Skip to Main Content

Putting the Mental Back into Mental Disorders

November 18, 2022
  • 00:00Doctor Ledoux his work is
  • 00:02focused on focuses on the brain
  • 00:04mechanisms of memory and emotion.
  • 00:07He is author of several books
  • 00:08including the emotional brain,
  • 00:09the synaptic self, anxious,
  • 00:10and most recently the
  • 00:11deep History of Ourselves,
  • 00:13the 4 billion year story of
  • 00:15how we got conscious brains.
  • 00:16Doctor Ladue has received numerous
  • 00:18awards including the William James Award,
  • 00:20the American Psychological
  • 00:23side from Excuse Me,
  • 00:24the Association for Psychological Science,
  • 00:26the Karl Spencer Lashley Award from the
  • 00:28American Psychological Philosophical Society.
  • 00:30The fission international prize in
  • 00:33cognitive science and many others.
  • 00:35Doctor Do is the fellow of the
  • 00:38American Academy of Arts and Sciences,
  • 00:40the New York Academy of Sciences,
  • 00:41the American Association for
  • 00:43Advancement of Science.
  • 00:44And he is a member of the
  • 00:45National Academy of Sciences,
  • 00:46and he's also a lead singer of the amyloids.
  • 00:51And I think this is the he is
  • 00:53the first actual rock star we've
  • 00:54had give rounds to the center.
  • 00:57So without further ado,
  • 00:58I give you Doctor Ladon.
  • 01:05Thank you very much. Yeah.
  • 01:08So the amygdaloid ads, you know, we.
  • 01:11Play around every now and then,
  • 01:13but during COVID we haven't had many chances,
  • 01:15but hopefully we'll start again.
  • 01:18So today I'm going to talk about what
  • 01:19happened to the middle and middle disorders.
  • 01:24So I'm going to start with kind of giving
  • 01:27you a bookmark of something that happened
  • 01:30and bookmarked this topic by talking about
  • 01:33something that happened in 1917 and 2018.
  • 01:36So in 1917, Freud said there's no question
  • 01:39that the problem with anxiety is a Riddle
  • 01:42whose solution would bound to throw a
  • 01:45floodlight on our whole mental existence.
  • 01:48And in 2018, my colleague Michael
  • 01:51Fanselow and his student Zachary.
  • 01:54Pennington said, returning to an emphasis
  • 01:56on subjective mental experience.
  • 01:58Something I had been arguing for would
  • 02:00put us back well over a century to what
  • 02:03was truly the dark ages of psychiatry.
  • 02:05So that's what I want to talk about,
  • 02:07what's going on in the middle
  • 02:09of these two events.
  • 02:11So here's a very short story,
  • 02:13a version of this story and
  • 02:15of course behaviorist bands,
  • 02:16mental States and psychology.
  • 02:18In the early 20th century,
  • 02:20the mental approach of psychoanalysis
  • 02:22was uninfluenced by all of this,
  • 02:24by behaviorism and thrived in Europe.
  • 02:27And then before and during World War Two,
  • 02:29psychoanalyst mini Jewish fled to
  • 02:31London and the Americas, at the time
  • 02:34the medical model of mental illness,
  • 02:35using medications to treat.
  • 02:37Fighting disease states was emerging to
  • 02:39help psychiatry become more scientific.
  • 02:41That is, less priority and less objective.
  • 02:44But instead of distancing
  • 02:45the feel from Freud,
  • 02:46subjective experience was
  • 02:48generally marginalized.
  • 02:49Some success has resulted from
  • 02:51clinical studies of medications,
  • 02:52but often from incidental findings.
  • 02:55To improve the efficiency of drug discovery,
  • 02:57pharmaceutical companies hired behaviors to
  • 02:59test new medicinal treatments in animals.
  • 03:01The assumption was that mental states
  • 03:03are quaint fictions, and the changing
  • 03:05behavior will make any lingering.
  • 03:07Problem disappear.
  • 03:08And that was the essence
  • 03:11of the fanselow argument.
  • 03:13Now few few efficacious agents
  • 03:16were discovered the after 50
  • 03:19years of disappointing results,
  • 03:20big forma began reducing funding
  • 03:22to find new treatments.
  • 03:24But researchers and universities
  • 03:25equipped with better and better T
  • 03:27techniques like optogenetics and all
  • 03:29sorts of genetic tools persisted in
  • 03:31studies using animal behavior to try
  • 03:33and discover treatments for mental suffering.
  • 03:35Assuming that the magic bullet
  • 03:37bullet was just waiting to be found.
  • 03:39It still doesn't exist in
  • 03:41Mill suffering continues.
  • 03:42So the conclusion of all this
  • 03:44is that people often seek help,
  • 03:46the help of therapists because they
  • 03:48feel bad subjectively drugs based on
  • 03:50animal behavior appropriate for changing
  • 03:52behavioral and physiological symptoms,
  • 03:53but not for treating troubling mental states.
  • 03:56If every change is avoidance
  • 03:58and physiological arousal,
  • 03:59but doesn't help with
  • 04:01the subjective symptoms,
  • 04:02the patient will continue to suffer mentally.
  • 04:04This doesn't mean that Troy was completely
  • 04:06right and behaviors were completely wrong.
  • 04:08Subjective and objective symptoms
  • 04:10must both be treated,
  • 04:12but they depend on.
  • 04:13Different brain circuits and have
  • 04:14to be treated separately now if you
  • 04:16need to go anywhere or leave early,
  • 04:17now would be a time because I've
  • 04:20summarized everything I'm going to say.
  • 04:22But let's get into the details.
  • 04:24So I want to use the science of fear to
  • 04:26illustrate what I see as the problems here.
  • 04:27And I'm not a psychiatrist or a clinician,
  • 04:30so in some ways I have no
  • 04:32business talking about this.
  • 04:33But I'm talking about it from the
  • 04:35perspective of someone who studied
  • 04:36the brain for decades, many decades,
  • 04:38and I'm just going to add my perspective
  • 04:42on what I think might be useful.
  • 04:45Doesn't mean it's completely correct,
  • 04:46but I hope it's useful.
  • 04:48So why is she screaming?
  • 04:51Well, the obvious answer is.
  • 04:56OK, so now everything is out of whack here.
  • 04:59OK, I obviously answered that too.
  • 05:01Why she's screaming is that she's
  • 05:02afraid I have two monitors here.
  • 05:04They're not quite sync, right?
  • 05:07So, but the question is,
  • 05:09is her fear the cause of her screen?
  • 05:12And that is the fundamental
  • 05:14topic I want to address here.
  • 05:15So the main theme of this lecture is
  • 05:18that conflation of subjective experience,
  • 05:20the subjective experience of fear,
  • 05:22with behavioral and physiological correlates
  • 05:24of fear has led to the false premise that
  • 05:27if the objective response are treated,
  • 05:29the mental part will come along for the ride.
  • 05:31So it's a it's a truism, really.
  • 05:34You know that the mental health is first
  • 05:37and foremost about a healthy mind.
  • 05:39If we don't have a healthy mind,
  • 05:40it doesn't matter if our body
  • 05:43is responding properly.
  • 05:44So I want to talk about all this
  • 05:46through my 40 year adventure in
  • 05:48studying emotional consciousness.
  • 05:49And this goes back to my PhD thesis in 1978,
  • 05:53which I studied conscious and unconscious
  • 05:55processing and split brain patients.
  • 05:57So that's me on the left and that's
  • 05:59like the zanaga on the right.
  • 06:01My mentor.
  • 06:03We did a lot of studies
  • 06:06that that were you know,
  • 06:07Mike had been involved in the early
  • 06:09days of Split brain research in the
  • 06:1260s at Caltech with Rogers Barrett and
  • 06:14all the basic kind of findings were
  • 06:16uncovered at that point about you know the.
  • 06:20The If you put information in
  • 06:21the right hemisphere,
  • 06:22the left hand can pull it out of a bag,
  • 06:24but the right can hand can't hand it,
  • 06:27the right can't,
  • 06:27right hand can't pull it out of the bag,
  • 06:30and so on.
  • 06:31But what we wanted to do was ask more
  • 06:34fundamental questions about what does it
  • 06:36mean for the actual living human being,
  • 06:40not just for what are the implications
  • 06:42for understanding split brain patients?
  • 06:44What does it mean for our middle existence
  • 06:47that we can do these dissociations?
  • 06:51Split brain patients.
  • 06:52So we did this one study that
  • 06:54got a lot of a lot of attention.
  • 06:56So the patient,
  • 06:57you know as you probably know
  • 06:59the and split brain patients,
  • 07:01the two hemispheres are separated by
  • 07:04cutting the connections between them
  • 07:06in an effort to relieve intractable
  • 07:09epilepsy and in these patients.
  • 07:12Left the left hemisphere,
  • 07:14typically, as in most people,
  • 07:16has the ability to speak
  • 07:17and comprehend language,
  • 07:18and the right hemisphere has
  • 07:21much more limited capacities.
  • 07:22Doesn't have all of the human equipment,
  • 07:25lacking significant amounts
  • 07:28of language there.
  • 07:30So we had what we did was we put a
  • 07:33stimulus into the left hemisphere.
  • 07:36In other words, the right visual feel,
  • 07:38the chicken claw goes to the left hemisphere.
  • 07:42Left visual field goes to the
  • 07:44right hemisphere.
  • 07:45Just the way the visual system is organized.
  • 07:47If you flash stimulate to the right.
  • 07:50Right it goes to the left hemisphere,
  • 07:52flush to the left it goes to
  • 07:54the right and so.
  • 07:55The left hand immediately
  • 07:56pops out and points to
  • 07:58a shovel in the right hand to the chicken.
  • 08:01So we ask the patient, why did you do that?
  • 08:03Now we're talking to the left
  • 08:05hemisphere because only the left
  • 08:07hemisphere can talk back to us.
  • 08:08So the left Timbers left,
  • 08:10left Hemisphere says. Well, I saw.
  • 08:14I saw chicken claw, so I picked a chicken.
  • 08:17And you need a shovel to
  • 08:19clean out the chicken shed.
  • 08:20So the left hemisphere had no idea why
  • 08:23the left hand had pointed to the shovel,
  • 08:26but it spun a narrative, a tale,
  • 08:29a story that made its behavior makes sense.
  • 08:32So what we concluded from all this was that.
  • 08:35You know, our conscious minds typically
  • 08:38believe that we have free will,
  • 08:40that, you know,
  • 08:40we're in charge of our body and
  • 08:42it's very disturbing if your body
  • 08:44is producing behaviors all the
  • 08:45time that you aren't in charge of.
  • 08:47So in order to protect mental unity,
  • 08:49our idea was that the human brain
  • 08:52developed some kind of capacity
  • 08:54for explaining behavior.
  • 08:56So you, you know,
  • 08:57you say something stupid in a conversation
  • 08:59or in a fight with your spouse,
  • 09:01and you regret it.
  • 09:02And so you, you might at the moment say,
  • 09:04I'm sorry, I should have said that.
  • 09:06Or you might become very defensive
  • 09:08and and try to compensate for it.
  • 09:11But in each case you're getting a new
  • 09:13narrative of the of what you did.
  • 09:16So Mike was a good friend of
  • 09:18Leon Festinger's at the time.
  • 09:20And so we've interpreted all this
  • 09:21in terms of cognitive dissonance,
  • 09:23that it's disturbing.
  • 09:24It's it's dissonance inducing
  • 09:26for your behavior to be produced
  • 09:28in a way that you aren't.
  • 09:29You don't think you're in charge of you,
  • 09:31not that your body did,
  • 09:33but you consciously didn't do and that.
  • 09:36When we spend these narratives,
  • 09:38that's a dissonance reducing
  • 09:40kind of activity, so.
  • 09:46That night we would these these patients
  • 09:48were all at Dartmouth Medical School.
  • 09:50They operate on the Dartmouth and lived up in
  • 09:53the rural areas of Vermont, New Hampshire.
  • 09:55And so we drive up to and then we have
  • 09:59this kind of yellow or pink was orange van
  • 10:02that we pulled a camper trailer behind.
  • 10:05We'd set up a testing lab and and so
  • 10:07we then we go hang out at the bar and
  • 10:09talk about what we had discovered
  • 10:11or or observed during the day.
  • 10:14And we talked about how emotion systems
  • 10:16might be the kinds of systems that
  • 10:19would generate behaviors that demand
  • 10:21these dissonance reducing narratives.
  • 10:24So at that point, Mike said,
  • 10:27you know, there's not much research
  • 10:29on emotion these days.
  • 10:30I said, well, yeah, OK, maybe I'll try that.
  • 10:33And so I decided that, you know, I did,
  • 10:36I wasn't going to have split brain patients
  • 10:38to work with after I left Mikes Lab.
  • 10:40I mean that was his thing, not mine.
  • 10:41And so I had to find something
  • 10:43else to do and I decided that.
  • 10:45But I would work on.
  • 10:47Was the study of.
  • 10:51How these what might be, how these
  • 10:54emotions systems might be generated,
  • 10:56these kinds of behavioral and physiological
  • 10:58responses that might demand this kind
  • 11:01of dissonance reduction activity.
  • 11:03So before I did any research I have
  • 11:05Mike asked me to write a chapter for
  • 11:08this book he was editing and so I wrote
  • 11:11this generated this hypothesis based
  • 11:13on our split brain research about what
  • 11:15how emotion might work in the brain.
  • 11:17So we have an emotional stimulus
  • 11:19goes to a sensory system.
  • 11:21The sensory system feeds into the brain.
  • 11:24It goes to cortical circuits to
  • 11:26generate these conscious feelings
  • 11:28that we have the emotions,
  • 11:29but it goes to subcortical areas to
  • 11:32automatically produce the responses.
  • 11:34Just as in the split brain patient,
  • 11:37you've got these automatic responses
  • 11:39being generated unconsciously.
  • 11:40And then because it's disturbing to you
  • 11:43for these behaviors to be coming out,
  • 11:45you generate a narrative or conscious
  • 11:48interpretation that becomes the
  • 11:49basis of the feeling now.
  • 11:51It was all kind of very poorly
  • 11:53conceived at the time because it was,
  • 11:55you know, I was just making it up.
  • 11:57But this has guided my whole set
  • 12:00of research and and all my books
  • 12:03and so forth over the years.
  • 12:05So the idea was that fear was a
  • 12:08cognitive interpretation or narration,
  • 12:09and the the innate emotional
  • 12:12behaviors and physiological responses
  • 12:14that that come along for the ride
  • 12:17are a different thing altogether.
  • 12:19So I turned to rats to do all of this,
  • 12:22to understand these circuits
  • 12:24that might demand these kinds of
  • 12:26narrative inducing just dissonance,
  • 12:30reducing narratives.
  • 12:32And I used the procedure called
  • 12:33Pavlovian fear conditioning as
  • 12:35a model of emotional behavior.
  • 12:36And I would that's called fear
  • 12:38conditioning because that's been
  • 12:39the assumption of how it works.
  • 12:41I mean,
  • 12:42the behaviors got rid of mental state terms,
  • 12:44sort of mental states,
  • 12:45but they didn't get rid of
  • 12:47mental state terms.
  • 12:48So fear became the relationship
  • 12:50between a stimulus and a response.
  • 12:53It was just a functional relationship.
  • 12:55It had nothing to do with subjectivity
  • 12:58but the rest of the world.
  • 12:59When you call something fear,
  • 13:01thinks you're talking about the
  • 13:03subjective experience of fear.
  • 13:04So that was a bit of a problem.
  • 13:07So but this,
  • 13:08this was an appealing procedure technically,
  • 13:10because you take a simple stimulus
  • 13:13that generates a very reliable
  • 13:16stereotyped response that occurs
  • 13:18in every rat that you tested in,
  • 13:21and it's kind of ideal for tracing
  • 13:24circuits from sensory to motor neurons,
  • 13:26which had been done in invertebrates.
  • 13:29Eric Candell and others were doing that,
  • 13:31but it wasn't really commonly.
  • 13:34Pursued in invertebrates in
  • 13:36mammals because it was thought
  • 13:37that the brain was too complicated.
  • 13:40But you know,
  • 13:40if you have a good stimulus
  • 13:42and a good response,
  • 13:43then you can go a long way
  • 13:45with this kind of stuff.
  • 13:46So I had,
  • 13:47I wrote this NIH NIMH application to study
  • 13:50the Neuropathways of Motion was rejected.
  • 13:53Why?
  • 13:54Because emotion is not
  • 13:56a neuroscientific term,
  • 13:57not neuroscientific topic.
  • 13:59This was 1986,
  • 14:00and it was the behaviors were still very,
  • 14:04very strongly dominating in neuroscience,
  • 14:06as they are today, and in many ways.
  • 14:10So I changed the title to emotional
  • 14:13conditioning and got the brand,
  • 14:14but one of the reviewers said,
  • 14:16well you're studying Pavlovian
  • 14:17conditioning and you don't have
  • 14:19a non associative control group.
  • 14:21So I put the control group in.
  • 14:22That's what it took to get the grant, so.
  • 14:25You know pretty quickly we we were able to
  • 14:29make a lot of progress by we I mean people,
  • 14:33three or four people who were doing this.
  • 14:35Mike Davis who was at Yale at the time
  • 14:37was was there right at the beginning.
  • 14:40I remember this society for neuroscience
  • 14:43meeting in 19 must have been 868586 and
  • 14:47it was me Mike Davis from Yale and Bruce
  • 14:51Cap from the University of Vermont.
  • 14:53And we were the only three people
  • 14:55doing fear conditioning in the brain.
  • 14:57The Society for Neuroscience and for
  • 14:59the for quite some time we were the
  • 15:02main people driving this kind of work.
  • 15:05A bit later fans slow and some
  • 15:07others got involved.
  • 15:08And that the, you know,
  • 15:10within a few years the the core people
  • 15:12who were doing this had generated
  • 15:15a pretty good understanding of how
  • 15:17information gets into the brain,
  • 15:20how it comes out of the brain
  • 15:22and what's in the middle.
  • 15:24And what was in the middle was the amygdala.
  • 15:26It's not like we were.
  • 15:27Looking for the amygdala,
  • 15:29but the stimulus took us there
  • 15:31and the response took us out.
  • 15:33So, you know, so a lot,
  • 15:36a lot was already known in terms of what,
  • 15:38in other words, the what.
  • 15:40Behaviorally,
  • 15:41about all the principles were what
  • 15:43we were able to do was connect
  • 15:45all of the dots in the brain to
  • 15:47put the circuitry together,
  • 15:48and then all these molecule genes
  • 15:50and molecules on the right.
  • 15:52All of them have been identified both
  • 15:55in invertebrates and in vertebrates,
  • 15:58as being important in the plasticity.
  • 16:01So the top part of that is
  • 16:03a presynaptic neuron, say,
  • 16:05coming from the auditory system,
  • 16:06and the bottom part is a postsynaptic
  • 16:09neuron in the amygdala that.
  • 16:10And then then the molecules and the
  • 16:13pre and post synaptic neuron are
  • 16:15being are interacting to generate
  • 16:17the plasticity that will allow the
  • 16:20conditioned stimulus now to get through
  • 16:22the amygdala and produce the responses.
  • 16:25So this all happened,
  • 16:26you know, by the end of.
  • 16:29The 1990s,
  • 16:30all of this was basically known.
  • 16:33So it was like, you know,
  • 16:3410 or 15 years it took to do this,
  • 16:37but that that's a pretty quick
  • 16:39schedule for this kind of work.
  • 16:41So anyway.
  • 16:42I never stopped thinking about and
  • 16:44writing about emotional consciousness.
  • 16:46So in the emotional brain I've
  • 16:48talked about working memory and
  • 16:49emotional consciousness and epic self.
  • 16:51Still on working memory and anxious,
  • 16:53I've kind of shifted to the
  • 16:55global workspace theory.
  • 16:56And then finally in the deep
  • 16:57history of ourselves,
  • 16:58I'd come across the higher order
  • 17:00theory of emotional consciousness.
  • 17:02And that's what I've kind
  • 17:03of adopted and will say
  • 17:04a little bit about towards the end,
  • 17:06if there's time. So.
  • 17:09Plugging the the anatomy that I've
  • 17:12just described to you into the
  • 17:14original model from the early 80s,
  • 17:17what we see is that we've got a,
  • 17:18you know, the,
  • 17:19it's still the cognitive interpretation,
  • 17:21but now I'm adding the prefrontal
  • 17:23cortex into there and freezing.
  • 17:25We can now add the amygdala and
  • 17:27periaqueductal Gray and so forth.
  • 17:29So it, you know, it's useful to have a
  • 17:31kind of conceptual model when you start,
  • 17:33but you have to be willing to throw it away.
  • 17:35But I was fortunate that I didn't
  • 17:36have to throw it away.
  • 17:37It seemed to kind of fill in.
  • 17:39So, but one of the things that I was
  • 17:42explicit about was the difference
  • 17:44between explicit fear and implicit fear.
  • 17:47So I'd always talked about the amygdala
  • 17:49as as being an implicit fear center.
  • 17:52Everybody was talking about
  • 17:53the amygdala fear center.
  • 17:55But I wanted to make it clear that the
  • 17:58amygdala was implicit or unconsciously
  • 18:00detecting and responding to danger,
  • 18:02and that the conscious experience
  • 18:04of fear was the cortical
  • 18:05interpretation of what was going on.
  • 18:07But that distinction.
  • 18:08Did not hold up the way it did in
  • 18:11memory research where it implicit
  • 18:13and explicit is widely accepted.
  • 18:15But you really don't hear a lot about
  • 18:18this distinction in the emotion feel,
  • 18:20especially not in terms of the amygdala
  • 18:23and its so-called role and fear.
  • 18:25So.
  • 18:28This is what I already showed you,
  • 18:29but what I want to emphasize is
  • 18:32this amygdala fear center idea.
  • 18:34I mean the the as I said the the implicit
  • 18:37explicit extinction didn't catch on and.
  • 18:41Everyone was talking about the
  • 18:43amygdala being involved in fear.
  • 18:44I'd be introduced in lectures as
  • 18:46having discovered how feelings of
  • 18:48fear come out of the amygdala,
  • 18:49and that's not what I ever really,
  • 18:51you know, I probably wasn't as clear as I
  • 18:54am today about what I I thought back then,
  • 18:57but because my books were a little
  • 18:58kind of sketchy, sometimes I'd talk
  • 19:00about fear without being clear.
  • 19:03But you know that at some point,
  • 19:06like in 2012 or so, I said, OK,
  • 19:09I really need to make myself.
  • 19:11Clear and say what I'm talking about.
  • 19:14So I wrote this article in Neuron
  • 19:17called rethinking the emotional brain.
  • 19:20But before I get to that,
  • 19:21what's wrong with the amygdala fear center?
  • 19:23Well, first of all,
  • 19:25behavioral physiological responses
  • 19:27don't always correlate strongly
  • 19:28with subjectively experienced fear,
  • 19:31and they should,
  • 19:31if they're all products of fear
  • 19:33in the amygdala, mean Peter Lang,
  • 19:35you know,
  • 19:36involved in anxiety research from
  • 19:38from decades ago in the late 60s,
  • 19:41made this point that the behavioral
  • 19:44and physiological responses don't
  • 19:46always correlate with subjective fear.
  • 19:49And #2 non consciously processed threats,
  • 19:52for example if you subliminally present
  • 19:55stimuli like with visual masking,
  • 19:57or if you're starting a patient
  • 20:00with blind side.
  • 20:02Stimulus will go into the brain
  • 20:03and for your image, the brain,
  • 20:05the amygdala will be activated.
  • 20:07The responses will be generated.
  • 20:10But the person or patient,
  • 20:12depending on the situation,
  • 20:13will not report any fear and not even be
  • 20:17able to tell you what the stimulus was.
  • 20:19So if the amygdala,
  • 20:21if the person is not experiencing
  • 20:23fear but the amygdala,
  • 20:25is active in generating these responses,
  • 20:27fear is not the source of those responses.
  • 20:30It's a separate thing.
  • 20:31And damage to the amygdala can interfere
  • 20:33with the ability of threats to elicit
  • 20:35behavioral and physiological responses,
  • 20:37but doesn't necessarily eliminate
  • 20:39the feeling of fear it should,
  • 20:41if that's where the fear is coming from.
  • 20:43So you know, neuroscientists can explain
  • 20:45fear conditioning in terms of system,
  • 20:47cells, synapses and molecules.
  • 20:49This logic works fine.
  • 20:51And invertebrates like ephesia or
  • 20:53flies and so on. And vertebrates,
  • 20:56so mammals, humans, everybody.
  • 20:58No need to call upon concepts such as fear.
  • 21:02All you need is heavy.
  • 21:03And plasticity like CS is a weak stimulus.
  • 21:06the US is a strong stimulus.
  • 21:08They come.
  • 21:09The two stimuli come together
  • 21:11on neurons in the amygdala.
  • 21:12The strong stimulus causes a change
  • 21:14in the neuron that allows the
  • 21:17weak stimulus to be synaptically
  • 21:19connected with the same neuron,
  • 21:21and this week stimulus can then go
  • 21:23into the brain and more effectively
  • 21:26activate those neurons to produce
  • 21:29the responses.
  • 21:30And if you have a neuromodulator
  • 21:32like norepinephrine being triggered
  • 21:33by the unconditioned stimulus,
  • 21:35the shockers as we know, as in case.
  • 21:39Then that neuromodulator will
  • 21:41potentiate the plasticity and also make
  • 21:44the responses stronger.
  • 21:46So it's not that, you know,
  • 21:48fear is a meaningless concept or construct,
  • 21:51it's just that it's not what's
  • 21:53involved in explaining these kinds
  • 21:55of simple behavioral paradigms.
  • 21:57So it's and that you know, it doesn't,
  • 22:01it doesn't help us to over interpret
  • 22:04what we're talking about or what
  • 22:06we're seeing for the sake of well,
  • 22:10I'll talk about that later.
  • 22:11So I wrote this paper rethinking
  • 22:13the emotional burden.
  • 22:13So the basic idea, this is the,
  • 22:15the classic idea since the stimulus
  • 22:17activates fear in the amygdala
  • 22:19and fear controls the behavior.
  • 22:21And I said, well,
  • 22:22let's get rid of that and instead talk about.
  • 22:25What the amygdala does,
  • 22:26or what's going on in the amygdala
  • 22:28in these situations of danger,
  • 22:29is a defensive survival circuit,
  • 22:32activity set of activities that
  • 22:35the stimulus will activate,
  • 22:37either because it's an innate stimulus
  • 22:39like a snake or something like that,
  • 22:41or condition stimulus.
  • 22:42The survival circuit will then be activated
  • 22:45and behavioral and physiological responses,
  • 22:48defensive responses that are
  • 22:50part of your survival activity,
  • 22:52survival protection,
  • 22:53and the presence of danger.
  • 22:56Will be activated automatically
  • 22:58because these circuits are innate.
  • 23:01And again, it could be activated by an
  • 23:03innate stimulus or condition stimulus,
  • 23:05but the if the responses
  • 23:07are pretty much innate,
  • 23:09so you don't need the actual
  • 23:11subjective experience of fear,
  • 23:12you just need a circuit that can
  • 23:14detect and respond to danger. So.
  • 23:16Why does it matter what we call things?
  • 23:19Well, there are lots of reasons,
  • 23:21and I'll just give you 4
  • 23:23here quotes from four people.
  • 23:25Francis Bacon in 1620 said scientists
  • 23:28should be vigilant and guard against
  • 23:30tacitly granting reality to things.
  • 23:32Simply cook because we have words for them.
  • 23:34George Mandler and William Kesson and a
  • 23:36book called the Language of Psychology.
  • 23:38It's it's a very interesting book.
  • 23:40In 1964 the common language is full
  • 23:42of quasi psychological assertions
  • 23:44and the language in which these
  • 23:46are framed is inadequate.
  • 23:48Jack Block was a personality psychologist
  • 23:50at the end of his career in 1995.
  • 23:52He said psychologists have
  • 23:54tended to be sloppy with words,
  • 23:55summary labels and shorthand ideas
  • 23:57come to control the way we think.
  • 24:00He also said that we should be more
  • 24:02become more familiar with things
  • 24:04denotatively and connotatively
  • 24:06to make ourselves clear,
  • 24:08and not just use kind of concrete
  • 24:11bized words that we've passed
  • 24:13on from generation to generation
  • 24:15and assume that it's simply that
  • 24:17the word has the meaning that it
  • 24:20had 25 or 30 or 100 years ago.
  • 24:22Melvin Marks there's a semantic day in 1951.
  • 24:25Marx was a a behaviorist,
  • 24:27and he's talking about the the behaviorist
  • 24:31tendency to use these mental state terms,
  • 24:34like for fear but not to mean fear.
  • 24:37What they meant was that there's some
  • 24:39kind of intervening variable that was
  • 24:41psychological but not necessarily subjective,
  • 24:44that connected the stimulus and the response,
  • 24:46but they didn't care about
  • 24:47what was in the brain.
  • 24:48But,
  • 24:48Marks pointed out,
  • 24:49there's a semantic danger that results
  • 24:51when a common language.
  • 24:52Term is used as a scientific
  • 24:54name for an intervening variable
  • 24:56or hypothetical construct.
  • 24:58The variable of construct becomes
  • 25:00infected with the subjective
  • 25:02properties of the common meaning.
  • 25:04So in psychology and neuroscience we
  • 25:06have unique conceptual challenges.
  • 25:08So biologists call a family of genes
  • 25:10hedgehog because of some feature
  • 25:11physical features of the gene.
  • 25:13But no one in biology believes
  • 25:15the gene has anything to do
  • 25:16with the animal called Hedgehog.
  • 25:18But when we use a word like fear,
  • 25:20there's assumption is subject
  • 25:21matter is our everyday experience
  • 25:23of fear when in danger.
  • 25:25And so again like Mark said,
  • 25:27the circuits,
  • 25:28the behaviors get infected with the
  • 25:31subjective meaning that the word carries.
  • 25:34So here's some examples of
  • 25:36where we get into trouble.
  • 25:38So David Anderson is one of the most
  • 25:41prominent neurobiologists in the world.
  • 25:42He studies flies,
  • 25:43and he's gotten interested in
  • 25:45in the emotional state of flies.
  • 25:47And he says an emotional state,
  • 25:49possibly analogous to fear and
  • 25:50mammals lies between the threat
  • 25:52and freezing behavior and flies.
  • 25:54By studying flies,
  • 25:55we can learn important things
  • 25:57about human emotions now.
  • 25:59Anderson was not talking about
  • 26:01the subjective experience, but.
  • 26:04Headlines.
  • 26:04Flies have feelings, fear,
  • 26:07and maybe more.
  • 26:08Flies experience emotions like
  • 26:09fear and maybe offer insights
  • 26:11into how the brain makes feelings.
  • 26:13So,
  • 26:13you know.
  • 26:14It's we have to be careful how we
  • 26:17talk about things because if we use,
  • 26:20I'm not saying we should never
  • 26:22use common language terms,
  • 26:23but we have to be clear what we mean by
  • 26:25those terms and not over interpret them.
  • 26:28Because the meaning that you imply
  • 26:30or think you're implying is not
  • 26:33necessarily the one that's going
  • 26:34to go out into the public and these
  • 26:37ideas go into the public and then
  • 26:39actually come back into science
  • 26:41and become part of the science.
  • 26:43So it's a problem. We have to be clear.
  • 26:45About what we mean as as Jack Black said,
  • 26:48Denotatively and Connotatively,
  • 26:50what do these words that we use to
  • 26:53describe brain circuits and behaviors mean?
  • 26:56Here's another one from Louisiana.
  • 26:58So we call these things
  • 27:00crawfish rather than crayfish.
  • 27:01Crawfish treated with a
  • 27:03benzodiazepine with more exploratory,
  • 27:04less inhibited in a chamber in
  • 27:06which they received electric shock.
  • 27:08I mean, did this belong in science?
  • 27:10I don't know.
  • 27:11This is perfectly fine set of findings,
  • 27:13but it's not that, you know,
  • 27:14insightful, but why was it published?
  • 27:17Well,
  • 27:18science had the headline anxious
  • 27:19crawfish can be treated like humans.
  • 27:21York Times,
  • 27:22even crawfish get anxious and BBC
  • 27:24crawfish may experience. Of anxiety.
  • 27:27No more sedate version of that.
  • 27:30So you know a practical example of
  • 27:33why words matter if we consider
  • 27:37anxiety anxiolytic drug development.
  • 27:39So how do you develop a drug?
  • 27:41So I handed it this early.
  • 27:43You take animals and you studied
  • 27:45their defensive behaviors and you
  • 27:48assume that they're going to measure
  • 27:50some brain state of fear or anxiety.
  • 27:53So a drug that's given to.
  • 27:56The animal of the drug company
  • 27:58that alters the defensive behaviors assumed
  • 28:01to change this brain state of fear anxiety
  • 28:03by targeting a fear or anxiety circuit.
  • 28:07And because humans have inherited this
  • 28:08circuit from the million ancestors,
  • 28:10giving the drug to humans to change
  • 28:12the brain state and make people
  • 28:14feel less fearful or anxious.
  • 28:15So in 2010, and we really Class
  • 28:19CEO Glasgow Smith Kline concluded
  • 28:20that the effort has failed and new
  • 28:23investments would not be made because
  • 28:24of the low probability of success.
  • 28:26Andrew Holmes, a leading researcher,
  • 28:28reached a similar conclusion.
  • 28:29These efforts have been disappointing.
  • 28:31It's promising results with novel
  • 28:33agents and rodent studies very rarely
  • 28:35translated into effectiveness in humans.
  • 28:37Eric Nestler and Steve ***** 2 very
  • 28:40prominent biological psychiatrist,
  • 28:42said they echoed the sentiment,
  • 28:44saying that the molecular targets
  • 28:46of current major classes of
  • 28:48psychotherapeutics were all reverse
  • 28:50engineered and animal studies from
  • 28:52drugs discovered incidentally prior
  • 28:54to 1960 by clinical observation.
  • 28:56So nothing new has really been
  • 28:58discovered because the same thing has
  • 29:00been searched for over and over again.
  • 29:02So yeah,
  • 29:03the effort continues because of a commitment
  • 29:05to a poorly conceived set of constructs,
  • 29:08both psychologically and psychiatrically.
  • 29:10So psychologically,
  • 29:12the assumption is that mental states
  • 29:13of fear can be measured by innate or
  • 29:15conditioned behaviors in animals and people.
  • 29:17And you see this not only
  • 29:19in the animal research,
  • 29:20but also in human research where you
  • 29:23show people pictures of, you know,
  • 29:26snakes or other kinds of threatening stimuli,
  • 29:28or you show them faces of aggressive
  • 29:31or fearful.
  • 29:32People.
  • 29:33People with expressing fear or
  • 29:35are aggression in their faces,
  • 29:38and you're assuming that this is
  • 29:40activating a fear center in the
  • 29:42amygdala and that this is going to
  • 29:45be measurable in by using galvanic
  • 29:47skin response or something like that.
  • 29:50But I don't think that's really
  • 29:52going to help.
  • 29:53Psychiatrically,
  • 29:53pathological fear is assumed to
  • 29:55be a product of a malfunctioning
  • 29:57genetically wired fear circuit,
  • 29:58and the delivery of the right chemical to
  • 30:01this first circuit will fix the problem.
  • 30:03So here are two scenarios in social anxiety.
  • 30:06The fear anxiety scenario.
  • 30:08The medical medication will
  • 30:09turn off your fear center.
  • 30:10You'll be able to go to the party
  • 30:13and not feel afraid or anxious.
  • 30:15This is what everyone hopes will happen,
  • 30:17but it's not really what happens.
  • 30:19The physiological symptom scenario,
  • 30:21the medication effects systems that
  • 30:23control behavioral and physiological
  • 30:25responses and challenging situations,
  • 30:27we find it easier to go to the party.
  • 30:28You'll be less avoidant and
  • 30:30less jittery or less aroused.
  • 30:32And while you probably still feel anxious,
  • 30:34I mean,
  • 30:34you've been anxious all your
  • 30:35life and you're probably still
  • 30:36going to be a little anxious.
  • 30:37You'll be better able to cope with
  • 30:39the threatening situation and can
  • 30:40use it as a way to become physically
  • 30:42more comfortable being there.
  • 30:43It's not a cure,
  • 30:44but it might help you cope
  • 30:45with your symptoms.
  • 30:46I mean,
  • 30:46I think you'll have to agree
  • 30:48that the second one is the more
  • 30:50reasonable thing to tell the
  • 30:51patient, because it's more based on what
  • 30:54the research from the animal animals
  • 30:56from the animal lab has really told
  • 30:58us about his behavior and Physiology.
  • 31:01We can't understand the mental states
  • 31:03of humans by studying rat behavior.
  • 31:08So here we go,
  • 31:09back to the defensive survival circuit.
  • 31:11The drugs are developed using these
  • 31:14defensive survival behaviors, right?
  • 31:16So they're based on these survival circuits,
  • 31:20but if people are feeling fearful and
  • 31:24anxious because they're cognitively
  • 31:26interpreting their situations in this way,
  • 31:29then you know, it's not surprising that
  • 31:32the medications are not working as well.
  • 31:34So a typical behaviors account
  • 31:36works fine sub cortically like that,
  • 31:38but we need something more elaborate,
  • 31:41more human like, to understand what's
  • 31:43going on in the middle states of people.
  • 31:48So, you know, critic might say that,
  • 31:50well, some medications used to
  • 31:51treat for anxiety sometimes too
  • 31:53effective some subjective feelings,
  • 31:55for example, benzodiazepines,
  • 31:56they often make people feel a little better.
  • 31:59But we have to ask whether the effects
  • 32:01are truly accounted for by reduction
  • 32:03in fear or anxiety or from up by other
  • 32:05factors that change the feeling somewhat,
  • 32:07but for reason reasons other than
  • 32:09specifically affecting fear anxiety.
  • 32:11If we're going to find new medications,
  • 32:13we've got to, you know,
  • 32:15really understand why a medication works.
  • 32:18Right.
  • 32:18We can't just say, well,
  • 32:20and in general it works.
  • 32:21We have to know something much more specific.
  • 32:24So, you know,
  • 32:25here's some reasons why a benzo might
  • 32:28make someone feel a little bit of
  • 32:30a blunting of emotion in general.
  • 32:31A lot of therapists have told me,
  • 32:33psychiatrist and psychotherapist
  • 32:34have told me that's what they think.
  • 32:36Benzos are basically doing #2 indirect
  • 32:40effects on feelings caused by reducing
  • 32:43feedback from body responses and
  • 32:45physiological arousal in the brain.
  • 32:47So it's, you know, when you.
  • 32:48We're getting all these,
  • 32:50these physiological symptoms turned down.
  • 32:52That will help a bit.
  • 32:53But I think it's kind of like,
  • 32:55you know,
  • 32:55if you go to a bar restaurant
  • 32:57and let's say it's a restaurant
  • 32:59and the music is too loud,
  • 33:01they're playing some awful heavy
  • 33:02metal music and you don't like it,
  • 33:03and you ask the waiter to turn it down,
  • 33:05they turn the volume down on the song,
  • 33:08but it's the same annoying song.
  • 33:10It's just not quite as annoying
  • 33:12because the volume is lowered.
  • 33:14And I think that this can be something that
  • 33:17the medications might be really useful for.
  • 33:19Lowering the volume and then
  • 33:22the the third reason is.
  • 33:24That the effects are due to
  • 33:27altering cognitive processes,
  • 33:28the constructor motions like memory,
  • 33:30attention, working memory,
  • 33:32self processing and so forth.
  • 33:35Then so the the reset GABA receptors
  • 33:37are throughout the brain and every
  • 33:40part of the brain literally.
  • 33:41So you're going to affect a lot of
  • 33:44different circuits and we can't just
  • 33:46assume that there's because it's a
  • 33:49a GABA receptor that binds them to
  • 33:51benzodiazepines that that is only working in.
  • 33:54Fear,
  • 33:55anxiety circuit,
  • 33:55obviously there are lots of side effects.
  • 33:58So that that alone shows that
  • 33:59the stuff is happening all over
  • 34:01the brain and there are cognitive
  • 34:04effects of benzodiazepine.
  • 34:05So you know we have to just kind
  • 34:07of ask what it is that we're
  • 34:10really getting out of these
  • 34:12medications because if we don't then
  • 34:14we can't go forward and that's why you
  • 34:17keep rediscovering benzodiazepines
  • 34:18and and reuptake inhibitors because
  • 34:20that's what you're looking for.
  • 34:24So we need a conceptualization of
  • 34:26fear and anxiety that recognizes
  • 34:28the importance of innate.
  • 34:30Make condition circuits sorry innate
  • 34:32and conditioned circuits that
  • 34:34have been inherited from animals,
  • 34:36but one that doesn't confuse those
  • 34:37circuits with circuits that underlie
  • 34:39conscious feelings of fear or anxiety.
  • 34:41So I want to now switch to
  • 34:43what exactly I think is going
  • 34:46on in emotional consciousness.
  • 34:48So the hypothesis that I've been
  • 34:50working with for a while now is that the
  • 34:53brain mechanisms underlying emotional
  • 34:54states of consciousness are not that
  • 34:56different from those underlying any
  • 34:58other kind of state of awareness.
  • 34:59In other words, we have one.
  • 35:01Kind of emotion,
  • 35:02one kind of consciousness
  • 35:04system in the brain and.
  • 35:06What that system is processing
  • 35:08is what you're conscious of.
  • 35:10So what's different about emotional
  • 35:12and non emotional experiences and
  • 35:14what's different about different
  • 35:16kinds of emotional experiences in
  • 35:19this idea are the inputs processed by
  • 35:22higher cortical cognitive network.
  • 35:23So these would involve,
  • 35:24you know, prefrontal cortex,
  • 35:26parietal cortex,
  • 35:27all these kind of higher cognitive circuits.
  • 35:32So rather than having different
  • 35:34subcortical circuits for different
  • 35:35emotions as in basic emotions theory,
  • 35:37which is, you know,
  • 35:38all these different emotion circuits
  • 35:40like the amygdala does this,
  • 35:41the periaqueductal Greg does that.
  • 35:44Hypothalamus does this.
  • 35:46This higher order circuit idea is that
  • 35:48we have these higher order cognitive
  • 35:51representations of redescription or
  • 35:53indexes or whatever you want to call them,
  • 35:55that account for emotional and non
  • 35:58emotional experiences in one system.
  • 36:00So the idea is that the information
  • 36:02being processed by this higher order
  • 36:04circuit is different in emotional and
  • 36:05non emotional situations and different
  • 36:07and different kinds of emotional situations.
  • 36:10So I don't believe in basic emotions.
  • 36:12For me all emotions are
  • 36:14cognitive interpretations.
  • 36:15I'll explain that more in a minute.
  • 36:18So here's, you know,
  • 36:19basic idea.
  • 36:19You've got water, onions,
  • 36:21garlic, celery, salt,
  • 36:22pepper through all that stuff into a pot.
  • 36:25And none of these are super ingredients,
  • 36:28right?
  • 36:28But somehow if you now throw some chicken in,
  • 36:31it becomes chicken soup.
  • 36:32And again,
  • 36:33chicken is not a soup ingredient either.
  • 36:35None of these things exist for the
  • 36:37purpose of making soup with soup.
  • 36:39Flavor of the Super emerges from all this,
  • 36:42and I think we can say the
  • 36:43same thing about making fear.
  • 36:45It's there are No Fear ingredients
  • 36:47in the brain.
  • 36:48Fear is the cognitive interpretation that
  • 36:50is based on all these kinds of processes
  • 36:53that that are generic processes really.
  • 36:58So let's put it in a different picture.
  • 37:00So.
  • 37:01The blue part there is basically
  • 37:05working memory.
  • 37:07And the idea is that within working
  • 37:09memory we assemble a mental model of
  • 37:12fear or love or any other emotion
  • 37:14that we're going to experience.
  • 37:16And we do this by monitoring, appraising,
  • 37:19attending to all of these inputs.
  • 37:22This allows us to have
  • 37:23metacognition is about,
  • 37:24in other words,
  • 37:25thoughts about what's going on.
  • 37:27We can introspect about it,
  • 37:28give verbal reports and control,
  • 37:30goal directed.
  • 37:31Behavior through this mental model.
  • 37:33So what are the ingredients
  • 37:34that go into that?
  • 37:35Well, start over here on the left,
  • 37:38like 9:00 o'clock we have sensory processing
  • 37:41with sensory processing by itself is
  • 37:43meaningless until you have some memory.
  • 37:46You don't come into the world
  • 37:47knowing what apples, chairs,
  • 37:48light bulbs and so forth are.
  • 37:52You have to add semantic memory
  • 37:54to sensory processing in order to
  • 37:56make that a meaningful perception.
  • 37:58And so semantic memory is a key factor that.
  • 38:01Makes sensory processing go beyond just what
  • 38:04the stimulus is brought into the brain is.
  • 38:07So, but we can also have memories of
  • 38:09the past episodes of our life in which
  • 38:12those semantic situations have occurred,
  • 38:16and this allows us to bring
  • 38:18ourself into the experience.
  • 38:20Now all of these kinds of ingredients are.
  • 38:26Could be part of any kind of situation,
  • 38:28any kind of conscious occurs.
  • 38:30There's nothing to do with the motion.
  • 38:32When we go to the other side of the the,
  • 38:35the emotion plot there and again we
  • 38:38can start it at 3:00 o'clock and go up.
  • 38:41So one of the things that often happens is,
  • 38:44let's say in the case of fear,
  • 38:45we have a survival circuit that's activated.
  • 38:48This is going to produce behavioral
  • 38:51responses that will produce body
  • 38:53signals that feedback to the brain.
  • 38:56It will.
  • 38:58The survival circuit will also
  • 39:00activate brain arousal circuits
  • 39:02that make brain hyper aroused.
  • 39:05But an important thing is the emotion scheme.
  • 39:08Emotion schema are bodies of knowledge
  • 39:11that you've acquired about emotions.
  • 39:14We, you know, we have emotion labels.
  • 39:16We learn about emotion and
  • 39:17we assign words to them.
  • 39:19You know,
  • 39:20you don't have to have the
  • 39:22word to have an emotion,
  • 39:23but you have to have the word to
  • 39:26have the emotion named by the word.
  • 39:28I can say that again if it's not clear, but.
  • 39:30Ask me again if it's not clear.
  • 39:34So schema are these bodies of
  • 39:36knowledge that are particular to
  • 39:38you because you are the person who
  • 39:41have learned what danger is to you,
  • 39:43what fear is to you,
  • 39:45what it what you how you want to
  • 39:47respond when you're in danger,
  • 39:48how you're expected to respond,
  • 39:51how other people respond,
  • 39:52and so on.
  • 39:53It's a catalog of things that
  • 39:56can be activated by a simple
  • 39:58stimulus of snake on the ground
  • 40:00or a mugger coming towards you.
  • 40:03Anything that.
  • 40:04It's a classic elicitor of fear is
  • 40:07going to activate enough information
  • 40:09about the fear schema to unpack
  • 40:12it to basically pattern complete
  • 40:14the experience of fear and as.
  • 40:17And this is not something that just happens,
  • 40:20it's slowly evolves and microseconds or
  • 40:23milliseconds as the episode unfolds so
  • 40:25you find yourself in front of a snake.
  • 40:28You know that snakes are reptiles,
  • 40:30but that's not where your mind goes.
  • 40:31Your mind goes to that.
  • 40:32Snakes are dangerous and they
  • 40:34can bite you and kill you.
  • 40:36So as soon as you are now you've
  • 40:39you pattern completed fear.
  • 40:42But now you start to worry
  • 40:44about what that thing
  • 40:45is going to do to you and what might be the
  • 40:48consequences of being bitten by a snake.
  • 40:50Will you be able to find a doctor?
  • 40:52Will they have the anecdote? And so on.
  • 40:54What will happen to your family if you die?
  • 40:56So fear instantly.
  • 40:58Dwarfs and anxiety.
  • 41:00And then you can go the other way as well.
  • 41:02So if you're anxious about something,
  • 41:04you start to see triggers of fear
  • 41:07all over the environment, right?
  • 41:09Particular stimuli now will trigger
  • 41:11you to be afraid of that stimulus,
  • 41:14because you have activated your brain in
  • 41:16a way that makes those things stand up.
  • 41:20So fear and anxiety kind of
  • 41:21both morph into each other.
  • 41:25But all of this is very particular to you,
  • 41:28because it's your brain that is picking
  • 41:29all this up out of the environment.
  • 41:31Things that are dangerous to me,
  • 41:33you're not dangerous to use, and so on.
  • 41:36So. Umm. All right, now,
  • 41:41one of the reasons we need this kind of view
  • 41:45of fear and emotion in general is because.
  • 41:48You know the the whole idea of the
  • 41:50amygdala fear center is based on this
  • 41:52I they basically what the amygdala is,
  • 41:55is to the extent that it's involved in
  • 41:57danger, it's a predatory defense circuit.
  • 42:00It evolved for mammals to
  • 42:02deal with predators. And.
  • 42:06But not everything that we are afraid of
  • 42:09or anxious about starts with the predator.
  • 42:12I mean, we can have all kinds
  • 42:13of fears and anxiety.
  • 42:15So you're stuck on a mountain top
  • 42:17and you've dropped your backpack
  • 42:18off the Cliff accidentally.
  • 42:20So you know you start to be afraid or anxious
  • 42:24about the fact that you don't have shelter,
  • 42:28food, and water.
  • 42:29So you're afraid that you might die of
  • 42:32starvation or dehydration or hypothermia.
  • 42:35You could be.
  • 42:36Afraid of something that
  • 42:37you hear in a conversation,
  • 42:39or anxious about something you hear
  • 42:42someone incidentally say about you that
  • 42:44they didn't realize you were there.
  • 42:45We we can have fear and anxiety from
  • 42:48endless number of reasons in life.
  • 42:51So it has something sometimes to do
  • 42:53with this predatory defense circuit.
  • 42:55But it's a predatory defense
  • 42:57circuit is not the answer.
  • 42:59And that's why it's it's really all about
  • 43:02the cognitive interpretation of the
  • 43:03situation because that is the the what.
  • 43:06You are your what your brain is
  • 43:08concluding as a result of this pattern
  • 43:11completion of stuff that you've
  • 43:13experienced in the past and giving you
  • 43:16a perspective on what's happening now.
  • 43:18So here's the conceptual hypothesis.
  • 43:21So you have a physical or social
  • 43:23threat in the environment.
  • 43:25It goes into a sensory system.
  • 43:28This is all non conscious processing.
  • 43:30Sensory system can contribute
  • 43:32to the mental model there if you
  • 43:35just go straight through.
  • 43:36But it also activates the threat.
  • 43:38The threat detector generates
  • 43:40behavioral and physiological responses
  • 43:42that contribute to the mental model
  • 43:44and the kind of body feedback way,
  • 43:46but also the brain arousal.
  • 43:49The threat detector will contribute
  • 43:50to the mental model.
  • 43:52But the century stimulus becomes meaningful
  • 43:54when it's combined with memory on top.
  • 43:57There sensory memory
  • 43:59interactions create perceptions,
  • 44:01perceptions and memory combine in
  • 44:04schema to activate to pattern.
  • 44:07Complete the schema and give you
  • 44:09a template for your mental model.
  • 44:11Now your mental model of the
  • 44:14experience is preconscious.
  • 44:16So we've got this completely unconscious
  • 44:18processing then some pre conscious
  • 44:20processing because everything.
  • 44:22Every conscious state.
  • 44:24Is preconscious before it's conscious, right?
  • 44:27Because you don't just.
  • 44:29Have a conscious state.
  • 44:31There's a lot of cognitive processing
  • 44:33that underlies each conscious experience.
  • 44:35So the mental model is pre conscious now.
  • 44:39Here's my we're going back to
  • 44:41the split brain now because.
  • 44:44This is where I started,
  • 44:45that there's some kind of narration
  • 44:48that is generated unconsciously
  • 44:50that then explains what's going on.
  • 44:53So the idea is that we have
  • 44:55this pre conscious narration
  • 44:57coming out of the middle model.
  • 44:59That underlies the conscious experience,
  • 45:02but there's some details that
  • 45:05that's often not understood.
  • 45:07And this is just stuff that I've kind
  • 45:09of uncovered myself in the process of
  • 45:12writing this new book that I just finished.
  • 45:15But the idea is that.
  • 45:17The preconscious narrative.
  • 45:19Can control your behavior,
  • 45:21your goal directed behavior,
  • 45:23and allow you to give a verbal report, but.
  • 45:26Those two things are going to
  • 45:29involve separate neuropathways,
  • 45:31verbal report coming out of language,
  • 45:34areas of the brain,
  • 45:35our goal directed behavior coming
  • 45:37out of straight and so forth.
  • 45:40So you're going to have additional
  • 45:42processing in those circuits that are
  • 45:44going to make the verbal report not
  • 45:46necessarily completely overlapping
  • 45:47with the goal directed behavior.
  • 45:49So you might.
  • 45:50You know, say things that are
  • 45:52different from what you do,
  • 45:53or the things you do are different
  • 45:55from what you say somewhat.
  • 45:56They can overlap,
  • 45:57but they can also diverge,
  • 45:59but there's more because.
  • 46:02Once you're conscious of all this,
  • 46:05the narration is giving you
  • 46:08something to interpret.
  • 46:09You can now.
  • 46:12Also give a verbal report and
  • 46:15control behavior consciously.
  • 46:18So it's not that all of this stuff
  • 46:20is just unconscious.
  • 46:21Once you're conscious,
  • 46:22that opens up a whole nother level
  • 46:24of decision making and control.
  • 46:26So I think one of the reasons that
  • 46:29consciousness is so hard to to
  • 46:31study and understand that because
  • 46:33we never know in an experiment.
  • 46:36Whether the subject is reporting.
  • 46:40Nonverbally when you ask for a
  • 46:42verbal report or are consciously
  • 46:44reporting non consciously or
  • 46:46consciously or whether their
  • 46:48behavior that they choose to perform.
  • 46:50Is gold directed and being directed
  • 46:53unconsciously or consciously?
  • 46:55So I think this, you know,
  • 46:58if you could say, well,
  • 47:00that really complicates things.
  • 47:01It does.
  • 47:02But if you know what the problem,
  • 47:03if you know what the complications are,
  • 47:05they can become a feature rather
  • 47:07than an impediment to understanding.
  • 47:12So this is this is the puts it what I just
  • 47:17said into a slightly different framework but.
  • 47:21On the bottom right there you have a
  • 47:23bunch of different kinds of systems
  • 47:26that contribute to the mental,
  • 47:28the unconscious mental model.
  • 47:30They have sensory, perceptual information,
  • 47:32memory information, goal information,
  • 47:34homeostatic information and
  • 47:35and lots of other things.
  • 47:38So these things are all coming into the
  • 47:40mental model unconsciously and they
  • 47:42are the basis of this non conscious
  • 47:45mental model which spawns 3 tributaries.
  • 47:48One is the tributary of
  • 47:50Goal directed behavior.
  • 47:52One is the tributary of speech
  • 47:54and writing and the last one is
  • 47:57the tributary of Consciousness.
  • 47:59So the tributary consciousness
  • 48:00creates the non sorry.
  • 48:02The tribute of conscious predates
  • 48:04the conscious mental model.
  • 48:05So it's a second mental model that you have.
  • 48:08And once you have that model then
  • 48:11you can also control speech and
  • 48:13writing and goal directed behavior.
  • 48:16So it's just restating what I said
  • 48:18but in a different way graphically.
  • 48:21OK so I just want to briefly
  • 48:24mention see what time of this.
  • 48:26OK,
  • 48:26just want to briefly mention the
  • 48:29higher order theory of consciousness
  • 48:30because I find this a very useful
  • 48:33way for thinking about all of this.
  • 48:35So the traditional view of
  • 48:38higher order theory is.
  • 48:41And most most research
  • 48:42on consciousness in fact,
  • 48:43is about sensory cortex and dorsolateral
  • 48:47prefrontal cortex that is the the main.
  • 48:52At the main kind of interaction
  • 48:54that is often talked about,
  • 48:56that's because a lot of the research
  • 48:59involves sensory processing and so forth.
  • 49:01But I think that the higher order
  • 49:04theory can really benefit from a
  • 49:07more elaborate anatomical framework
  • 49:09because rather than asking how
  • 49:11does a a lower order state.
  • 49:14Become conscious.
  • 49:14What I tried to do with what I'm about
  • 49:18to show you is assume that prefrontal
  • 49:21cortex has something to do with all this.
  • 49:25And work backwards to what are the
  • 49:27inputs to these prefrontal circuits
  • 49:29that might shape our our understanding
  • 49:32of of what those circuits do.
  • 49:35So this is the framework that I
  • 49:37like to work with. So we have.
  • 49:43And you see my cursor on this. Yeah. OK.
  • 49:45So from higher order it what prefrontal
  • 49:48cortex has granular components and what
  • 49:51we'll call sub granular components.
  • 49:53So let's look at the bottom here.
  • 49:55So here's the the granular prefrontal
  • 49:57cortex and I think this now my hypothesis
  • 50:01is this is responsible for the middle model,
  • 50:04but the schematic information is coming
  • 50:08from these sub granular areas like
  • 50:11the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
  • 50:13The anterior cingulate are the what?
  • 50:16The more general medial prefrontal cortex,
  • 50:18orbital frontal cortex.
  • 50:20These are our areas that connect with
  • 50:23memory systems and allow semantic and
  • 50:26episodic memory to generate schema and
  • 50:29the schema then become the basis for
  • 50:31the granular prefrontal mental model.
  • 50:35So if we work backwards from here,
  • 50:37we see that.
  • 50:38That the representation on top is
  • 50:42very sparse in terms of explaining
  • 50:45complex mental states.
  • 50:47And in general, you know,
  • 50:48all this research on consciousness
  • 50:51in the perceptual system is highly
  • 50:54limiting because what it focuses on
  • 50:57is like dots or lines on a screen,
  • 51:00and whether you perceive them or
  • 51:02can you see this or not,
  • 51:04but our life experiences are
  • 51:05all multimodal and we have to.
  • 51:07Always we are.
  • 51:08Always integrating what is there in
  • 51:10the world with our perceptions and our
  • 51:13past memories and our understanding
  • 51:15of the world and our schema.
  • 51:17Those are the mechanisms that interpret
  • 51:19who we are and what's going on in our lives.
  • 51:22And we we can't understand that
  • 51:25from a simple visual cortex and
  • 51:27prefrontal cortex framework.
  • 51:29But I think we can go pretty far with
  • 51:32the with this as a starting point
  • 51:34where we just begin to appreciate
  • 51:36the complexity of the way the
  • 51:38brain puts all this together.
  • 51:40So these are just illustrating the
  • 51:44the granular prefrontal cortex in
  • 51:46the in the lateral cortex there.
  • 51:49So dorsolateral Pat Goldman,
  • 51:51Rakesh put that on the map in terms
  • 51:54of working memory and so forth.
  • 51:56We have ventral ventral lateral paths lab,
  • 52:00Liz Romanski,
  • 52:01who was my graduate student or
  • 52:03postdoc with Pat, she and Pat.
  • 52:05Put ventrolateral on the map
  • 52:07as being important for auditory
  • 52:09working memory and so forth.
  • 52:12Then we have the arbiter frontal cortex,
  • 52:14lateral orbital frontal cortex,
  • 52:15but I think one of the most important
  • 52:19and under underappreciated structures
  • 52:21is the frontal pole and I'll tell
  • 52:23you why that's important in a minute.
  • 52:25And then on the medial side we have
  • 52:27some granular cortex in the dorsal
  • 52:29medial and the medial frontal pole
  • 52:32and then the lighter Gray areas
  • 52:33are all the sub granular areas
  • 52:36like anterior cingulate.
  • 52:37Prelimbic our medial orbital and
  • 52:40ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
  • 52:42Now the reason these are important
  • 52:44to distinguish is that Umm,
  • 52:46the granular cortex has an
  • 52:48architecture that allows human
  • 52:50cognition to unfold the way it does,
  • 52:53whereas the sub granular cortex is
  • 52:56less complex in that and doesn't
  • 53:00have that particular architecture.
  • 53:02So lateral prefrontal cortex is involved
  • 53:06in working memory cognitive control.
  • 53:08However,
  • 53:08their representations higher order indexing,
  • 53:10global broadcasting and so
  • 53:12forth and and the medial areas
  • 53:14are in memory integration,
  • 53:16schema assembly, self processing,
  • 53:19default mode processing.
  • 53:21But we need more recognition of the the
  • 53:24distinction than than this often used
  • 53:27at least in the consciousness view.
  • 53:29And this is a a kind of
  • 53:30way to think about it,
  • 53:32that you have these lower order
  • 53:35states that have recurrent
  • 53:37networks between all of their.
  • 53:38Various interconnections,
  • 53:40but there are also recurrent
  • 53:42interconnections between the the sub
  • 53:45granular prefrontal and the bottom
  • 53:46and the granular on top and between
  • 53:49the various components of granular
  • 53:50and sub granular and so forth.
  • 53:52So there's a lot that's going on that
  • 53:55if we understand the anatomy better
  • 53:57and build it into the way we talk
  • 54:00about all these things more thoroughly,
  • 54:02I think we would make more
  • 54:04progress on all this.
  • 54:05So let me just close with
  • 54:08emphasized by emphasizing.
  • 54:09What's going on here?
  • 54:11So let's start with the the
  • 54:13light Gray on the right side.
  • 54:16Area, so anterior, cingulate, medial,
  • 54:18prefrontal, that's prelimbic,
  • 54:20ventromedial and so forth.
  • 54:22Media, larval,
  • 54:23these light Gray areas are
  • 54:25all the sub granular areas.
  • 54:27The reason that's important is because
  • 54:29all mammals have those areas, so.
  • 54:33The darker Gray areas are
  • 54:35all primate unique areas.
  • 54:37So you don't have a dorsal medial or medial,
  • 54:40medial, frontal pole or lateral frontal pole,
  • 54:43dorsal lateral or ventral lateral in
  • 54:45rats and and other non primate mammals,
  • 54:48but you have them in in primates.
  • 54:51And in the human brain we have an area,
  • 54:53the a component of the lateral frontal pole,
  • 54:56that is only present in the human brain.
  • 54:58So.
  • 55:00Let's just for the sake of argument,
  • 55:02say that.
  • 55:04The human unique.
  • 55:06Frontal pole has an important role in
  • 55:09human unique kinds of consciousness just
  • 55:12for the sake of argument and that we.
  • 55:16The primate unique areas of the the.
  • 55:20Primate brain.
  • 55:21Are going to be the responsible
  • 55:24for kinds of consciousness that
  • 55:26are shared by all primates,
  • 55:27including humans and including
  • 55:29monkeys and chimps and so forth.
  • 55:32And the Gray,
  • 55:33light Gray areas,
  • 55:35sub granular areas are going to be
  • 55:37the kinds of consciousness that might
  • 55:40be shared throughout all mammals.
  • 55:43So even if we only,
  • 55:45let's just start with the the sub granular.
  • 55:49So there's a lot of concern
  • 55:51about how do we get from human,
  • 55:53how do we get from animal
  • 55:55consciousness to human consciousness?
  • 55:56Well,
  • 55:56if these sub granular areas are
  • 55:59involved in a kind of consciousness
  • 56:01in mammals and all mammals,
  • 56:04then we have a foundation for saying
  • 56:07how did the dorsolateral ventrolateral
  • 56:10evolve in primates from these.
  • 56:14Other kinds of areas that are shared
  • 56:17by all mammals and how did the human?
  • 56:21Prefrontal cortex evolve from the
  • 56:24monkey and other primate prefrontal cortex.
  • 56:27So it's a framework for kind
  • 56:29of starting with what we know.
  • 56:30The idea is that if we knew more
  • 56:32about what the human specific
  • 56:34part of the frontal poll does
  • 56:35for human consciousness.
  • 56:37What the?
  • 56:40What the primate kinds of areas,
  • 56:43dorsolateral, ventrolateral,
  • 56:44and so forth, do for human consciousness,
  • 56:47and what the medial prefrontal
  • 56:49areas do for human consciousness.
  • 56:50We could then try to reverse engineer
  • 56:53what's going on in the brains of other
  • 56:56primates from the areas that we share
  • 56:58with primates and what's going on in the
  • 57:00brains of other mammals on the basis of
  • 57:02what we share with other mammals because.
  • 57:06You know, basically just study consciousness.
  • 57:09Verbal report is almost essential.
  • 57:12Because it's not that because language
  • 57:15is necessary for consciousness,
  • 57:17but it provides an important
  • 57:20leverage point so I can respond.
  • 57:24To something that I'm conscious of
  • 57:26verbally by telling you what's there,
  • 57:28or by pointing.
  • 57:30But if I'm unconscious of something,
  • 57:33I can only respond nonverbally,
  • 57:36pointing or other things.
  • 57:39So in humans, we have verbal report
  • 57:43to separate to give us a clue.
  • 57:45When something is conscious
  • 57:46doesn't mean it's perfect,
  • 57:48but it gives us a kind of way to
  • 57:51we can't talk about things that
  • 57:53we aren't conscious of, right?
  • 57:55So I mean, we can.
  • 57:59Pull things out and so forth,
  • 58:00but I don't want to get into that.
  • 58:03Just want to keep it simple and say that.
  • 58:05That in an experiment.
  • 58:07When you are able to verbal
  • 58:09report about something,
  • 58:11that is the best evidence that the person is
  • 58:15conscious of what they are are experiencing.
  • 58:18So in animals having only
  • 58:21nonverbal responses,
  • 58:22we don't have that way to leverage it.
  • 58:25So I think instead of like trying to to use
  • 58:28simple behavioral responses like other words,
  • 58:31extrapolation from analogy with
  • 58:33human behavior to try to understand
  • 58:37what's going on when a rat freezes.
  • 58:39We we say,
  • 58:40OK,
  • 58:40it's afraid because we're afraid
  • 58:42when we freeze.
  • 58:43But this extrapolation from from human
  • 58:46behavior I think gets us into trouble.
  • 58:49We need a different way to approach it.
  • 58:51I think this anatomical approach of
  • 58:53reverse engineering based on what
  • 58:55we learn about human consciousness
  • 58:57might tell us more about what might
  • 58:59be going on in in other mammals.
  • 59:01OK, So what is an emotion?
  • 59:02It's a mental model based,
  • 59:03narrative driven,
  • 59:05culturally shaped,
  • 59:06subjective experience and biologically or
  • 59:09psychologically significant situation.
  • 59:11All humans must deal with danger,
  • 59:13and all cultures have words for
  • 59:15danger and words for experiences
  • 59:16that occur when in danger.
  • 59:18But emotional experiences
  • 59:19vary across culture.
  • 59:21Because we can translate words
  • 59:22like fear across cultures,
  • 59:23we assume that people in these cultures have
  • 59:25the same basic experience named by the word.
  • 59:28But cultural differences and experiences are
  • 59:30based on differences in the schema involved,
  • 59:32that is the schema or personal.
  • 59:34So the different people within
  • 59:36a culture have different schema
  • 59:37and hence different experiences
  • 59:39and people in different cultures.
  • 59:41We also have different schema
  • 59:43and different experiences.
  • 59:44Other animals,
  • 59:45lacking our specific kind of
  • 59:46cognition and brain,
  • 59:47can't have our kind of schema
  • 59:49and our kind of fear.
  • 59:50But they may have their own emotions
  • 59:52made possible by their own brains.
  • 59:54So she's screaming and she's
  • 59:56afraid that these are not
  • 59:59the same. So what happened
  • 01:00:01between 1917 and 2018?
  • 01:00:03The effort to put fraud in the
  • 01:00:05rearview mirror? Psychology,
  • 01:00:06psychiatry and brain science through
  • 01:00:08the middle baby out with the bathwater.
  • 01:00:10Until we accept that emotions are
  • 01:00:12first and foremost mental states,
  • 01:00:13efforts to treat mental
  • 01:00:15disorders will languish.
  • 01:00:16Patients, therapists,
  • 01:00:17pharmaceutical companies,
  • 01:00:18and researchers will all
  • 01:00:20continue to be disappointed.
  • 01:00:21So some of this is from anxious
  • 01:00:23and the deep history of ourselves.
  • 01:00:25And I have a bunch of fear
  • 01:00:27rants that you can look up,
  • 01:00:29and a bunch of clinical rants and
  • 01:00:32some consciousness partition rents
  • 01:00:33I don't have time to talk about.
  • 01:00:35Thank you very much.
  • 01:00:43Thank you Doctor Lulu for an amazing talk.
  • 01:00:46I'm really blown away by by the
  • 01:00:49breath of your work we're at.
  • 01:00:50We're out of time now.
  • 01:00:51If people would like to stay for questions,
  • 01:00:54you can. If you need to go,
  • 01:00:56please feel free to go.
  • 01:00:56I apologize for using all the time.
  • 01:01:00Thank you. Any questions?
  • 01:01:03Come on, you got some Georgia.
  • 01:01:07I guess I would think
  • 01:01:08there are some thanks.
  • 01:01:11I I would say there's some anyway
  • 01:01:14that would emphasize interoception
  • 01:01:15more than you appear to.
  • 01:01:18And I just wonder what you
  • 01:01:19would say to them or what
  • 01:01:21your thoughts are about that.
  • 01:01:23You know, I said the body
  • 01:01:25responses are very important,
  • 01:01:27but I don't I think that until those
  • 01:01:30responses are cognitively interpreted.
  • 01:01:32They're not giving you an experience,
  • 01:01:35but the experience requires the
  • 01:01:37interpretation of signals whether
  • 01:01:38they're external signals or
  • 01:01:40internal signals and that's what
  • 01:01:41the these cognitive networks are
  • 01:01:43important for that you know you
  • 01:01:45and intraception is going to come
  • 01:01:47into those come into the amygdala,
  • 01:01:49hypothalamus,
  • 01:01:50their aqueductal Gray and then they
  • 01:01:51will go into the medial prefrontal
  • 01:01:53areas that I was just talking about.
  • 01:01:55So those medial prefrontal areas are
  • 01:01:58providing kind of and I didn't have
  • 01:02:00time to go into the last part which was.
  • 01:02:02The partition of consciousness
  • 01:02:04into three kinds of states that
  • 01:02:06entail traveling talks about.
  • 01:02:08One is autonoetic consciousness,
  • 01:02:10which is reflective self-awareness.
  • 01:02:12Basically it's based on episodic memory of
  • 01:02:16who you are and what you know about yourself.
  • 01:02:18No edit consciousness,
  • 01:02:19which is based on semantic memory and the
  • 01:02:22conceptual knowledge and semantic knowledge,
  • 01:02:24and then a noetic consciousness,
  • 01:02:26which is more of a kind of William
  • 01:02:29James Fringe state that is simply there.
  • 01:02:32You know,
  • 01:02:33you know your body states are yours without
  • 01:02:36having to affirm that they're your,
  • 01:02:38that your mental state to yours
  • 01:02:39without having their permit.
  • 01:02:40But there are some people who have
  • 01:02:42brain damage that lose the ability to
  • 01:02:45know their mental states of theirs or
  • 01:02:47to know their body states of theirs.
  • 01:02:49And that's when it becomes apparent
  • 01:02:50that we always know that our you know,
  • 01:02:53unless your brain is is damaged
  • 01:02:55in this way you know that your
  • 01:02:57mental states are yours.
  • 01:02:58So the idea is that when
  • 01:03:01you have some kind of.
  • 01:03:03Violation of that normality,
  • 01:03:05what William James called the
  • 01:03:07rightness of mental states.
  • 01:03:09Then that's when you have to go
  • 01:03:11into the more cognitive levels of
  • 01:03:13consciousness to interpret them.
  • 01:03:16But otherwise you're interoception
  • 01:03:17is going to be one of those kinds of
  • 01:03:21analytic States and medial prefrontal
  • 01:03:23cortex and unless it becomes.
  • 01:03:25For the sake of you know,
  • 01:03:26if you have an intense body state,
  • 01:03:29then it's not going to stay in the
  • 01:03:32normal level of analytic consciousness.
  • 01:03:33It's going to rise to have the
  • 01:03:36semantic interpretation or even
  • 01:03:38autonoetic interpretation of being
  • 01:03:40something that you are personally
  • 01:03:42involved with and conscious of.
  • 01:03:44That's my two cents.
  • 01:03:47Any more questions?
  • 01:03:50Heartland.
  • 01:03:56That was awesome and I turned around
  • 01:03:58so happy to see two of my postdocs in
  • 01:04:00the back because I always talk to them
  • 01:04:02about how I think theory is so important.
  • 01:04:04It's really fun to see the way you
  • 01:04:06have evolved your own theories over
  • 01:04:08time as and it's a mechanism to
  • 01:04:09drive I think one's own work forward
  • 01:04:11and a whole field forward.
  • 01:04:13So thank you for that and I guess
  • 01:04:14my question is you observe verbal.
  • 01:04:16You said two things that struck me.
  • 01:04:18One you tripped consciousness is is
  • 01:04:20contingent upon brain mechanisms
  • 01:04:21like everything else.
  • 01:04:23Two you said that verbal report.
  • 01:04:25Is a you
  • 01:04:26know, is the the the best maybe way
  • 01:04:29we can measure consciousness but.
  • 01:04:31Would you think that there could be
  • 01:04:33other biological things that we can
  • 01:04:35measure that should be as we don't
  • 01:04:36know yet but should be as indicative
  • 01:04:38of consciousness and maybe better than
  • 01:04:40verbal report. So there going to be lots
  • 01:04:43of correlates, but the question is.
  • 01:04:45How do we separate correlates from causes?
  • 01:04:48You know and. That's the problem and I don't,
  • 01:04:53I, I really don't know how we get past
  • 01:04:55that because I think we can, you know,
  • 01:04:58as a scientist, I'm more concerned with.
  • 01:05:01What we can learn using verbal report then?
  • 01:05:04As opposed to what?
  • 01:05:06We can't learn from them.
  • 01:05:07That makes any sense because I think.
  • 01:05:11You know, with better understanding of
  • 01:05:13neuroanatomy, we can make progress.
  • 01:05:17Beyond where we stand right now,
  • 01:05:19we need to build.
  • 01:05:20The understanding of the brain and but
  • 01:05:23not over interpret the brain because we
  • 01:05:25need to have it the interpretations we
  • 01:05:28develop have to go hand in hand right.
  • 01:05:30We've got to understand something
  • 01:05:31about the brain but we can't let
  • 01:05:33the brain tell us what is conscious
  • 01:05:35and that is not we verbal report
  • 01:05:37is so good for that but again it's
  • 01:05:40limited but we can make progress on
  • 01:05:42that area now from the point of view
  • 01:05:44of the clinician you know So what
  • 01:05:46do we do well I think they're.
  • 01:05:49You know, obviously you can't simply
  • 01:05:51trust what the person says, right? But.
  • 01:05:56If the person, if you can tell if
  • 01:05:59the person is suffering and the
  • 01:06:01behavioral treatments are the you know.
  • 01:06:05Pharmaceutical treatments.
  • 01:06:06How are you treating the
  • 01:06:08person if they're not working?
  • 01:06:09The person is still suffering, right?
  • 01:06:11And maybe the reason they're still
  • 01:06:13suffering is because even if they
  • 01:06:15aren't telling you what's wrong,
  • 01:06:16they're suffering mentally in a way that.
  • 01:06:21Is more causal to their.
  • 01:06:23What's their overall experience and
  • 01:06:25what they're able to talk about?
  • 01:06:27Because, you know, again,
  • 01:06:29the preconscious cognitive states don't
  • 01:06:31all come to conscious and to consciousness.
  • 01:06:34So when I'm talking about consciousness,
  • 01:06:37I'm including all that pre conscious stuff as
  • 01:06:40part of the emotion part of the experience.
  • 01:06:43And it can't all come out.
  • 01:06:44Some of the will come out and
  • 01:06:47unconscious or conscious,
  • 01:06:48willful report.
  • 01:06:49But some of it is stuck in there and
  • 01:06:52you have to pull it out in other ways.
  • 01:06:54So the science and the clinic
  • 01:06:57don't always kind of you know
  • 01:07:00go hand in hand completely,
  • 01:07:02but I think they're they're probably some
  • 01:07:05things that are valuable what I'm trying
  • 01:07:07to say that could be helpful clinically.
  • 01:07:10For example, like,
  • 01:07:12take the whole psychedelic revolution.
  • 01:07:15Maybe what's going on there
  • 01:07:18is some borrowing from Kalina
  • 01:07:21Christoff work on all this?
  • 01:07:24Is the that the psychedelics are.
  • 01:07:30Allowing you to have a a new higher
  • 01:07:33order experience about who you
  • 01:07:35are and what's going on, right?
  • 01:07:37Because it's it's breaking down
  • 01:07:39the rigid thought processes, right,
  • 01:07:42and allowing novel thought process.
  • 01:07:46And also, you know, mindfulness of
  • 01:07:48course is about higher order States and
  • 01:07:50the experiences that you have, right,
  • 01:07:52and shutting down certain kinds of things.
  • 01:07:55So I think that the. That we have.
  • 01:08:01We need a broader perspective on.
  • 01:08:03I can't explain how all these things work,
  • 01:08:06obviously, but I think that the
  • 01:08:08fact that the field is moving
  • 01:08:10in this direction seems to be.
  • 01:08:12Opening new avenues for
  • 01:08:14thinking about this thing.
  • 01:08:18I have a question,
  • 01:08:19see if I can get this right.
  • 01:08:20So as psychologists,
  • 01:08:23psychiatrists we use questionnaires
  • 01:08:26and if you think about it,
  • 01:08:27each person interprets a
  • 01:08:29liquor scale differently.
  • 01:08:30They use their own semantic
  • 01:08:32network to interpret item and then
  • 01:08:35they fill out different items,
  • 01:08:36they endorse different items.
  • 01:08:38We sum those up,
  • 01:08:39we have a number and we use that
  • 01:08:42number to correlate with things and
  • 01:08:43we usually do it after the fact.
  • 01:08:46And some people use exemplar,
  • 01:08:47they want to report my kids afraid of dogs,
  • 01:08:49they think of an example, right.
  • 01:08:50Something might just out in general,
  • 01:08:52right.
  • 01:08:52So all these things go into that
  • 01:08:54subjective report and I worry that
  • 01:08:56all we have known studies this right.
  • 01:08:59And I just would like to hear your
  • 01:09:00thoughts on it, I guess.
  • 01:09:02I don't, I think that you know,
  • 01:09:05you bring up an interesting thing
  • 01:09:07which is the the use of metrics
  • 01:09:10and I think that that's where.
  • 01:09:14You know, obviously to.
  • 01:09:17Be reimbursed for sessions and so forth.
  • 01:09:21The patient has to be evaluated
  • 01:09:23in in some metric way defined by
  • 01:09:25the insurance companies, right?
  • 01:09:27And because they want objective measurements.
  • 01:09:31So. I think that we.
  • 01:09:36That's part of where what I've met.
  • 01:09:38But the middle has gone out of mental
  • 01:09:40disorders because it's so much is
  • 01:09:42based on the objective response.
  • 01:09:44Now you know,
  • 01:09:45I don't know enough about what
  • 01:09:47people that what clinicians do
  • 01:09:49with these Likert scales to really
  • 01:09:51comment too much but So what
  • 01:09:53what's the consequence of you're
  • 01:09:54saying no consequence just done?
  • 01:09:58There's a lot of consequences
  • 01:09:59misinterpretation.
  • 01:10:00So remember back having a client
  • 01:10:02that came into clinic and the mom
  • 01:10:04was saying ohh she's really anxious.
  • 01:10:05You're anxious. And then we got
  • 01:10:07into it. She yeah she she
  • 01:10:09hits her brother and she you know and it was
  • 01:10:11it was something it was driven by emotion
  • 01:10:13and neighborhood reactive to anxiety.
  • 01:10:15But it but it looks very different but
  • 01:10:16the way she was using that language.
  • 01:10:18There's that. Then there's. In our I just.
  • 01:10:22I guess what I'm thinking is that the
  • 01:10:24essence of a person's experience is is lost.
  • 01:10:26But and and in all the ways that I described
  • 01:10:29and I'm wondering if that short circuits
  • 01:10:31are models, so I mean they.
  • 01:10:34What I've been trying to say is that
  • 01:10:37the experience is very complicated and
  • 01:10:39that like sums a lot of information.
  • 01:10:41So yeah, I think it's consistent
  • 01:10:43with what you're saying.
  • 01:10:43Can't partition it into little
  • 01:10:45elements that that that are
  • 01:10:47greater than the whole is greater
  • 01:10:48than the sum of the parts. And
  • 01:10:51I mean, I could vision we do.
  • 01:10:53We see when when you assess a
  • 01:10:54child from the parents perspective
  • 01:10:55and the kids perspective,
  • 01:10:56they have reports on different environments.
  • 01:10:58Those don't correspond.
  • 01:10:59But other examples could be would
  • 01:11:00be good to know when someone filling
  • 01:11:02out that questionnaire what what
  • 01:11:03their semantic network looks like
  • 01:11:05for the words that you've asked
  • 01:11:06them about. And that may tell you
  • 01:11:08something different about what those
  • 01:11:10numbers mean when you use them to predict.
  • 01:11:16George.
  • 01:11:21I wonder if you. Ever
  • 01:11:24had occasion sit down with Aaron
  • 01:11:25Beck and had a conversation with him
  • 01:11:28and and and the question is what you
  • 01:11:31might bring from your research in
  • 01:11:33terms of how you what suggestions
  • 01:11:36you might have for CBT. In general,
  • 01:11:38so I'm I collaborated quite a bit
  • 01:11:42recently with Stefan Hoffman,
  • 01:11:44who was very close to Beth.
  • 01:11:46And we did have a meeting with
  • 01:11:48him together a few years ago,
  • 01:11:50but it was, you know, he was pretty.
  • 01:11:52Getting on in age,
  • 01:11:54so to speak and so we didn't get,
  • 01:11:56I mean he was still relatively sharp,
  • 01:11:58but we didn't get into a lot
  • 01:11:59of the the details like.
  • 01:12:01You know, in this paper,
  • 01:12:02putting the middle back in the
  • 01:12:04middle of this order stuff on has
  • 01:12:06added some important ideas about.
  • 01:12:10Early cognitive.
  • 01:12:12Therapy, cognitive behavioral therapy
  • 01:12:15was much more mental than contemporary
  • 01:12:19cognitive behavioral therapy,
  • 01:12:21again because of the kind of
  • 01:12:23the objectifying of the things.
  • 01:12:26That's about as far as I can say that the.
  • 01:12:29You know, I think every course,
  • 01:12:31every therapist wants their
  • 01:12:32patient to feel better, right?
  • 01:12:34To be better. But there's so many.
  • 01:12:38As again, talking totally from the outside,
  • 01:12:40there's so many.
  • 01:12:43Things that are dictated about how they
  • 01:12:46report and summarize what the what's
  • 01:12:49going on with the patient that I think.
  • 01:12:52It's it's hard for the memo to have
  • 01:12:55the role that I think it should.
  • 01:12:57Again, I'm not a therapist,
  • 01:12:58so you're looking skeptical.
  • 01:13:03But please tell. I mean I
  • 01:13:04want to learn so tell me.
  • 01:13:12Well, it's a quarter after.
  • 01:13:14I want to thank you all for
  • 01:13:14coming and and and Doctor Leduc.