extinction,
  • 59:38freezing extinction by
  • 59:39stimulating dopamine to the Q,
  • 59:41and so we both prevent extinction.
  • 59:43We prevent extinction by basically
  • 59:45like increasing the salience of that
  • 59:47event so that it doesn't go away,
  • 59:49which is not the same as you'd expect
  • 59:52by these prediction error terms.
  • 59:54So what we think is happening
  • 59:55is that these novel stimuli
  • 59:57are increasing dopamine that
  • 59:58increase in dopamine promotes and.
  • 01:00:00Exploration term,
  • 01:00:01we also did some like deep lab cut based,
  • 01:00:04you know,
  • 01:00:05machine learning algorithms to
  • 01:00:06look at orienting responses.
  • 01:00:08It's not associated with
  • 01:00:09general motor activity,
  • 01:00:10it's associated with orientation
  • 01:00:12towards the novel stimulus.
  • 01:00:13And so we think that these these
  • 01:00:15this saliency term in the Commons
  • 01:00:17is changing the way the animals are
  • 01:00:19interacting with the environment
  • 01:00:21rather than just the associated value.
  • 01:00:23But the problem is these are such
  • 01:00:26complex things to dissociate that.
  • 01:00:28I understand why people solve
  • 01:00:29the data before instead.
  • 01:00:31Oh, it's that balance goes up to rewards,
  • 01:00:33goes down to a fear conditioning Q
  • 01:00:35that looks like violence to me too.
  • 01:00:37You only start to see that
  • 01:00:38it can't be balanced.
  • 01:00:39When you do these kind of really
  • 01:00:41in the weeds like someone showed
  • 01:00:43this in 1950 in psychology,
  • 01:00:44we're gonna do this again with optogenetics
  • 01:00:46kinds of experiments which I don't know.
  • 01:00:48I think those are the fun experiments,
  • 01:00:50but does that answer your question?
  • 01:00:51Yes, thank you, awesome.
  • 01:00:53I think James next and then Rick.
  • 01:00:55Hi that was a great talk.
  • 01:00:59So my question is.
  • 01:01:02You talk about increasing dopamine,
  • 01:01:05and in most of your experiments where
  • 01:01:08you're actually measuring dopamine,
  • 01:01:10you're looking at.
  • 01:01:12Using Delight an if you go back to the
  • 01:01:16not the old psychology experiments,
  • 01:01:19but the 80s dopamine literature.
  • 01:01:21People think about tonic versus phasic,
  • 01:01:24dopamine and a lot of your experiments
  • 01:01:26seem to be focused more on what
  • 01:01:30the phasic dopamine signal is.
  • 01:01:32An I'm wondering whether you have
  • 01:01:35some way that you can simultaneously
  • 01:01:38look at dopamine tone because it
  • 01:01:40may be that things like novelty.
  • 01:01:43Might actually be linked to some
  • 01:01:45of those and you get you know
  • 01:01:47interactions between the two,
  • 01:01:49and you know when you're you're seeing
  • 01:01:51your increase with the delight.
  • 01:01:53What baseline is that on?
  • 01:01:54This is,
  • 01:01:55I'm like you guys
  • 01:01:56are like making my day.
  • 01:01:58I have like I'm this is
  • 01:02:00such a great question.
  • 01:02:02So this is like our first like thing.
  • 01:02:04We're getting out the door.
  • 01:02:06We have a bunch of extra data where what
  • 01:02:09we've been doing and this is the thing.
  • 01:02:12OK, so voltammetry.
  • 01:02:13Is background subtracted?
  • 01:02:14So you can't really get both tonan,
  • 01:02:17phasic stuff in the same experiment.
  • 01:02:19So what people historically done as
  • 01:02:21they said, microanalysis histone,
  • 01:02:23gigha Ruth and voltammetry is
  • 01:02:24is phasic an my the 80s domain.
  • 01:02:27Literatures like where I started my
  • 01:02:29career so I'm very excited about that.
  • 01:02:31Do you like this kind of nice because
  • 01:02:34you have some photobleaching but you
  • 01:02:36can control for that and you don't
  • 01:02:39know what the the problem with it
  • 01:02:41is that you don't know the number.
  • 01:02:44So with microdialysis you get an amount.
  • 01:02:46With voltammetry you calibrate your probe,
  • 01:02:47you have an estimated amount with delight.
  • 01:02:51I haven't found a great way to
  • 01:02:53figure out what the number is,
  • 01:02:54but you can look at relative
  • 01:02:57changes over the session.
  • 01:02:58I don't have this data up and is easier
  • 01:03:01way because we're putting it into
  • 01:03:02something so we have a manuscript that
  • 01:03:05we're getting together and its focus
  • 01:03:07on novelty based changes and joking signals,
  • 01:03:10and so it's focused on Lane in addition,
  • 01:03:12But what we're looking at is
  • 01:03:14the phasic response relative
  • 01:03:15to longer changes in dopamine,
  • 01:03:17and I don't want to call it tone
  • 01:03:20because it's over like 10 minutes.
  • 01:03:22Not like ours,
  • 01:03:23but it's definitely not what you
  • 01:03:25would call a phasic fast response.
  • 01:03:27What novelty in the environment does?
  • 01:03:29Is it increases that phasic response?
  • 01:03:31But then it does it.
  • 01:03:32The baseline is much higher.
  • 01:03:34So what you have is this shift and
  • 01:03:36what we think is happening is that
  • 01:03:38the novelty is changing the state of
  • 01:03:41the system so that if the next thing
  • 01:03:43that's encountered in that situation
  • 01:03:45the domain response will be bigger.
  • 01:03:47Now one of the things I've always
  • 01:03:49been interested in over my career
  • 01:03:51is what matters for the animal that
  • 01:03:53change from baseline or the peak.
  • 01:03:55And so we're trying to get into that.
  • 01:03:57Now to say like, OK, that increasing.
  • 01:04:00Slide does that just increase the peak?
  • 01:04:03Or does that actually still amplify
  • 01:04:05more this signal to noise and so these
  • 01:04:09novelty terms are definitely changing.
  • 01:04:11What I would call.
  • 01:04:13I don't want to,
  • 01:04:15but they are definitely changing
  • 01:04:16these slower baseline fluctuations in
  • 01:04:18dopamine over longer periods of time,
  • 01:04:20and we think that's really important
  • 01:04:22for the effects of novelty on
  • 01:04:24other types of learning,
  • 01:04:25and so this dopamine,
  • 01:04:26perceived salience,
  • 01:04:27perceived failings is influenced by novelty,
  • 01:04:29so anything that changes novelty
  • 01:04:31will change this as well,
  • 01:04:32and so it's really important in
  • 01:04:34these novelty based learning
  • 01:04:36things on these both slow and fast
  • 01:04:38timescales in a way that is,
  • 01:04:40I think,
  • 01:04:40consistent with what people have seen
  • 01:04:42with microdialysis and voltammetry,
  • 01:04:44but in a more you are able to more granularly
  • 01:04:46relate them with this, But again,
  • 01:04:48you don't have the amount, so I kind of,
  • 01:04:50you know, it's hard because there's no
  • 01:04:52like calibration like you can't say,
  • 01:04:54oh, this is the amount,
  • 01:04:55and I think that's where I hesitate
  • 01:04:57a little bit to make these really
  • 01:04:59strong conclusions about like what.
  • 01:05:00It is, I know that it's changed from
  • 01:05:02the minute one of this session.
  • 01:05:04The question is like exactly what is that?
  • 01:05:06But there are those changes and I think
  • 01:05:08that's a great point that people.
  • 01:05:10I think a lot of people have gone into
  • 01:05:12these kind of optical measurements and they.
  • 01:05:15That was ignored,
  • 01:05:16but they're not necessarily rooted in these,
  • 01:05:19like microdialysis.
  • 01:05:19Will Tanistry fields,
  • 01:05:21and so they don't understand that
  • 01:05:23there has been a ton of work parsing
  • 01:05:26what these tonic changes mean?
  • 01:05:28How tonic dopamine is regulated relative
  • 01:05:31to like fast release like the domain
  • 01:05:33transporters? My favorite protein because
  • 01:05:35I guess the question I guess I'm asking is,
  • 01:05:39would you see some more reward evidence
  • 01:05:42of more reward prediction error if
  • 01:05:45you somehow subtracted what the?
  • 01:05:47Basil Change is an are you missing some
  • 01:05:50of that? Because your tonic salience?
  • 01:05:52I mean, you started out talking a lot about
  • 01:05:55how dopamine could be doing this and that,
  • 01:05:58but it also could be doing all of it.
  • 01:06:01You know it's not mutually exclusive.
  • 01:06:04It could be.
  • 01:06:05It could be let me.
  • 01:06:07I do have a data that answers
  • 01:06:09that question because actually a
  • 01:06:11really astute reviewer asked us.
  • 01:06:13Now I need to find it. It wasn't me.
  • 01:06:17They they they asked us saying,
  • 01:06:18oh, there it is, we did this,
  • 01:06:21so this is actually a really,
  • 01:06:22really good question.
  • 01:06:23Why is this coming up?
  • 01:06:25Oh I'm like not looking at the right file.
  • 01:06:27Maybe if I can get it in like
  • 01:06:302 seconds I'm going to write,
  • 01:06:31But basically we did what you
  • 01:06:33your question is a good one.
  • 01:06:35I think one of the things that they ask,
  • 01:06:37which is a great question.
  • 01:06:39We'd already been thinking about
  • 01:06:40this so we were like, OK, cool was.
  • 01:06:43If these changes in baseline are the
  • 01:06:45reason that we don't see changes.
  • 01:06:47In in a, let's see file new release.
  • 01:06:51All these will be in here now. Insert.
  • 01:06:55Side OK. OK, So what we did here?
  • 01:07:01Is we had that repeated shock
  • 01:07:04experiment and their question
  • 01:07:05was kind of what yours is like.
  • 01:07:07Well, maybe the difference is the baseline,
  • 01:07:09like the baseline is changing overtime
  • 01:07:11and so we calculated the shocks in
  • 01:07:14the original stuff I showed from like
  • 01:07:16a 2 second window before the event
  • 01:07:18and then we went back and calculated
  • 01:07:20all the shocks from a global baseline
  • 01:07:23at the very beginning of each trial.
  • 01:07:25And what we found is that
  • 01:07:27the data is correlated,
  • 01:07:28like very highly correlated.
  • 01:07:29And if we look at the baseline change.
  • 01:07:32Over that trial it is not changing,
  • 01:07:34so it can't explain all of it.
  • 01:07:36Like I understand,
  • 01:07:37I do agree that there is a lot of
  • 01:07:39baseline stuff that's this where we
  • 01:07:41are like kind of taking out here that I
  • 01:07:43think does paint a more complex picture,
  • 01:07:45but I think for a lot of these
  • 01:07:47things that would say OK,
  • 01:07:48this is definitely not our PE.
  • 01:07:50The baseline is not the factor
  • 01:07:52that's driving all of it,
  • 01:07:53but I think that's something actually
  • 01:07:54that people should be thinking
  • 01:07:56about in these papers and everyone
  • 01:07:57does imaging now and they're all
  • 01:07:59doing everything is baseline,
  • 01:08:00But there's like slower changes
  • 01:08:01that are totally left out of that.
  • 01:08:03They're going to change the way
  • 01:08:05you interpret the data.
  • 01:08:06But that's I mean it's a great question.
  • 01:08:08It will look into this more
  • 01:08:09'cause I think maybe it does.
  • 01:08:11All of that is totally reasonable.
  • 01:08:13Explanation and it might be like nice at
  • 01:08:15a different synapses and the same area,
  • 01:08:16and so like how to parse those.
  • 01:08:18I think it's important.
  • 01:08:21Last question from Rick.
  • 01:08:23The outstanding thank you.
  • 01:08:25Very much so this is I'm not
  • 01:08:27sure exactly this question,
  • 01:08:28but since there's so much regulation
  • 01:08:30and release at terminals Strike and dip
  • 01:08:33compared to mean cell body activity,
  • 01:08:34do you think that other neurotransmitters
  • 01:08:36responsible for the specific
  • 01:08:38components of your learning model
  • 01:08:39and like dopamine release the end
  • 01:08:41result and integration of those?
  • 01:08:43And can you speak this one?
  • 01:08:45I can think of right off the bat.
  • 01:08:47So one thing we think is really
  • 01:08:49important for this is acetylcholine
  • 01:08:52regulation of dopamine release.
  • 01:08:54We we have started to do some of this
  • 01:08:56where we're starting on the slice level
  • 01:08:58trying to workout these parameters
  • 01:09:00because it's like calling regulation
  • 01:09:02of domain releases actually really
  • 01:09:04kind of cool regulatory mechanism
  • 01:09:06because it's one of the few that really
  • 01:09:09robustly releases domain from terminals,
  • 01:09:11totally independent of the semantic activity.
  • 01:09:13And you can even get this in
  • 01:09:15isolated terminals and slices.
  • 01:09:17And so we think that there is probably,
  • 01:09:19you know, maybe it's the.
  • 01:09:21Maybe it's both questions.
  • 01:09:23Maybe Domain was released.
  • 01:09:24In response to RP signals,
  • 01:09:26but also attentional signals
  • 01:09:27that are coming from these,
  • 01:09:29maybe like that's the first thing I think
  • 01:09:32of acetylcholine is like attention arousal,
  • 01:09:34but here's the thing.
  • 01:09:36Those are all integrated.
  • 01:09:37What the domain signature finally is,
  • 01:09:39and so I think that it is
  • 01:09:41probably both in some contexts,
  • 01:09:43right?
  • 01:09:44It's probably both acetylcholine
  • 01:09:45regulated domain release and RPE
  • 01:09:47regulated cymatic activity that leads
  • 01:09:49to this kind of saliency based term.
  • 01:09:51One thing I'll tell you some
  • 01:09:53ants about it as we see pretty
  • 01:09:56interesting sex differences.
  • 01:09:57In terminal regulatory
  • 01:09:58mechanisms through the A4 beta,
  • 01:10:00two containing nicotinic receptors
  • 01:10:02that are on those terminals,
  • 01:10:04and So what we're trying to do first
  • 01:10:06is outline the mechanism or how this
  • 01:10:09is different and then take that
  • 01:10:11model into this and say do those
  • 01:10:14differences predict differences
  • 01:10:15in these learning parameters
  • 01:10:16because it's really hard to get it.
  • 01:10:19Don't mean terminal regulation by
  • 01:10:21acetylcholine in vivo because it's
  • 01:10:22your calling regulates other cell
  • 01:10:24types that also regulate dopamine release.
  • 01:10:26So it regulates GABA release.
  • 01:10:28That regulates terminals and so,
  • 01:10:30like without some sort of like biological
  • 01:10:33system that you know is different.
  • 01:10:35It's really hard to isolate just
  • 01:10:37the effects of cetyl choline from
  • 01:10:40the effects of acetylcholine media
  • 01:10:43dopamine terminal regulation.
  • 01:10:44I hope tools come along.
  • 01:10:45There's like darts and stuff
  • 01:10:47which are awesome.
  • 01:10:48Those are coming and like we'll see.
  • 01:10:49But like anyway I'm like this is a that's
  • 01:10:51a great question I'm excited about so.
  • 01:10:55Well thank you were coming up to 11:30
  • 01:10:57and we peppered you with questions
  • 01:10:59we really appreciate the great talk
  • 01:11:01and the time that you spent with us.
  • 01:11:03Thank you to everyone for your attention
  • 01:11:05and we hope to hear more about the
  • 01:11:08next steps in the research. Yeah,
  • 01:11:09thank you guys so much.
  • 01:11:11These questions were awesome man.
  • 01:11:12I had a great time
  • 01:11:14chatting with everybody so
  • 01:11:15thanks. Thanks Erin.