Skip to Main Content

Child Study Center Grand Rounds 01.11.2022

March 21, 2022
  • 00:00Great, so there's still people coming in,
  • 00:04but we might just make a start and so
  • 00:07good afternoon everyone and welcome to
  • 00:09grand rounds at the Child Study Center
  • 00:12and I'd like to start by thanking
  • 00:15Doctor Linda Mays for kicking off
  • 00:17our 2022 lecture series last week.
  • 00:21And you know, one of the themes that
  • 00:22emerged from Linda's presentation
  • 00:23was the importance of community,
  • 00:25so it's so heartening to see you
  • 00:27all on the call today as we continue
  • 00:29with our grand rounds series.
  • 00:31Now just a couple of notices,
  • 00:33and next week we'll hear from Usha
  • 00:35Tummala Narra and from Boston
  • 00:37College after Russia to molinara,
  • 00:39they'll be speaking to us about a
  • 00:42psycho analytical perspective on
  • 00:43the origins of xenophobia and racism
  • 00:45and how such xenophobia and racism
  • 00:47contributes in perpetuates suffering and
  • 00:50trauma within racial minority immigrants.
  • 00:52Here in the United States and now
  • 00:54rounding off our speaker series in
  • 00:56January will be Doctor Jonathan
  • 00:58Omer Hearty from Kings College.
  • 01:00London will be sharing some new data.
  • 01:02From the developing Human Connectome project,
  • 01:04and really emphasizing the importance
  • 01:07of studying individual trajectories of
  • 01:10brain development from the prenatal
  • 01:12period across early life to better
  • 01:14understand braydan behavior associations.
  • 01:17Now today it's my distinct privilege
  • 01:19and pleasure to introduce Doctor
  • 01:22Jenny Tongue from Duke University.
  • 01:24I'd like to especially thank
  • 01:26Jenny for being so flexible.
  • 01:27We really reschedule this to be a virtual
  • 01:30format and with very short notice.
  • 01:33So thank you,
  • 01:34Jennifer,
  • 01:34for being with us today and as you'll hear
  • 01:38from Doctor Tongues presentation today,
  • 01:40the Tongue Group seamlessly integrates
  • 01:42functional genomics with behavioral
  • 01:44ecology to really ask and answer
  • 01:47questions of importance regarding
  • 01:48how the social environment and
  • 01:50social stress shapes individual
  • 01:52differences in a range of phenotypes,
  • 01:54and then how those changes in
  • 01:56behavior can change the function
  • 01:58and the evolution of the genome.
  • 02:00Now,
  • 02:01Doctor Tung's work has been
  • 02:02the impact of doctor Tongue.
  • 02:04Work has been recognized by a number
  • 02:06of different funding institutions,
  • 02:08agencies, and foundations,
  • 02:09including the MacArthur Foundation,
  • 02:11that named Jenny MacArthur Fellow in
  • 02:132019 and which I think was the same
  • 02:15year that you renamed as a fellow in
  • 02:17the Canadian Institute for Advanced
  • 02:19Research Child and Brain Development Program.
  • 02:22Now it's at this stage that I really
  • 02:23wish that I had some canned laugh or
  • 02:25some canned applause to like welcome
  • 02:27you to the Child Study Center.
  • 02:28We're getting some virtual applause
  • 02:30in on zoom,
  • 02:32but really,
  • 02:32it's a pleasure to have you with.
  • 02:34Today and welcome to the CHILD Study Center.
  • 02:40Thank you so much Karen.
  • 02:41Thanks to all of you for for being
  • 02:43willing to carve out time in your
  • 02:44day to do another virtual seminar,
  • 02:46and especially to Karen and Rosemary
  • 02:48for being so flexible and making
  • 02:51this thing work as we go through
  • 02:53the sort of whiplash of wave for
  • 02:56whatever we happen to be on.
  • 02:59OK, can you guys see my screen?
  • 03:01OK, does this look like
  • 03:03it's it's supposed to look?
  • 03:04This is great alright.
  • 03:06As Karen mentioned,
  • 03:07my focal system is largely non
  • 03:10human primates.
  • 03:11I do some work on other social
  • 03:13mammals but by and large not
  • 03:15humans which I suspect is the the
  • 03:17study system of most of you here.
  • 03:19So I'm going to just start my
  • 03:22presentation by introducing you
  • 03:24to a few of our study subjects.
  • 03:27The animals in my title slide are.
  • 03:30Two known females.
  • 03:31This is Rwanda on the bottom
  • 03:33right and her then adolescent
  • 03:35daughter rodeo up on the top.
  • 03:38I'm showing you these particular
  • 03:39individuals because they are the benefits
  • 03:42of substantial amounts of privilege.
  • 03:44At least what counts is privilege
  • 03:46in a wild baboon society,
  • 03:48Rwanda was born to a particularly
  • 03:50high status female and because
  • 03:52in species like these,
  • 03:53females inherit their social status,
  • 03:55their position on the on the social
  • 03:58hierarchy from their mothers, she's.
  • 04:01Tire life.
  • 04:03As either the top ranking female and
  • 04:05her social group or just right below
  • 04:08that that's had some pretty profound
  • 04:10effects on on her life history.
  • 04:13High ranking females in the population
  • 04:16that we study reach maturation
  • 04:19earlier and because of increased
  • 04:21or better access to resources,
  • 04:25they tend to have shorter
  • 04:26inter birth intervals as well.
  • 04:28So Rwanda has been remarkably
  • 04:30successful at producing offspring.
  • 04:32She's had eight.
  • 04:33Live birth so far 2 miscarriages and her
  • 04:36most recent offspring was born in 2020,
  • 04:39so she was a pandemic baby and Rwanda
  • 04:43is still going the the advantages
  • 04:46that accrue to her have been passed
  • 04:49down in an intergenerational
  • 04:50fashion to her daughter Rodeo here,
  • 04:53who benefits from having a large
  • 04:55family including a large number of
  • 04:57sisters who are likely to be her
  • 04:59closest social partners throughout life.
  • 05:01And in fact we know from previous
  • 05:02work in our study.
  • 05:03Population that females who have a
  • 05:05lot of close social partners live
  • 05:07on average years longer than those
  • 05:09who do not see the top quartile
  • 05:12versus the bottom quartile.
  • 05:13Most socially integrated versus
  • 05:15socially isolated baboons.
  • 05:17So the circumstances of early life
  • 05:20surrounding the birth of these
  • 05:22animals shapes their phenotype
  • 05:24in a long lasting fashion,
  • 05:27parallel in some ways to what
  • 05:29has been observed in humans.
  • 05:32This type of the importance of early
  • 05:35life effects I'm talking about here
  • 05:37is something that's been observed
  • 05:39repeatedly in other species,
  • 05:40and in fact,
  • 05:42in much more striking fashions that
  • 05:44I'm even talking about in the baboons.
  • 05:46So here in the top,
  • 05:47I'm showing you spadefoot,
  • 05:50toad tadpoles, water fleas,
  • 05:52and tobacco horn worm larvae,
  • 05:55which actually produced
  • 05:57entirely different morphs,
  • 05:59carnivore versus omnivore, or morphs.
  • 06:02Of the tadpoles, can you see my cursor?
  • 06:05Actually, I can't tell if you can see what.
  • 06:07Yeah, OK. Great, so carnivore and
  • 06:10omnivore or more fear is actually
  • 06:12a carnivore eating an omnivore
  • 06:14morph based purely on what early
  • 06:16life diet looks like in these.
  • 06:17In these in these toads,
  • 06:20this elaborated helmet or long sword
  • 06:24depending on whether eggs of Daphnia
  • 06:27are exposed to predator cues or a
  • 06:29completely different color morph,
  • 06:31just depending on the temperature.
  • 06:33In early development these
  • 06:35are pretty far afield from us,
  • 06:37but there are examples of fairly striking.
  • 06:40Early life effects in other mammals as well.
  • 06:43We know from experimental evidence
  • 06:45that wild red squirrels who are exposed
  • 06:48to cues of high density actually
  • 06:50accelerate the their offspring growth.
  • 06:52We know that voles who are born
  • 06:54in the cold season versus a wet
  • 06:56season develop a thicker codes and
  • 06:58from work in the population that
  • 07:00I'll be telling you about today.
  • 07:02These baboons we know that diet in
  • 07:04the first year of life postnatally
  • 07:07has effects on the overall lifetime
  • 07:09reproductive success of these.
  • 07:10Animals, even years or decades later.
  • 07:14And of course,
  • 07:15in our own species there's been
  • 07:17abundant work linking childhood
  • 07:18adversity in advantage,
  • 07:20including in the adverse childhood
  • 07:22experiences framework to later
  • 07:24life health and mortality rates.
  • 07:27So we know that these things exist.
  • 07:31We know they're common across species,
  • 07:32but there are a number of
  • 07:34lingering questions about why,
  • 07:36how and when these types
  • 07:38of relationships arise,
  • 07:40including whether childhood adversity or
  • 07:42early life adversity leads to differences.
  • 07:45In natural lifespan,
  • 07:46in completely natural primate populations
  • 07:49in the way that has been observed
  • 07:52in humans to get at these questions,
  • 07:54I've been lucky enough to Co direct
  • 07:56the Amboseli Baboon Research Project,
  • 07:59which is a launch toodle field study
  • 08:01of wild primates in southern Kenya.
  • 08:03That's now been running continuously
  • 08:04for over 50 years,
  • 08:06so this is actually the first talk
  • 08:07where I get to say over 50 years
  • 08:09and what we mean by that is that
  • 08:11individually recognized animals
  • 08:12in this population so recognized
  • 08:13on site by trained observers.
  • 08:15Have been watched on a near
  • 08:17daily basis for those 50 years.
  • 08:19Of course,
  • 08:20that that constitutes multiple
  • 08:21generations of baboons.
  • 08:22We collect data on their social
  • 08:25interactions on their reproductive history.
  • 08:28On life span and we also complement
  • 08:30those data with information on
  • 08:32their endocrine profiles on their
  • 08:35genetic relatedness to one another
  • 08:37on their microbiome and on their
  • 08:40gene regulation more recently.
  • 08:42Like I said,
  • 08:42this has been a 50 year plus project,
  • 08:45so I've had the the ability to work
  • 08:48on this really singular resource
  • 08:51through the foresight of Jean Altman,
  • 08:53who founded the project in 1971
  • 08:55with her husband, Stuart Altman.
  • 08:57Susan Alberts,
  • 08:58who's also at Duke and Beth Archie
  • 09:00at the University of Notre Dame.
  • 09:02And together we Co. Direct this project.
  • 09:05A large number of our employees
  • 09:07on the project our Kenyan and are
  • 09:09based in Kenya at the field site
  • 09:12or in Nairobi and so all of the
  • 09:14data that I'll be talking to
  • 09:17you about today were collected.
  • 09:19In partnership with them and they are
  • 09:22a really extraordinary professional
  • 09:24and talented group of people.
  • 09:26So I want to acknowledge them and here too.
  • 09:29OK, so I told you we've been
  • 09:31watching these animals for 50
  • 09:33some years in the background.
  • 09:34Here is the pedigree for those animals.
  • 09:37Both maternal lines and yellow
  • 09:38and paternal lines in blue.
  • 09:40We're now up to just over 2100
  • 09:43known individuals in the population,
  • 09:46and the ones who we followed the
  • 09:48longest are from families that we
  • 09:50have up to 9 generations of data for.
  • 09:52So using this information which
  • 09:54goes across the full life course
  • 09:56and intergenerationally.
  • 09:57We are interested in understanding
  • 09:59the consequences of early life
  • 10:01experience and early life adversity
  • 10:03for natural mortality in this
  • 10:05sort of prospectively intensively
  • 10:07monitored setting that is free
  • 10:09from the types of potentially
  • 10:11confounding or potentially mediating.
  • 10:13Depending on your question,
  • 10:15factors that influence early
  • 10:17life effects in humans.
  • 10:18We're interested, of course,
  • 10:20as as as scientists trained from an
  • 10:23evolutionary biology, tradition,
  • 10:24and understanding why these
  • 10:26effects exist in the first place.
  • 10:29Is there a reason for animals to
  • 10:31adjust their phenotypes in a way that,
  • 10:33for example,
  • 10:34predicts how they'll deal with later life,
  • 10:37environmental adversity,
  • 10:38and as many of you may be?
  • 10:42We are interested in how these types
  • 10:44of early life effects may arise.
  • 10:46What links and experience that may
  • 10:49occur decades prior to the phenotypes
  • 10:52that we observe with the internal
  • 10:55physiological states that those organisms?
  • 10:58So there is quite a bit of
  • 11:00literature in our population as
  • 11:02well as in nonhuman primates.
  • 11:04Generally, including from your colleague,
  • 11:06Amanda Detmer,
  • 11:07who I think I saw here.
  • 11:09Hi Amanda,
  • 11:09on different sources of early life
  • 11:12experience and downstream effects
  • 11:13in the juvenile or adult period,
  • 11:16for example, in Amboseli alone,
  • 11:18we know that early life social status
  • 11:20has long term predictive relationships
  • 11:22with the timing of maturation with
  • 11:25glucocorticoid Physiology and
  • 11:26with the ability of animals too.
  • 11:28Resist drought later in life.
  • 11:30We know that mothers are exceptionally
  • 11:32important for baboons because like humans,
  • 11:35baboon babies experience long
  • 11:37periods of nutritional and social
  • 11:40dependency and and and individuals
  • 11:42who lose their mothers early in
  • 11:44life are very unlikely to survive
  • 11:47themselves to adulthood.
  • 11:48Similarly,
  • 11:48animals who have relatively socially
  • 11:51isolated mothers are less likely to
  • 11:54make it to their own reproductive maturation.
  • 11:57Resource competition influences
  • 11:58many of these outcomes as well,
  • 12:00including maturation timing and
  • 12:02certain patterns of gene regulation
  • 12:04and early life drought.
  • 12:05This is a highly variable environment,
  • 12:07has strong effects on female fertility
  • 12:10and later life resilience to drought.
  • 12:13So all of these papers pursued
  • 12:15individual sources of early life
  • 12:18experience and in connection
  • 12:20with individual outcome variables
  • 12:23we became very inspired.
  • 12:24Actually by work done in humans in the
  • 12:27Asus framework to ask what happens
  • 12:29if you look at them in conjunction.
  • 12:32In fact,
  • 12:32if you do something as simple as counting
  • 12:35up the number of sources of advantage
  • 12:38or adversity that baboon baboons
  • 12:40can experience early in life,
  • 12:42so we considered. Six of them,
  • 12:44in a baboon parallel of an ACE score,
  • 12:47early life social status.
  • 12:49So the the dominance rank the
  • 12:51position on a linear social hierarchy
  • 12:53of the mother of a baby baboon.
  • 12:56Whether or not that baby reached
  • 13:00reproductive maturation.
  • 13:00So men are key for females.
  • 13:02Testicular enlargement for
  • 13:04males without losing its mother.
  • 13:08How isolated or integrated that
  • 13:10mother was based on the results I
  • 13:13showed you earlier that that type of
  • 13:15pattern predicts juvenile survival,
  • 13:18whether maternal resources were
  • 13:19diverted by a competing younger sibling.
  • 13:22So inter birth intervals in
  • 13:25our population get very short,
  • 13:26the lowest quartile is about a year
  • 13:28and a half in her birth interval.
  • 13:30So we asked whether individuals were
  • 13:32faced with a little brother or sister
  • 13:35within that very short period of time.
  • 13:38We asked about resource competition.
  • 13:41This experience density so
  • 13:42the size of social groups.
  • 13:44Who of animals?
  • 13:45Who are a given focal animals
  • 13:47immediate competitor?
  • 13:48And we asked about exposure to environmental
  • 13:51adversity in the form of drought.
  • 13:53This is a very dry environment as
  • 13:54I'll show you a little bit later,
  • 13:56but some years are much wetter than others,
  • 14:01so our interest here was not
  • 14:03what happened immediately.
  • 14:04It's perhaps unsurprising if an
  • 14:06animal loses its mother when it's
  • 14:08so nutritionally dependent that
  • 14:10it doesn't do very well,
  • 14:12but rather in what happens
  • 14:13over a longer stretch of time,
  • 14:15separated from early life.
  • 14:16So here we're specifically asking me about.
  • 14:19Events that happen early in life
  • 14:21exposures that occur early in
  • 14:23life and their predictive value
  • 14:25for survival in adulthood.
  • 14:27So starting around age 4 for these animals.
  • 14:31Unlike what is typical in aces and humans,
  • 14:35these six sources of adversity
  • 14:37are actually not very closely
  • 14:39correlated with each other.
  • 14:40In other words,
  • 14:41it's not the case that if an animal
  • 14:43is born to a low status mother,
  • 14:45she is more likely to be born
  • 14:47to a socially socially isolated
  • 14:49mother or experience higher degrees
  • 14:51of resource competition.
  • 14:53So we're able to parse those different
  • 14:55types of experiences separately from another.
  • 14:57A little bit more cleanly than is
  • 14:59typical in human studies, and.
  • 15:01In the same vein,
  • 15:02early life environment is not very
  • 15:05correlated with environment that
  • 15:07experience that animals experience
  • 15:09in adulthood.
  • 15:10So here's the breakdown about 1/5
  • 15:12to 1/4 of this is actually females
  • 15:14in this case of females in our
  • 15:17study population are what we think
  • 15:19of as our silver spoon babies who
  • 15:22experience no particular sources
  • 15:24of major adversity early in life.
  • 15:26Another third of them experienced
  • 15:28one of these six,
  • 15:29and then the more unfortunate ones are.
  • 15:31Faced with two or even three or more
  • 15:34sources of major early adversity.
  • 15:37So I'll cut right to the chase again here.
  • 15:39The ages on the X axis represent
  • 15:42adulthood in baboons,
  • 15:43and here I'm showing you survival
  • 15:45curves stratified by that baboon.
  • 15:47Aces score from zero to three or more.
  • 15:51This is the kind of result where we
  • 15:54actually did not expect something so clean,
  • 15:57and when I show it to you,
  • 15:58it's you know you almost don't
  • 16:00need statistics to see it,
  • 16:01but I'll tell you that what we're showing
  • 16:04you is a difference in median survival.
  • 16:07Highly significant difference in
  • 16:09median survival depending on the
  • 16:11number of adverse experiences,
  • 16:13a baby baboon faced that leads to
  • 16:16a difference in lifespan between
  • 16:17about 18 or 19 years.
  • 16:19Assuming that an animal gets to
  • 16:22reproductive maturation to about 9
  • 16:24years for those animals who experience
  • 16:26three or more sources of adversity,
  • 16:28it's sometimes useful to put these in,
  • 16:30you know, coarsely translated terms,
  • 16:33so this is a decade in real time.
  • 16:36The lifespan.
  • 16:37And the sort of life history of pace
  • 16:40of life of baboons is about 2 1/2
  • 16:43to three times faster than humans.
  • 16:46So what I'm showing you here is a decade.
  • 16:48If we put that in human terms,
  • 16:49we're talking about differences of
  • 16:5120 to 30 years in a population where
  • 16:54everyone has equivalent access to healthcare.
  • 16:57Because there is no health care.
  • 16:59There is no smoking.
  • 17:01There is no alcoholism.
  • 17:03There are no illicit drugs.
  • 17:04There are no motorcycles, etc etc.
  • 17:07And yet there's this very pronounced.
  • 17:09Long lasting effect on adult mortality rates.
  • 17:13OK, perhaps interestingly,
  • 17:14for any of you who use the Asus framework,
  • 17:19which is proposed in some cases to move
  • 17:22through an intermediary of effects on social,
  • 17:25emotional and cognitive development,
  • 17:26what we find is that individuals
  • 17:28are silver spoon.
  • 17:29Babies end up growing up to be socially
  • 17:31more integrated and socially better
  • 17:33connected than are individuals who
  • 17:35experienced a lot of early life adversity,
  • 17:38which is perhaps one of the mediating
  • 17:40factors that may explain this relationship.
  • 17:43Although separate analysis suggest
  • 17:45it certainly can't explain at all.
  • 17:48And for any of you who may be
  • 17:50interested in the evolutionary
  • 17:51ramifications of this result,
  • 17:53what we find is that this shortened
  • 17:56lifespan not only influences a
  • 17:59female's own time on time on earth,
  • 18:02but also the likelihood that
  • 18:04she'll leave many copies of her
  • 18:06own genome in future generations,
  • 18:08and Amboseli females produce another
  • 18:11surviving offspring you know,
  • 18:13not quite like clockwork,
  • 18:14but pretty close to it about every 2.1 years.
  • 18:17So a difference in lifespan of 10 years
  • 18:21is a dramatic difference in terms of an
  • 18:25individual's lifetime reproductive success.
  • 18:28So a former graduate student working
  • 18:31with the project specifically,
  • 18:33and Susan Alberts lab,
  • 18:34was interested in whether this
  • 18:36also had knock on effects.
  • 18:38Intergenerationally given the importance
  • 18:40of moms to their offspring in particular.
  • 18:44So what I've been showing you so far is early
  • 18:46adversity accruing to a particular female,
  • 18:49and the consequences for her own life.
  • 18:51What he wanted to know is whether.
  • 18:54Early adversity experienced by the mother
  • 18:57had cascading effects on her kids survival,
  • 19:00even controlling for that kids own
  • 19:02exposure to the same sources of adversity.
  • 19:05Remarkably remarkably,
  • 19:06we see that it does so here again,
  • 19:09our survival curves, in this case,
  • 19:11survival to reproductive maturation
  • 19:13for the offspring of mothers
  • 19:15who experienced maternal loss.
  • 19:17You know what could have been
  • 19:19decades earlier in life,
  • 19:20or mothers who experienced that competing
  • 19:22younger sibling again in what could
  • 19:25have been decades earlier in life?
  • 19:27So in both cases kids of moms who
  • 19:31experienced early adversity were
  • 19:33more likely to to die before they
  • 19:36hit their own period of independence.
  • 19:38We think we know what's mediating this,
  • 19:41at least at a gross level and
  • 19:44and that is likely an effect on
  • 19:47maternal health or viability.
  • 19:48That is, the moms of those.
  • 19:50Those second generation
  • 19:52offspring to ask this question.
  • 19:54Matthew divided up those
  • 19:56first four years of life.
  • 19:58From birth to earliest maturation,
  • 20:00he asked what the survival probability
  • 20:04was of offspring in the in the
  • 20:07period from age 0 to age 2 as a
  • 20:11function of whether mothers were.
  • 20:14Able to survive or not during the
  • 20:16period in which that offspring would
  • 20:17have been age 2 to 4 sodas later,
  • 20:21maternal mortality predict something
  • 20:24about the survival for offspring
  • 20:26earlier in life and for females
  • 20:28who either experience maternal loss
  • 20:30themselves or competing younger sibling.
  • 20:32That's the case.
  • 20:33So in other words, if you are a baboon,
  • 20:37is the offspring of an individual who
  • 20:39experienced early adversity in its own life,
  • 20:42that individual is likely to
  • 20:43be in poor somatic.
  • 20:44Quality in a way that influences
  • 20:46whether or not that kid is able to
  • 20:49make it to age 2 even if its mother
  • 20:51is there the whole time and I'll just
  • 20:53tell you that this relationship.
  • 20:55This difference between offspring
  • 20:56survival as a function of later
  • 20:59maternal death does not exist for
  • 21:01the offspring of mothers who did not
  • 21:04themselves experience early mortality.
  • 21:06So we think that this is explained
  • 21:09by what's going on with the mother's
  • 21:11condition and doesn't necessarily
  • 21:13require any sort of complex.
  • 21:15For example,
  • 21:16epigenetic explanations that that go
  • 21:18into sort of transgenerational effects.
  • 21:21So in this population we find
  • 21:24that as in humans,
  • 21:26early life is a critical period for
  • 21:28development that affects lifelong survival.
  • 21:30Even in a time period that's quite
  • 21:32separated from the early life exposures.
  • 21:34It appears to be profoundly
  • 21:35affected by social resources.
  • 21:36In particular,
  • 21:37many of the things that pop out to
  • 21:40us as individually predictive sources
  • 21:42of variance have to do with moms,
  • 21:45in particular maternal presence
  • 21:47and maternal attention or maternal
  • 21:50resources spent.
  • 21:51With that particular offspring.
  • 21:53Our data suggests that multiple
  • 21:55hits compound to influence risk,
  • 21:57so the the risk of or of earlier
  • 22:01death with higher aces exceeds the
  • 22:04explanatory power of looking at each
  • 22:06of those individual effects alone,
  • 22:08and this has intergenerational consequences,
  • 22:11meaning that the viability of an
  • 22:14animal that we happen to watch at a
  • 22:16given point in time is affected not
  • 22:18only by its own experience but by
  • 22:20the experiences in previous generations.
  • 22:22So I already told you that.
  • 22:25But this has major consequences
  • 22:27for the the currency of
  • 22:29Darwinian fitness, right lifetime,
  • 22:31reproductive success,
  • 22:32how many offspring females leave behind.
  • 22:35So this raises a natural question
  • 22:37about why these early life effects
  • 22:38have evolved in the 1st place.
  • 22:40If this has such costly
  • 22:43consequences for fitness,
  • 22:44then shouldn't over the course of
  • 22:48evolutionary history we and other
  • 22:50longer lived primates you know,
  • 22:52quit paying attention to
  • 22:54those early life experiences.
  • 22:56This was a question that a
  • 22:57former PhD student of mine,
  • 22:58Amanda Lea,
  • 22:59is now faculty at Vanderbilt
  • 23:01was very interested in,
  • 23:03and she attempted to disentangle 2
  • 23:05of the predominant hypothesis for
  • 23:08why early life effects evolved.
  • 23:10These are often used to explain
  • 23:12early life effects in humans too,
  • 23:14so I think that there is some
  • 23:17some generalizability here.
  • 23:20The 1st is a class of of explanations
  • 23:22I'll refer to as early life programming,
  • 23:25adaptive programming,
  • 23:26or sometimes you'll see.
  • 23:27Adaptive responses which posits
  • 23:29that what's going on is that young
  • 23:34animals are taking cues from their
  • 23:36environment to adjust their phenotype
  • 23:38in a way that better prepares
  • 23:40them for a similar environmental
  • 23:43exposure later in Life OK,
  • 23:45and so if you use these kind of fitness
  • 23:47nor sorry reaction norm representations,
  • 23:50what that means is that individuals
  • 23:52who are born in a poor early
  • 23:54environment actually do better.
  • 23:56If the quality of the environment.
  • 23:57Is also poor in adulthood and vice versa.
  • 24:00Individuals who are born in a
  • 24:03benign environment do better
  • 24:04in an environment that are high
  • 24:07quality in adulthood relative to
  • 24:09that other class of individuals.
  • 24:11A major alternative class of hypothesis
  • 24:13is what is often termed developmental
  • 24:15constraints or a silver spoon effect,
  • 24:18which basically posits that good
  • 24:21benign early environments are
  • 24:23good for you no matter what your
  • 24:26adult environment looks like,
  • 24:27and so the consequences of early
  • 24:30life adversity are because.
  • 24:32Individuals have to physiologically
  • 24:34adapt to their environment,
  • 24:35and they're basically making
  • 24:36the best of a bad job.
  • 24:39A real challenge with distinguishing these
  • 24:41between these two hypothesis is that often,
  • 24:44particularly in human natural experiments,
  • 24:46what we what we have our data from.
  • 24:48Individuals born in poor versus
  • 24:50high quality early environments.
  • 24:52You can think about classical
  • 24:53studies like the Dutch hunger,
  • 24:54winter or the Great Leap
  • 24:57Forward studies in China,
  • 24:58but they're measured in adulthood
  • 25:00in relatively benign environments.
  • 25:02In other words,
  • 25:02we're seeing two of these points,
  • 25:04not four of these points,
  • 25:05and if you only see two of these
  • 25:06points on the right hand side,
  • 25:07you can't actually distinguish.
  • 25:09Between that crossing pattern,
  • 25:11that interaction pattern or a pattern
  • 25:13that would be much more consistent
  • 25:15with developmental constraints.
  • 25:17So we think we can do this in
  • 25:20Amboseli because there is a major
  • 25:22source of environmental variation
  • 25:23that can cause hardship or relative
  • 25:26advantage that is completely
  • 25:28exogenous to the fabulous themselves,
  • 25:30and that's simply defined by
  • 25:31patterns of rainfall in Amboseli,
  • 25:33which can be quite dry in some years less
  • 25:35than rainfall in Phoenix for comparison.
  • 25:38So desert like these are
  • 25:40hydrological years going back into
  • 25:41the 70s, or they can be relatively high,
  • 25:45not as high as New Haven.
  • 25:46In case you're interested
  • 25:47in putting this in context,
  • 25:48New Haven get spelled.
  • 25:491200 millimeters of precipitation a year,
  • 25:52but high enough that we aren't talking
  • 25:54about desert like conditions anymore.
  • 25:56OK, and of course this variation again is
  • 25:59something that the baboon like baboons,
  • 26:01nor we have any kind of control over.
  • 26:04Now in 2009 we had the equivalent
  • 26:06of a weight of a natural experiment
  • 26:08in our own natural population,
  • 26:10which was the worst drought ever recorded
  • 26:12in the history of this ecosystem,
  • 26:14and it was compounded by the fact
  • 26:16that the year before 2008 was
  • 26:18actually also a low rainfall year,
  • 26:19so animals were really suffering
  • 26:21in the basin.
  • 26:22There was large scale die
  • 26:24off of large animals.
  • 26:25We did not see a lot of mortality
  • 26:28consequences in the baboons,
  • 26:30but we did see a huge drop in fertility.
  • 26:32So here on the Y axis.
  • 26:34I'm showing you rates of conception per
  • 26:37adult female by hydrological year and this
  • 26:40is 2009 where it dropped by about 25%.
  • 26:43So the animals are very much
  • 26:45feeling these kinds of effects,
  • 26:47so this gave us the ability to ask in
  • 26:50a poor quality adult environment 2009
  • 26:53versus good quality adult environments.
  • 26:56So the middle 50% of rainfall years
  • 26:59were treating in that sort of way.
  • 27:01In this analysis,
  • 27:02how did individuals who were born in poor?
  • 27:04Early environments during early life
  • 27:07droughts do compared to individuals born
  • 27:09in modern high quality early environments.
  • 27:12In terms of their ability to conceive
  • 27:15offspring and also to resume reproductive.
  • 27:20Cycling after a period of postpartum minaria.
  • 27:24OK,
  • 27:25so that's what we'll focus on.
  • 27:28These fertility related outcomes.
  • 27:29And here's what we get as a result.
  • 27:32What we find is that for females who are
  • 27:34born in relatively benign environments,
  • 27:37there's actually very little
  • 27:39difference in their probability of
  • 27:42conceiving or resuming cycling.
  • 27:44This is conception data.
  • 27:45Here in moderate years versus in the drought.
  • 27:49They're relatively buffered,
  • 27:50although you see a little bit of a decrement.
  • 27:53This is actually a comparison within
  • 27:55individuals for for females who conceived
  • 27:58in both of those types of environments,
  • 28:00so these comparisons are going
  • 28:02to be centered around 0,
  • 28:04whereas for females who were
  • 28:05born during droughts,
  • 28:06they took a much larger hit in
  • 28:09comparison to their own reproductive
  • 28:10performance and mop in moderate years.
  • 28:13In other words,
  • 28:14there is a difference between how well
  • 28:16females who were born and droughts and
  • 28:18females who were born in good years did,
  • 28:20but is in the opposite direction
  • 28:22as predicted by the predictive
  • 28:24adaptive response model.
  • 28:26We actually also see some,
  • 28:27some some preliminary evidence for
  • 28:29social buffering in this situation
  • 28:31for females who were both born in a
  • 28:33drought and lived as reproductive adults
  • 28:35through that very severe 2009 drought,
  • 28:38we find that females were able to maintain
  • 28:42their ability to conceive if they were
  • 28:45born to high status mothers versus
  • 28:47females who were born to low status.
  • 28:49Mothers were less able to buffer
  • 28:52her against these multiple hits.
  • 28:54And so you know, Amanda came to me.
  • 28:56And she found this result.
  • 28:57She said, well, I think all we're
  • 28:59saying is that if you were born in
  • 29:01a terrible year and then you had
  • 29:02the bad luck of living through,
  • 29:04you know one of the worst years on record.
  • 29:05And you know you are.
  • 29:09Experiencing social disadvantage
  • 29:10is the result of being low on a
  • 29:13social hierarchy than that is bad.
  • 29:14And that's true.
  • 29:15I mean, maybe that's not very surprising,
  • 29:17but the fact is it is counter to one
  • 29:20of the dominant predictions in the
  • 29:23literature about why these things happen.
  • 29:26So
  • 29:27you're going on to that.
  • 29:28There's just a little bit of *********
  • 29:29that we're hearing from the audio.
  • 29:30I'm wondering if there's anything with
  • 29:32your microphone or something on your
  • 29:34microphone that we could try moving,
  • 29:36and it's not. It's not too too bad,
  • 29:38it's just a little bit of *********.
  • 29:40OK, I can probably switch my
  • 29:42microphone. This may switch.
  • 29:46This may switch the image
  • 29:47for a SEK, so bear with me.
  • 29:50Sorry to interrupt you. I just
  • 29:51Oh no, no problem. It's better
  • 29:54to to actually be able to hear
  • 29:56we can. We can hear you fine. It's just
  • 29:58a little bit of of of of *********.
  • 30:01OK yeah I'm going to switch
  • 30:02mikes in just a second once.
  • 30:06Just as you did your shadow to Amanda,
  • 30:07I thought that was a knife, nice.
  • 30:10Correct, OK? Are you seeing a
  • 30:12presenter view now or are you seeing
  • 30:16no? We're seeing a blank screen?
  • 30:18Well, just seeing a square of white
  • 30:21square of white, that's not
  • 30:22what I want to show you, OK?
  • 30:26Yeah, the baboon pictures
  • 30:27are far more preferable.
  • 30:30Let me let me try and work on that.
  • 30:32I can probably go. We've got him.
  • 30:33Now we can see your slides, but just OK.
  • 30:38And then let me go back to zoom.
  • 30:41I'm gonna stop share for just a second.
  • 30:43I just fix this problem.
  • 30:45Sorry about that Dani.
  • 30:46No no, no that's OK.
  • 30:47Thank you for telling me.
  • 30:51OK.
  • 30:55OK, I've just switched.
  • 30:57Microphones is that better?
  • 30:59We don't hear any *********
  • 31:00at the moment then.
  • 31:01OK, great and then.
  • 31:06Go back to the screen share, oops.
  • 31:18OK, are you seeing my slides?
  • 31:20We can see your Internet actually.
  • 31:23Yeah now we can see your slides
  • 31:26and it looks like the way it's
  • 31:27supposed to and not not presented.
  • 31:29It looks great.
  • 31:30OK perfect, thanks so much no problem.
  • 31:33So I think the evidence that we have is
  • 31:36against the idea of adaptive programming,
  • 31:38but much more easily explained
  • 31:42by contingently experience,
  • 31:43developmental constraints in other words.
  • 31:45Females who are born in a
  • 31:47disadvantageous environment do
  • 31:48worse even in that same type of dis,
  • 31:50and fit advantageous environment
  • 31:52when they grow up.
  • 31:54We actually don't see those effects
  • 31:56in moderate years and for females
  • 31:59who were born in moderate years.
  • 32:01The effect is is much attenuated relative
  • 32:04to females who were born in in dry years.
  • 32:09Additionally,
  • 32:09there are other sources of relative
  • 32:11advantage in diversity that can
  • 32:12have the same kind of effect,
  • 32:14including being born to a relatively
  • 32:17socially privileged family.
  • 32:20OK, and I'll just note that this is
  • 32:22fairly consistent with the pattern that I
  • 32:24think is emerging from long lived species,
  • 32:27including humans that because
  • 32:29of our very long lives,
  • 32:32setting a strong, making a strong bet,
  • 32:34making a strong prediction from
  • 32:36an experience in in utero,
  • 32:38or in the first years of life,
  • 32:41is probably not wise for animals that live,
  • 32:45you know, decades,
  • 32:46whereas it may be very wise
  • 32:48for a water free or.
  • 32:50Or for a tobacco hornworms?
  • 32:52OK.
  • 32:52So finally I think one of the biggest
  • 32:57puzzles that is of shared interest
  • 32:59to people interested in early life
  • 33:01effects is of course this very
  • 33:02general question of how where we can
  • 33:04be talking about multiple types of
  • 33:06different types of mechanisms from
  • 33:08social and behavioral mechanisms
  • 33:10to biological mechanisms that are
  • 33:12adjusted based on the early life
  • 33:14environment and one of the puzzling
  • 33:15things about relating early life
  • 33:18adversity to phenotypic outcomes
  • 33:19later in life is that they don't
  • 33:22affect you know single types of
  • 33:24outcomes with very clear ideology.
  • 33:26But rather tend to have very general
  • 33:29effects on a lot of different
  • 33:31outcomes that have lots of different
  • 33:34underlying mechanisms.
  • 33:35So I think a common and influential
  • 33:38idea about how this works is through a
  • 33:42general process of biological embedding.
  • 33:44And here I'm using the criteria
  • 33:46defined by herzman,
  • 33:48where the environment somehow you know,
  • 33:51influences what's going on under
  • 33:53the skin is at the physiological
  • 33:55and molecular level to influence
  • 33:58biological and developmental processes,
  • 34:00meaning that systematic differences
  • 34:03in experience like being born.
  • 34:05Two in a low resource environment
  • 34:08and an environment that produces
  • 34:10material deprivation or social
  • 34:11deprivation can lead to systematically
  • 34:14different types of biological
  • 34:16states that remain stable overtime.
  • 34:19And crucially, to actually mediate,
  • 34:22you know this sort of bubble on the left.
  • 34:25At the relationship with bubble on
  • 34:27the left and the bubble on the right,
  • 34:29these differences,
  • 34:30whatever changes at that molecular
  • 34:32and physiological level,
  • 34:33must have the capacity to influence
  • 34:36trait variation over the life course.
  • 34:38For those interested in social epigenetics,
  • 34:43much of this much of the attention to
  • 34:46a potential mechanism has therefore
  • 34:48focused on the epigenome, and for both,
  • 34:50I think reasons of measurement,
  • 34:51and because DNA methylation is a relatively
  • 34:55stable epigenetic mark to the epigenetic
  • 34:59marker of DNA methylation, in particular,
  • 35:01that is the addition or removal of a methyl
  • 35:05group to invertebrates, typically cytisine.
  • 35:08Nucleotides where they're followed
  • 35:09by Queens in the genome.
  • 35:11So there are potentially about 20 million or
  • 35:14so of these CPG sites in in a human genome.
  • 35:18So this is thought to be a plausible
  • 35:20mechanism in part because DNA methylation
  • 35:22is known to be environmentally responsive.
  • 35:25It's part of the gene regulatory machinery,
  • 35:27and our genome must be able to
  • 35:29flexibly respond to its immediate
  • 35:31environment throughout life.
  • 35:33In fact, this is happening even as we speak
  • 35:35as a consequence of what we might have
  • 35:38eaten before circadian rhythms and so on.
  • 35:40Depending on where in the
  • 35:42genome you're talking about.
  • 35:43Again, DNA methylation has relatively
  • 35:46remarkable fidelity across cell division,
  • 35:49and so has the potential to
  • 35:52remain stable overtime even from
  • 35:54early life into into later years.
  • 35:57Evidence that this may in fact be
  • 35:59a plausible pathway comes from
  • 36:01correlative studies that link
  • 36:03early life environmental exposures
  • 36:05with epigenetic change in humans.
  • 36:07But of course,
  • 36:08suffer from the potential confounding
  • 36:10of early environments that affect
  • 36:12adult environments that are actually
  • 36:14immediately responsible for the types
  • 36:16of epigenetic patterns that have been
  • 36:18documented in many population studies so far.
  • 36:22However,
  • 36:22I think of a potentially bigger
  • 36:24problem is that although we know
  • 36:26that DNA methylation can influence
  • 36:28gene regulation and therefore change
  • 36:30the Nixon expression in a way that
  • 36:32it could be phenotypically relevant,
  • 36:35it doesn't always do so.
  • 36:37And we know this from experimental studies,
  • 36:39for example that if used epigenomic
  • 36:42editing technologies to specifically
  • 36:44change DNA methylation at individual sites,
  • 36:47so these are four data from another lab
  • 36:50that focused on changing DNA methylation,
  • 36:53and a very specific manner.
  • 36:55By 4 CPG sites at 1 gene in
  • 36:58the genome and looked at it.
  • 37:00Effects on expression and only
  • 37:02one of these sites is sensitive
  • 37:04to DNA methylation lawyers.
  • 37:05The other changes are effectively silent.
  • 37:08We don't know whether the the cases
  • 37:11where early environment has even been
  • 37:13correlated with DNA methylation later
  • 37:15in life are in that class of silent
  • 37:17changes or in the class of things that
  • 37:20might have physiological consequences,
  • 37:22which are the things that we
  • 37:23have to care about.
  • 37:24If we believe this is mediating.
  • 37:25Early life effects on health and mortality.
  • 37:29So we have the opportunity to
  • 37:30study this in Amboseli as well,
  • 37:32where we can again divorce some of
  • 37:34these early and adult environmental
  • 37:36processes and separate out different
  • 37:38types of early life effects.
  • 37:39We can do this because,
  • 37:41although most of the research we
  • 37:42do on the baboons is non invasive,
  • 37:45occasionally we have reason to
  • 37:47want to take biological samples,
  • 37:48collect morphometric data and so on,
  • 37:50and so we periodically engage in in
  • 37:54brief and estimations darting in order
  • 37:57to collect those sorts of samples.
  • 37:59So this is our very talented field
  • 38:01assistant Kenya Larry Terry here.
  • 38:03You probably can barely see it,
  • 38:04but he's holding about a metre
  • 38:06long metal tube in his right arm
  • 38:08and trying to look as innocuous
  • 38:10as possible around these baboons.
  • 38:12We wait for a period in which nobody
  • 38:14is looking and then very rapidly
  • 38:16deliver an anesthetic containing
  • 38:17dart at our animals in order to
  • 38:19collect these types of samples.
  • 38:21And over time we've been able to
  • 38:22collect hundreds of these samples,
  • 38:24including from individuals where we know
  • 38:26a lot about both their early life and
  • 38:29what's going on with them in adulthood.
  • 38:31We've used a sequencing based method to
  • 38:34generate genome scale DNA methylation
  • 38:37data for several 100 individuals,
  • 38:39including a number of of whom we've
  • 38:41actually had repeated sampling,
  • 38:43overtime and post quality filtering are
  • 38:45able to assess DNA methylation in adulthood.
  • 38:49In this period,
  • 38:51in blue for these baboons,
  • 38:52this is years of life for about
  • 38:55half a million sites in the genome.
  • 38:58We asked how individual sources
  • 39:00of early adversity as well as
  • 39:02cumulative early adversity,
  • 39:03that sort of Aces score.
  • 39:05I talked to you about earlier adding
  • 39:07up these individual exposures in
  • 39:09the first few years of life or at
  • 39:12birth influenced DNA methylation
  • 39:14collected in this blue period.
  • 39:16We also paid attention to whether
  • 39:18animals were born in a relatively high
  • 39:21habitat quality environment versus a
  • 39:23relatively low habitat quality environment.
  • 39:25We had again a sort of natural
  • 39:27experiment in our population.
  • 39:28Where our animals made a fairly
  • 39:30dramatic shift as the quality of
  • 39:32their initial habitat declined to
  • 39:34a much higher quality environment
  • 39:36outside the original area of study.
  • 39:40Finally,
  • 39:40we included a measure of their
  • 39:43adult social circumstance based
  • 39:45on dominance rank or the position
  • 39:47in the hierarchy for both females
  • 39:49and males based on prior evidence
  • 39:51from my student Jordan's work that.
  • 39:56That dominance rank is a major
  • 39:58predictor of differences in gene
  • 40:00expression in our population.
  • 40:01So all of this work was really
  • 40:03led by by Jordan and we're in the
  • 40:05process of putting it together now
  • 40:07again to just skip to the results.
  • 40:09What we find is that early
  • 40:11life effects do persist.
  • 40:13Do leave a signature in
  • 40:15DNA methylation profiles,
  • 40:17but that this is really most
  • 40:19apparent for those individuals
  • 40:20born in that low quality habitat
  • 40:22and not the high quality habitat.
  • 40:25So what I'm showing you here.
  • 40:26On the X axis are the effect sizes
  • 40:30of cumulative early adversity on DNA
  • 40:33methylation levels multi measured
  • 40:35in adulthood for about 470,000.
  • 40:37Sites in the genome.
  • 40:39They're quite close to 0 for animals
  • 40:41born in a resource rich environment,
  • 40:43but they move quite further
  • 40:45away from zero on average.
  • 40:47For those born in low quality
  • 40:49habitats and we can see a
  • 40:52very similar type of quality.
  • 40:54For each of the individual
  • 40:56sources of building per city.
  • 40:59Investigated separately.
  • 41:02So this is what it looks like genome wide.
  • 41:05These are the effect sizes in
  • 41:07the high quality environment.
  • 41:08Individuals born in high quality environment.
  • 41:10These are effect sizes for
  • 41:12for individuals born in low
  • 41:13quality environment for exactly
  • 41:15the same sites in the genome.
  • 41:17And again, you can see that replicated
  • 41:20in individual at individual
  • 41:22sites for individual exposures.
  • 41:23Basically, individuals who
  • 41:25are born during a drought.
  • 41:27You can see a market effect of drought
  • 41:29if they were born in that low quality
  • 41:31habitat that's in that sort of pinkish color.
  • 41:33But a much more attenuated effect for
  • 41:35those born in high quality habitats.
  • 41:37So it looks like we're looking at
  • 41:40compounding effects of resource limitation
  • 41:42that the whole population is exposed
  • 41:45to and individual level exposure to.
  • 41:48Early life adversity.
  • 41:50So to give you a sense of the
  • 41:51relative magnitude of these effects,
  • 41:52these are significantly associated
  • 41:54CP G sites in the genome.
  • 41:56Simply, the number of them of those
  • 41:59that we tested based on each of these
  • 42:01predictor variables and the major effects
  • 42:03other than large scale effects of age,
  • 42:05which are expected based on our
  • 42:07and other people's previous work,
  • 42:09are those habitat quality effects.
  • 42:11The difference between being born
  • 42:13in an environment looks like this,
  • 42:15or exactly the same place in the
  • 42:17ecosystem that's been denuded of the major.
  • 42:20Dietary resources for baboons.
  • 42:22The individual sources of early
  • 42:24adversity that matter are are,
  • 42:26particularly those also associated
  • 42:28with resource deprivation.
  • 42:30Drought,
  • 42:30loss of a mother early in life,
  • 42:32and high levels of resource competition.
  • 42:36If the group is is, is dense.
  • 42:39This by itself doesn't answer the question,
  • 42:41though about whether those types of
  • 42:43epigenetic changes are effectively silenced.
  • 42:45Maybe they're simply passive
  • 42:46biomarkers or early life exposure,
  • 42:48or whether they have any potential to mediate
  • 42:51downstream effects on health and survival.
  • 42:54Part of that question can be
  • 42:56answered at least circumstantially,
  • 42:57by asking where in the genome,
  • 42:59differentially expressed early
  • 43:01adversity associated differentially
  • 43:02methylated sorry sites fall.
  • 43:05The genome is a diverse place in
  • 43:07different parts of your DNA sequence
  • 43:10have different roles in gene regulation,
  • 43:12and so a simple question to ask is whether
  • 43:15those differentially methylated sites
  • 43:16tend to fall in regulatory elements
  • 43:19like gene promoters or enhancers.
  • 43:21These elements that tend to loop around
  • 43:23physically interact with the promoters
  • 43:25of genes to modulate gene expression.
  • 43:27Or whether they fall in kind of deserts
  • 43:30of genes or regulatory elements
  • 43:32unannotated regions of the genome and
  • 43:35what we've revealed what we found we think,
  • 43:38is a fairly bimodal pattern,
  • 43:41where if you look at age
  • 43:43differentially expressed sorry,
  • 43:45differentially methylated sites.
  • 43:46For example,
  • 43:47we find that they are enriched in
  • 43:49this pinkish color in Unannotated
  • 43:50region of the unit.
  • 43:51We find a lot of them,
  • 43:52but they don't tend to fall in
  • 43:54places where we believe they're
  • 43:55likely to influence gene regulation.
  • 43:58Habitat quality is fairly neutrally
  • 44:00spread across the genome,
  • 44:01with a little bit of tendency towards
  • 44:03more enrichment in these sort of
  • 44:05functionally important regions of
  • 44:06the genome versus the unannotated,
  • 44:08but it's not very striking,
  • 44:09whereas we if we look at drought
  • 44:12effects or the effects of a social
  • 44:13environment at the time of sampling,
  • 44:15like male rank, we see a pattern
  • 44:17that is opposite to that of age where
  • 44:20those differentially methylated
  • 44:22sites tend to fall non randomly.
  • 44:24In enhancers and gene bodies,
  • 44:26that is, regions of the genome.
  • 44:28That we believe may be important
  • 44:29to the Physiology of the Organism,
  • 44:31and then they tend to be depleted
  • 44:34in those unannotated regions.
  • 44:36Another way to look at this is to use
  • 44:38the chromatin states annotated by the
  • 44:40road map of the Genomics Consortium.
  • 44:43This was done for humans that we can
  • 44:45pull over these annotation to baboons.
  • 44:47They recognize a number of different sites.
  • 44:49You can just think about painting
  • 44:51the genome different colors depending
  • 44:52on what part of that genome,
  • 44:54what that part of the genome
  • 44:55is likely to be doing,
  • 44:56which is defined in turn by
  • 44:58different types of histone marks.
  • 44:59As well as the DNA configuration
  • 45:02DNA methylation.
  • 45:04Where the ones on the top of this
  • 45:06list tend to be linked with active
  • 45:08regulation and the ones on the bottom
  • 45:10of the list tend to be associated
  • 45:13with repression or silencing.
  • 45:15If we look at age,
  • 45:16associated State state sites again,
  • 45:18we see under enrichment or depletion
  • 45:21in those actively regulated regions
  • 45:23of the genome and enrichment in in
  • 45:26repressed or quiescent parts of the genome.
  • 45:29If we look at socio ecologically
  • 45:32associated sites, on the other hand,
  • 45:34here are things like habitat quality,
  • 45:36drought, or male social status.
  • 45:38We see the opposite effect on
  • 45:40this left hand side.
  • 45:41There's enrichment in regions of the
  • 45:43genome that are associated with active
  • 45:45regulation in blood cells as opposed
  • 45:47to depletion in those age associated sites.
  • 45:49We see the same kind of pattern as
  • 45:52age or even more neutral pattern
  • 45:55for just technical batch effects.
  • 45:58Now finally I want to say that.
  • 46:00This is. Still circumstantial.
  • 46:02What we're saying is that there's
  • 46:04an association between a which
  • 46:06is early life exposure and B,
  • 46:08which is DNA methylation and adulthood,
  • 46:10and those associations tend to
  • 46:12fall in particular regions of
  • 46:14the genome that are probably more
  • 46:16interesting than just the background.
  • 46:18They don't provide any direct
  • 46:19causal evidence is which,
  • 46:21which is what you really want to have
  • 46:22if you want to argue for epigenetic
  • 46:25mediation that an epigenetic change
  • 46:26directly influences the phenotype,
  • 46:28or at least gene expression as
  • 46:31approximate molecular phenotype.
  • 46:33So we ended up going after this too.
  • 46:36Inspired by some work done by Alexander
  • 46:38Stark Lab in Vienna on using massively
  • 46:41parallel reporter assays to look at
  • 46:44causal effects of DNA sequence on
  • 46:46the capacity for regulatory activity.
  • 46:49There assay is called star seep
  • 46:51and it basically works by randomly
  • 46:54shearing or amplifying lots and lots
  • 46:56of fragments of the genome and cloning
  • 47:00them into little episomal plasmids like.
  • 47:02This in a structure so that if the
  • 47:05piece you cloned in this little olive
  • 47:07piece actually has the potential to
  • 47:10drive differential gene regulation
  • 47:12when you transfect this little circle
  • 47:14DNA into your cell type of interest,
  • 47:16then it will cause its own its
  • 47:19own sequence to
  • 47:20be transcribed. Basically,
  • 47:22this green sequence loops around,
  • 47:24interacts with the promoter,
  • 47:25and drives its own expression in a
  • 47:28way that we can track using high
  • 47:30throughput sequencing technology.
  • 47:31So regions where you end up with a lot of.
  • 47:33Seeds when you sequence libraries
  • 47:35from this type of assay point
  • 47:38to regions of the genome that
  • 47:40have active regulatory capacity.
  • 47:42So we thought, well, this is really cool.
  • 47:45Can we modify this to look at
  • 47:47changes in DNA methylation and
  • 47:48how those changes in isolation?
  • 47:50Just changing DNA methylation influences
  • 47:53or fails to influence gene expression.
  • 47:56So we ended up tweaking the assay
  • 47:59and and producing a separate plasmid
  • 48:02PM star seek one which you can
  • 48:05actually order yourself from addgene
  • 48:08if you're interested and producing
  • 48:10the same kind of assay idea, but.
  • 48:12But leaving sites targets of DNA
  • 48:15methylation vertebrates only in those
  • 48:17regions that we clone in in olive and
  • 48:19either experimentally methylating them
  • 48:21or leaving them experimentally methylated.
  • 48:24That means we can compare the regulatory
  • 48:26activity of the exact same sequence,
  • 48:28and we can do this for hundreds of thousands
  • 48:29of fragments in the genome at once,
  • 48:31where the only difference between two
  • 48:33fragments of the same location is whether
  • 48:35those sites are methylated or not,
  • 48:37and the results of that assay
  • 48:39for like 1 region of the genome
  • 48:41looks something like this.
  • 48:43This happens to be data from the human
  • 48:46genome in and around the gene NF Kappa BIA,
  • 48:49where if you see higher levels
  • 48:51of RNA produced at that region,
  • 48:54higher levels of expression
  • 48:56relative to the input DNA.
  • 48:58Then that points to regulatory activity,
  • 49:01and in this case that happens
  • 49:03in the methylated condition.
  • 49:04This is an active enhancer,
  • 49:06but not when the exact same
  • 49:07sequence is methylated,
  • 49:08so this was work that was pioneered.
  • 49:10This protocol was led by Amanda Lea,
  • 49:13the same person who took,
  • 49:14but I'm predictive adaptive response stuff,
  • 49:16and we generated data for
  • 49:19bedroom specifically in work led
  • 49:21by my postdoc Dana Lynn.
  • 49:23So basically we cross referenced that
  • 49:24with all the regions in the genome that
  • 49:27we know are differentially methylated.
  • 49:28An association with early life drought,
  • 49:30for example,
  • 49:31or with aging,
  • 49:32and what we find is the following
  • 49:35of about 200,000 windows.
  • 49:36We tested genome wide.
  • 49:38I just want to point out that really only a
  • 49:41minority of them in a particular cell type
  • 49:44have regulatory capacity to begin with,
  • 49:46right?
  • 49:46Most of CP G sites and most of the genome,
  • 49:49whether they're methylated or not,
  • 49:50don't do much of anything,
  • 49:52but if they are drought associated
  • 49:55sites or male ranked associated sites,
  • 49:58they're significantly.
  • 49:58More likely to fall in one of those
  • 50:01active regulatory regions than
  • 50:03expected just by background chance,
  • 50:05where again if we use age,
  • 50:07differentially methylated sites as a control,
  • 50:09you see no such signal of active
  • 50:12participation in regulation.
  • 50:13And so now we were able to start
  • 50:15putting together our results like
  • 50:17this where for a very particular
  • 50:19region of the genome that contains
  • 50:21specific set of CPG sites
  • 50:22we see. Increases.
  • 50:25In RNA relative to DNA,
  • 50:28if that region fragment is not methylated,
  • 50:30so zero methylation means that you
  • 50:32see regulatory activity he there,
  • 50:34whereas if that exact same sequence
  • 50:37is methylated then we see complete
  • 50:40repression of regulatory activity,
  • 50:42so pointing this is a way of
  • 50:44identifying methylation dependent
  • 50:45regulatory activity across the genome.
  • 50:47This particular example gives us
  • 50:49causal evidence in an in vitro
  • 50:51framework that those sites have
  • 50:52the capacity to drive differential
  • 50:54expression and the particular sites
  • 50:56I'm showing you here happen to be.
  • 50:59Forked sites.
  • 50:59That's it.
  • 51:00Just upstream of a gene that's
  • 51:02important to T cell receptor activation.
  • 51:05We can then couple that with the observation.
  • 51:07ULL data from the animals themselves
  • 51:09in vivo in a completely unmanipulated
  • 51:11environment where we see that,
  • 51:13for example,
  • 51:14male social status is also associated
  • 51:17with levels of DNA methylation,
  • 51:20and independently with levels
  • 51:22of gene expression.
  • 51:23So the correlation is consistent
  • 51:25with the causal of the correlation.
  • 51:27Evidence in vivo is consistent
  • 51:30with the causal evidence in vitro.
  • 51:33So to sum up,
  • 51:34we have this hypothesis about
  • 51:36this pathway that connects early
  • 51:39environment to adult phenotypes.
  • 51:41Our data suggests that early
  • 51:42environments also predict DNA
  • 51:44methylation in adulthood.
  • 51:45In this natural environment,
  • 51:46where there's absolutely no
  • 51:48correlation between drought in the
  • 51:49first year of life and rainfall
  • 51:51at the time of measurement,
  • 51:52but that those types of patterns are
  • 51:55compounded by further resource limitation.
  • 51:58In other words,
  • 51:58we're really only seeing this
  • 52:00when the animals are exposed
  • 52:01to fairly severe deprivation.
  • 52:02Material deprivation linked to both
  • 52:06a low quality environment as a whole
  • 52:08and then further knock on effects
  • 52:09of other types of individual adversity.
  • 52:12And that leads to substantial
  • 52:13heterogeneity across different
  • 52:14forms of early life experience.
  • 52:16Where,
  • 52:16grossly speaking,
  • 52:17I would say our data are consistent
  • 52:21with effects of deprivation
  • 52:23related early life experiences
  • 52:25rather than social threat related.
  • 52:29Early life experience like being
  • 52:31born to a low status mother.
  • 52:33Additionally,
  • 52:34our data suggests that DNA
  • 52:36methylation associated with the
  • 52:38social or ecological environment
  • 52:40and including sources of early life
  • 52:43adversity are more likely to be
  • 52:45functionally relevant than background
  • 52:47sites identified in the genome,
  • 52:49including those that we can detect
  • 52:50in the exact same data set in
  • 52:52association with age, for example.
  • 52:54So that's promising, right?
  • 52:56It?
  • 52:56It speaks to the potential for this
  • 52:59mediating pathway to really matter,
  • 53:01but I think that our results also
  • 53:03suggest that care is still warranted.
  • 53:04Warranted we see this enrichment,
  • 53:06but there's lots of individual sites
  • 53:08that are early life associated that,
  • 53:10as far as we can tell from
  • 53:12the data available to us,
  • 53:13don't particularly do anything,
  • 53:15and if they don't particularly do anything,
  • 53:17then they are very unlikely to
  • 53:20rely on a causal pathway between
  • 53:22early life environment and,
  • 53:25for example,
  • 53:26compromised health or earlier
  • 53:27mortality in adulthood.
  • 53:29And I think that the lesson for us
  • 53:31is that those correlations that
  • 53:33we observe in population studies.
  • 53:35So far are really the first step
  • 53:37and the second
  • 53:38step that we should increasingly
  • 53:40think about embedding into studies of
  • 53:43environmental or social epigenetics
  • 53:45in general are causal tests,
  • 53:47especially experimental tests where possible
  • 53:50of whether those differences even matter.
  • 53:53So in some we see effects of early life
  • 53:56environment on later life phenotype that
  • 53:59are in many ways strikingly parallel
  • 54:01to what's been described in humans.
  • 54:04And in fact some of the measurement
  • 54:05constructs that we've been using.
  • 54:06The baboons are directly borrowed
  • 54:08from the literature in humans.
  • 54:10These suggest that early life effects
  • 54:13on later life health are not something
  • 54:16that humans invented and not purely
  • 54:20explained by the types of highly developed.
  • 54:22Urban environments that many
  • 54:24of us live in today.
  • 54:27Rather, they're part of the fabric of
  • 54:31the societies of primates and other long
  • 54:33lived social mammals that have probably
  • 54:36predated our species for millions of years.
  • 54:39However, because these species,
  • 54:40like the baboons live in a relatively
  • 54:43simplified environment compared to humans,
  • 54:45studying them gives an ability
  • 54:47to ask questions about the types
  • 54:49of early environments,
  • 54:50the way they split between
  • 54:52different types of exposure.
  • 54:53And the relationship between early
  • 54:55life and adulthood that are sometimes
  • 54:58difficult to come to grips with in humans.
  • 55:00And so with that I just want to thank the
  • 55:04people who have led a lot of this work.
  • 55:07Susan, Albert Smith,
  • 55:08Archie and Jean Altman,
  • 55:09who are my fellow travelers on all of
  • 55:11the research that has to do with baboons.
  • 55:13Matthew Schippel, Susan,
  • 55:15former student who led the
  • 55:18intergenerational adversity work.
  • 55:19My own lab,
  • 55:20and particularly Amanda Lea,
  • 55:22Jordan Anderson and Dana Lynn,
  • 55:24who were the trainees who produced some of
  • 55:26the work that I talked to you about today.
  • 55:28And if there's time I'd be happy
  • 55:30to take any questions.
  • 55:34Fantastic thank you so much Jenny and
  • 55:36and you do indeed have we do indeed
  • 55:39have some time for questions despite me
  • 55:42interrupting you mid mid presentation.
  • 55:44You know just what. So first of all,
  • 55:47ask anyone that wants to raise
  • 55:48their hand and they can mute
  • 55:50themselves and ask questions.
  • 55:51Or feel free to put your question
  • 55:54into the chat for Jenny.
  • 55:59Hi I have a question. My name
  • 56:00is Tara Vaccarino. Are the trust
  • 56:04a decent? Are wonderful seminar
  • 56:06I can you hear me I wanted yeah I
  • 56:10wanted to ask you to what extent
  • 56:13this you think that this early effect
  • 56:16of the environment are actually
  • 56:17acting on the prenatal stage
  • 56:19rather than early postnatal?
  • 56:20I think in principle there is no
  • 56:23proof that drastic conditions like
  • 56:25what you're studying like drought
  • 56:28for example, or even dominance
  • 56:30amongst these primates
  • 56:32could actually not affect.
  • 56:33Much earlier phases of development,
  • 56:36including the brain,
  • 56:38not just the blood,
  • 56:39which is what you can study postnatally.
  • 56:42So what would
  • 56:44be a potential Ave to study earlier
  • 56:47effect and to what extent do you think
  • 56:50they're possible or even likely? Thank
  • 56:52you. I think they're entirely possible,
  • 56:56and it sort of depends on our
  • 56:58ability to get at that question.
  • 56:59Depends on the source of
  • 57:01early adversity in question,
  • 57:03so for some of the things we're
  • 57:06considering like early life.
  • 57:10Social status, maternal social status
  • 57:12or maternal social integration.
  • 57:14Those don't change a whole lot in our
  • 57:16study system prenatally to postnatally
  • 57:18within a short period of time,
  • 57:21and so we really can't disentangle
  • 57:24whether the crucial point there
  • 57:27is in utero or or post Natal.
  • 57:30We can do a little bit actually with
  • 57:33the drought effects because we have
  • 57:35such seasonality in our population
  • 57:36in year to year variation differs.
  • 57:38So for example, when we were looking
  • 57:40at drought effects on fertility,
  • 57:42we used the first year of life,
  • 57:44but we also did some comparisons
  • 57:46with the prenatal period.
  • 57:48So if you just take birth
  • 57:50minus a year instead,
  • 57:52which would cover conception as
  • 57:54well in these animals and we
  • 57:56get similar kinds of patterns,
  • 57:58but they're weaker, which.
  • 58:00You know,
  • 58:01obviously I think you would
  • 58:02take that with a grain of salt,
  • 58:04but they would suggest to us by
  • 58:05themselves that for that particular
  • 58:07exposure the post Natal period maybe
  • 58:09more important potentially because
  • 58:11mothers are actually buffering
  • 58:13their offspring against against.
  • 58:17The challenges posed by a
  • 58:18drought and they can do so more
  • 58:20effectively when they're neutral.
  • 58:21But here I'm I'm speculating a little bit,
  • 58:23so sometimes we can get it,
  • 58:24and sometimes we can't,
  • 58:25because those correlations can
  • 58:26be quite tight across that
  • 58:28about a year and a half or so.
  • 58:30And I think Karthik had a question.
  • 58:34Great talk is really cool stuff.
  • 58:35My question is kind of similar
  • 58:37to Doctor Vaccarino's,
  • 58:38but one of the interesting things
  • 58:39was just the reduced like fertility,
  • 58:41like or like having less children.
  • 58:43And I mean I could think of
  • 58:44a lot of causes for that.
  • 58:46Like you know their eggs are less
  • 58:47viable or they change their behavior
  • 58:49so they're like having less like
  • 58:51intercourse or the fact that like
  • 58:52just 'cause of their like hierarchy,
  • 58:54they just have less opportunity.
  • 58:56Have you looked at?
  • 58:57Like what is the actual
  • 58:58like granular cause of
  • 58:59this change in like?
  • 59:02Yeah, so in in that severe drought they
  • 59:05just stopped cycling and I think the
  • 59:08the rationale for that is very similar
  • 59:11to reproductive biology in humans,
  • 59:13which is when you know energy
  • 59:16expenditures exceed energy intake.
  • 59:19We stop cycling. I mean you see that,
  • 59:20for example in athletes,
  • 59:22but you also see it in in Syria in
  • 59:26situations of severe caloric deprivation.
  • 59:28Baboons have actually.
  • 59:30In many ways,
  • 59:31very similar reproductive biology,
  • 59:32so I think that's what's happened.
  • 59:34They're not getting pregnant because
  • 59:36they're they're they're not ovulating.
  • 59:38But
  • 59:38that would be like 'cause
  • 59:39there was differences between like the
  • 59:41higher status versus the lower status,
  • 59:43like the higher status it would
  • 59:45affect both of them, right?
  • 59:46'cause they're both starving,
  • 59:47but it seemed like it wasn't as effective.
  • 59:48So what would be like the mechanism
  • 59:50where effects one at the other?
  • 59:52Yeah, so that I can tell you with
  • 59:55less certainty, but one of the reasons
  • 59:57that being a high status female baboon
  • 59:59is probably a nice thing to be.
  • 01:00:01Is a nice thing to be is not only because
  • 01:00:04they suffer reduced much less targeting,
  • 01:00:07social targeting by other animals, right?
  • 01:00:09There's a lot of reinforcement
  • 01:00:11of hierarchies in baboons,
  • 01:00:13so there's a lot of psychosocial stress
  • 01:00:15as well, but because you know it,
  • 01:00:17it it actually increases their
  • 01:00:19access to resources.
  • 01:00:20They have the ability to displace
  • 01:00:23other animals from areas where
  • 01:00:25food might still exist,
  • 01:00:26and so I suspect that in energetic
  • 01:00:28rationale has a big role to play.
  • 01:00:30That's certainly.
  • 01:00:31That's for example,
  • 01:00:32the reason why we think females who
  • 01:00:34are higher status have shorter inter
  • 01:00:36birth intervals than females who
  • 01:00:38are low status in our population.
  • 01:00:40They just come back to reproductive
  • 01:00:42condition faster.
  • 01:00:43It's shorter postpartum area,
  • 01:00:45interesting, cool, thanks sure.
  • 01:00:50I'm very did you have a
  • 01:00:52question there or Preston?
  • 01:00:54Yes, Preston has a question.
  • 01:00:57Question do you want to unmute there?
  • 01:00:59Yep, I'm Preston hi.
  • 01:01:04This was this was fascinating.
  • 01:01:06I I was really intrigued by.
  • 01:01:08I was just wondering with
  • 01:01:10the causes of death.
  • 01:01:12I don't know if if you knew
  • 01:01:13any difference in the causes of
  • 01:01:15death with those who have the
  • 01:01:16social hits versus those who are
  • 01:01:18able to live a long happy life
  • 01:01:19and had that privilege kind of
  • 01:01:21lifestyle you're talking on it.
  • 01:01:22I don't know if there's anything
  • 01:01:23on that. We have limited information on
  • 01:01:27cause of death because we can't do you know,
  • 01:01:30full clinical workups of dead baboons.
  • 01:01:32And honestly we barely recovered
  • 01:01:34their bodies in a state where we
  • 01:01:37would be able to do that, right?
  • 01:01:39'cause so so I'll say the the
  • 01:01:41proximate cause of death for most of
  • 01:01:43our animals is they got eaten by by.
  • 01:01:45Leopard or a lion or something like that?
  • 01:01:48You know we do see pathologies and we record
  • 01:01:50wounds and pathologies over the the lifespan.
  • 01:01:52But they're pretty crude and so the
  • 01:01:55short answer is, we really wish we knew.
  • 01:01:58But everything I showed you today is an
  • 01:02:01all 'cause mortality sort of situation.
  • 01:02:03We could parse some of the individuals
  • 01:02:05who we have better data for,
  • 01:02:06but that just drops our sample size a lot.
  • 01:02:10Thank you I. I imagine that
  • 01:02:12being depressed probably makes
  • 01:02:13you more likely to be eaten,
  • 01:02:15so I I think that's.
  • 01:02:16It make probably makes you slower,
  • 01:02:18probably makes you less liked by
  • 01:02:20your your social peers if you're
  • 01:02:23causing them difficulties too,
  • 01:02:24so I think it makes sense.
  • 01:02:25I just thank you this is this is great.
  • 01:02:28Thanks, I know we've run
  • 01:02:30a little bit over time,
  • 01:02:30but Amanda does have her hand raised,
  • 01:02:32so Amanda,
  • 01:02:32would you like to ask a question?
  • 01:02:34Thank
  • 01:02:35you. Jenny is great talk.
  • 01:02:36I always love hearing a research
  • 01:02:37and as a treat to hear about baboons
  • 01:02:40in a world where consumed
  • 01:02:42by my cats. So but related.
  • 01:02:47I was struck by an image near
  • 01:02:48the end of your talk where a baboon
  • 01:02:50mother and infant appeared to be engaging
  • 01:02:53in some face to face mutual gazing,
  • 01:02:56and this led me to wonder.
  • 01:02:58Are you guys looking at or
  • 01:03:00thinking of looking at?
  • 01:03:02You know mother infant interactions
  • 01:03:03in the middle period and how this
  • 01:03:06might be influencing infant outcomes.
  • 01:03:08Yeah, absolutely.
  • 01:03:09So that was the last part
  • 01:03:12of Matthew Zippel thesis,
  • 01:03:14so he was the the former PhD
  • 01:03:16student who did the work on
  • 01:03:18intergenerational adversity, right?
  • 01:03:19And so where we are there is that we think
  • 01:03:22OK moms who experience early adversity.
  • 01:03:24They grow up and then they have
  • 01:03:28more difficulty keeping their kids
  • 01:03:30alive and that's the phenomenon.
  • 01:03:32But it's not the explanation right?
  • 01:03:34And we think that they're having
  • 01:03:36more difficulty because they
  • 01:03:37themselves are in poor condition.
  • 01:03:39Well,
  • 01:03:39in order for that to translate to the kid,
  • 01:03:41I mean there are a few different
  • 01:03:42ways that could happen,
  • 01:03:43but one is certainly in their
  • 01:03:46interaction and caretaking style,
  • 01:03:48and so he's been aggregating very
  • 01:03:50very granular data on mother infant
  • 01:03:53pairs to try and understand what
  • 01:03:56the differences in sort of very,
  • 01:03:58very granular levels of experience are
  • 01:04:00for the kids of moms who have those.
  • 01:04:03Those adverse early experiences
  • 01:04:05versus those that don't,
  • 01:04:07and they certainly appear to be different.
  • 01:04:09Although not in ways that we completely
  • 01:04:11have our fingers on yet right,
  • 01:04:12they spend more time with adult males.
  • 01:04:14For instance,
  • 01:04:15they spend more time away from Mom.
  • 01:04:19Who is driving that behavior
  • 01:04:21is not entirely clear yet,
  • 01:04:23but hopefully we'll get a little
  • 01:04:26bit more more of an understanding
  • 01:04:28as as that analysis proceeds.
  • 01:04:31I want to see thank you.
  • 01:04:33I know we are, we're time,
  • 01:04:35but I if there are any trainees
  • 01:04:37that are still on the line that
  • 01:04:39would like to ask any questions
  • 01:04:41please do now is your opportunity.
  • 01:04:43Any other questions from the audience?
  • 01:04:48And you know, I just when I,
  • 01:04:50when you're presenting your
  • 01:04:52developmental constraints versus
  • 01:04:53the predictive adaptive response,
  • 01:04:54really resonated with me.
  • 01:04:56Because obviously,
  • 01:04:56with the kind of work I do with exposure
  • 01:04:59to prenatal anxiety or depression,
  • 01:05:00you know the clinical implications
  • 01:05:02of saying the predictive adaptive
  • 01:05:04response is the best fit.
  • 01:05:05Model is actually really appalling,
  • 01:05:07because it suggests that you shouldn't
  • 01:05:09treat anxiety or depression.
  • 01:05:11Pregnancy obviously makes no sense at all,
  • 01:05:13so I can't really subscribe to
  • 01:05:14the predictive adaptive response.
  • 01:05:15And in the context, so.
  • 01:05:17Mental anxiety and depression or
  • 01:05:18perinatal anxiety and depression.
  • 01:05:19So the developmental constraint
  • 01:05:20model really seems to fit a little
  • 01:05:22bit better with the data that I've
  • 01:05:23seen from my my own research as well,
  • 01:05:25you know, Karen, as you probably have,
  • 01:05:27I've seen a couple of papers that
  • 01:05:29actually do go down that path.
  • 01:05:31Yeah, well, we shouldn't try to
  • 01:05:34address this because you know,
  • 01:05:36the phenotype is meant to be matched.
  • 01:05:39And I, I think I think that's problematic
  • 01:05:42from a variety of perspectives.
  • 01:05:45And beyond, whether or not one hypothesis,
  • 01:05:49one class of models is a better
  • 01:05:50explanation versus the other,
  • 01:05:51it also seriously conflates what
  • 01:05:55evolution may have produced with what we
  • 01:05:57might want our societies to look like.
  • 01:06:00And those are not not always
  • 01:06:02the same thing, right?
  • 01:06:04So
  • 01:06:05exactly. Yeah, well, we've got
  • 01:06:07messages coming and saying great talk.
  • 01:06:09I'd just like to reiterate that
  • 01:06:11and thank you once again Jenny,
  • 01:06:12you have an open invitation to New Haven.
  • 01:06:14We will get you here.
  • 01:06:15We will make that pizza comparison
  • 01:06:17happen that we promised.
  • 01:06:19But please join me once again in thanking Dr.
  • 01:06:21Chung for a wonderful presentation.
  • 01:06:23Thanks to all of you. I really
  • 01:06:25appreciate the opportunity to do
  • 01:06:26this and I'm sorry I couldn't be
  • 01:06:27with you in person.
  • 01:06:28Oh my God will make it happen.
  • 01:06:31Wonderful, I think we'll stop
  • 01:06:32the recording that bye bye.
  • 01:06:33Jennie thanks bye.