Skip to Main Content

Child Study Center Grand Rounds 01.26.2021

March 23, 2021
Beyond Goon Park What Rhesus Monkeys Reveal About the Intergenerational Effects of Secure Attachment
ID
6325

Transcript

  • 00:00I'm so excited to share with you the
  • 00:03body of work I've been engaged in with
  • 00:06primates for really over the last 20 years,
  • 00:09so please bear with me for one moment while
  • 00:12I finalize my screen sharing and I just
  • 00:15need to click a few more buttons here.
  • 00:18So please enjoy this lovely photo of
  • 00:20a monkey, Mother, infant and mom.
  • 00:27OK.
  • 00:43Great.
  • 00:46So I'm so excited to talk today about
  • 00:48the intergenerational effects of secure
  • 00:50attachment in rhesus monkeys and what we
  • 00:52can learn from studying primate models.
  • 00:55But first, before I get into that
  • 00:57work and the work that led to it,
  • 01:00I think it's really important to put this
  • 01:02body of research into context and to do that,
  • 01:05we're going to start at Green Park.
  • 01:08Probably many of you are wondering
  • 01:10what the heck is going park.
  • 01:12Hogan Park was the name of the psychology
  • 01:15Department at University of Wisconsin,
  • 01:17Madison and its street address
  • 01:19was actually 600 N Park Ave.
  • 01:21But when people would quickly scrawl the
  • 01:23address back in the middle of the last
  • 01:26century early last century, you know,
  • 01:29for intercampus Mail they would write
  • 01:31600 and Park Anna sloppy handwriting.
  • 01:33It looks like Goon Park and the nickname
  • 01:36just stuck and so even though the building
  • 01:38has long since been demolished and renovated,
  • 01:41the address.
  • 01:42Is still the same and good park is
  • 01:45important because it sets the stage for
  • 01:47the work that will be talking about today.
  • 01:51It's also the location for this
  • 01:53fabulous book which I recommend to
  • 01:55everybody here called Love at Goon Park.
  • 01:57Written by Pulitzer Prize winning
  • 01:59journalist and science writer
  • 02:01extraordinaire Deborah Blum,
  • 02:02here's a little bit about that book for Eons,
  • 02:05Love was the province of poets and Dreamers.
  • 02:08Scientists considered it unworthy
  • 02:09of real study,
  • 02:10yet in the middle of the last century,
  • 02:13one scientist had the courage.
  • 02:15And the curiosity to uncover
  • 02:17the true power of love.
  • 02:19And he forever changed the way we
  • 02:21think about human relationships.
  • 02:22This is the story of that
  • 02:24great transformation.
  • 02:25And in fact,
  • 02:26that one human whom she
  • 02:28writes about was Harry Harlow.
  • 02:30Now, although he died almost 40 years ago,
  • 02:33Harry Harlow had a lasting
  • 02:35impact on the science.
  • 02:36We do the treatments we give to children,
  • 02:39and most importantly,
  • 02:40what we know to be true
  • 02:43about children's needs.
  • 02:44Now Harlow is best known for his now
  • 02:48controversial studies on maternal separation,
  • 02:50like social isolation Ring,
  • 02:52which is depicted here in which
  • 02:54infant monkeys would be reared
  • 02:56in complete total isolation.
  • 02:58No visual, auditory,
  • 03:00or tactile access to any other animals
  • 03:03for periods ranging up to 12 months.
  • 03:06These studies actually followed
  • 03:07his very earliest studies,
  • 03:09which he's also known for into attachment
  • 03:12by studying monkeys on attachments too.
  • 03:15Inanimate surrogate mothers
  • 03:16covered in a soft fuzzy cloth.
  • 03:19Ann,
  • 03:19it's important to know that these studies
  • 03:22were not so controversial at the time.
  • 03:24In the 1950s and 60s,
  • 03:26because but they are now and the
  • 03:28reason for that is that we need to
  • 03:31recognize that scientific standards
  • 03:33change overtime as we gain knowledge
  • 03:35an this includes the ethical standards.
  • 03:37Now, for a look into how ethical standards,
  • 03:40and also oversight, has changed.
  • 03:42I mean, just take a look at that PPE right?
  • 03:46He's not wearing any,
  • 03:47and there's no way it that.
  • 03:49Anybody now could walk up to
  • 03:51any monkey in it
  • 03:53in their street clothes?
  • 03:54So not only has PPE standards change,
  • 03:57but the way we treat the animals that
  • 04:00we work with and the types of research
  • 04:02that we can do have evolved overtime.
  • 04:05And in fact there is very strict
  • 04:07oversight at multiple levels.
  • 04:09Anybody conducting monkey research
  • 04:10has to adhere by law by federal
  • 04:13law to institutional oversight and
  • 04:15multiple levels of federal oversight.
  • 04:17And so, along with the change
  • 04:20in ethical standards,
  • 04:21the science has changed as well.
  • 04:23Now most people don't know that
  • 04:26for several decades before his
  • 04:27experimental work into attachment,
  • 04:29Harlow actually studied learning cognition.
  • 04:31An even neural function in monkeys.
  • 04:34So for the first few decades of his
  • 04:37careeer working first at a nearby zoo,
  • 04:40but then developing his own lab,
  • 04:42he studied cognitive abilities in primates,
  • 04:45and he discovered that monkeys were far
  • 04:48more cognitively complex than rats were.
  • 04:50Again,
  • 04:50we didn't know that at the time and
  • 04:52they could form complex learning
  • 04:55and problem solving strategies.
  • 04:56That is,
  • 04:57they could learn to learn and the
  • 04:59way he devised this information
  • 05:01is by developing a Now classic
  • 05:03apparatus at least classic,
  • 05:05to cognitive psychologists and
  • 05:07comparative psychologists called
  • 05:08the Wisconsin General Testing
  • 05:09apparatus depicted here on the left,
  • 05:11or the WDT,
  • 05:12and this is important because it's
  • 05:14going to make an appearance later,
  • 05:17so Harlow and discovering how
  • 05:18smart monkeys were.
  • 05:20He wanted to study the ontogeny of learning.
  • 05:23So to do this,
  • 05:23he needed easy access to infant monkeys,
  • 05:25so he started a nursery.
  • 05:28And he observed that during the routine
  • 05:30care and cleaning up these nursery
  • 05:32monkeys where they had cloth diapers
  • 05:34lining the bottoms of their cages.
  • 05:36Whenever the diapers were removed,
  • 05:37the infants would throw major
  • 05:39tantrums and would only be sued
  • 05:41when a new cloth diaper was put in.
  • 05:43And so he thought.
  • 05:44Could these monkeys be attached
  • 05:46to their cloth diapers?
  • 05:47And this in fact led to his series
  • 05:50of studies in the next several
  • 05:52decades for the rest of his
  • 05:54career into affectional systems.
  • 05:56Now it's impossible to overstate
  • 05:58just how much we now take for
  • 06:00granted that it is normal, natural,
  • 06:02and necessary to touch and hug and
  • 06:04hold our children and to nurture them,
  • 06:06and not just for moms to do it,
  • 06:09but for dads to do it too,
  • 06:11and not just to do this in infancy,
  • 06:13but across their whole lives,
  • 06:15and not just to do it when
  • 06:17they're behaving well,
  • 06:18but maybe most importantly,
  • 06:19when they are upset and need need that
  • 06:22comfort most like you can see in this
  • 06:24final picture here in the bottom right,
  • 06:26where my husband is comforting her son.
  • 06:28Sorry,
  • 06:29the day we had to Bury our cat.
  • 06:31It was emotional for all of us.
  • 06:34So we take this for granted now,
  • 06:36and it's because of Harlow's work
  • 06:37that we now can take this for granted.
  • 06:40And his work has also laid the
  • 06:42foundation for the treatments we give.
  • 06:44For example,
  • 06:45to infants in the Nick you for the
  • 06:47breast feeding practices,
  • 06:48and also again for the ways that
  • 06:50we care for children across their
  • 06:52life spans and across generations.
  • 06:55As I said, this was not always the
  • 06:57document in the mid 20th century.
  • 07:00Common dogma was that too much physical
  • 07:02contact with children would make them weak,
  • 07:05both physically via exposure
  • 07:06to germs and psychologically.
  • 07:08And you can see this quote from
  • 07:10one of the fathers of behaviorism.
  • 07:12In his book on Infant Child Care,
  • 07:15you will soon be ashamed of
  • 07:17the mawkish sentimental way you
  • 07:18have been handling your child.
  • 07:20Well, Harlow disagreed and based on
  • 07:23what he observed in his infant monkeys.
  • 07:26He stated in his first presentation
  • 07:27of these findings to the American
  • 07:29Psychological Association
  • 07:30presidential address.
  • 07:31He boldly stated there is more to mother.
  • 07:34There is more to Mother love than milk,
  • 07:37and that's perfectly
  • 07:38illustrated by this image here,
  • 07:39in which the infant monkey is maintaining
  • 07:42as much physical contact as possible
  • 07:44with a soft cloth surrogate mother
  • 07:46and stretching over to drink from the
  • 07:48bottle that's attached to the wire mother.
  • 07:51And in fact,
  • 07:52he also discovered that monkeys
  • 07:54would learn to open windows just
  • 07:56to get glimpses of their.
  • 07:57Soft cloth covers.
  • 07:59Surrogate mother.
  • 08:01And so his studies really
  • 08:03revolutionized the scientific thinking
  • 08:05about socialization processes.
  • 08:07Now around the same time,
  • 08:09someone else was getting into attachment,
  • 08:12and we're probably all very familiar
  • 08:14with this father of attachment theory.
  • 08:17British psychologist John Bowlby.
  • 08:19I really hope that anyone here who works
  • 08:22with children is familiar with John
  • 08:24Bowlby's early theories of attachment,
  • 08:26which also changed the way
  • 08:28we think about children.
  • 08:29However, Bowlby knew of no
  • 08:31experiments that manipulated the
  • 08:32potentially relevant variables in
  • 08:34the domain of attachment formation,
  • 08:35and that is to say he knew of no
  • 08:39science to support his theories.
  • 08:42Well, Fortunately enough,
  • 08:43and this is something interesting
  • 08:45I discovered in the research
  • 08:47for this presentation.
  • 08:48Turns out boldly in Harlem became
  • 08:50friends and I really want to
  • 08:52encourage everyone point you to
  • 08:54this fascinating historical article.
  • 08:56I love you Tana that talks about and
  • 08:59really goes into detail about their
  • 09:01professional correspondence that
  • 09:02evolved giver, hug and kiss for me.
  • 09:05OK,
  • 09:06dad and collaboration give her
  • 09:08a hug and kiss from.
  • 09:15Amanda, can you unmute yourself? Sorry.
  • 09:23OK, that's better.
  • 09:24OK, so their correspondent started in
  • 09:271957 and in one of the first response is
  • 09:30that Harry Harlow gave to John Bowlby,
  • 09:32which is almost exactly 63 years
  • 09:35ago to the day. Harlow rotable be.
  • 09:38It's an understatement.
  • 09:39To add that we have research interests
  • 09:41in common and from this point forward,
  • 09:43the two greatly influenced each other's work.
  • 09:45Bowlby sent Harlow, for example,
  • 09:46a draft paper for which he would be
  • 09:48most grateful for any comments and
  • 09:50criticisms that Harlow care to make.
  • 09:52What was the name of that paper?
  • 09:54Well, it was his first draft of the
  • 09:56nature of the child's tide to its mother,
  • 09:59which he published in 19.
  • 10:0058 and Harlow would refer to this
  • 10:02paper as the reference Bible.
  • 10:07And so, over the years,
  • 10:09hard over the next several years,
  • 10:12Harlows work his empirical findings on
  • 10:14maternal separation provided Bowlby with
  • 10:16evidence that he needed for his own knew
  • 10:19conceptualisation of separation anxiety
  • 10:21as a survival mechanism for infants,
  • 10:23and particularly of the mother or
  • 10:26caregiver as a haven of safety with
  • 10:28a term which Harlow would use in
  • 10:31subsequent publications or what
  • 10:33we now call a secure base.
  • 10:36And so if it's true that John Bowlby is
  • 10:38considered the father of attachment theory,
  • 10:40then I think it's only fair to consider Harry
  • 10:44Harlow the father of attachment science.
  • 10:47And Harlow intern.
  • 10:48This relationship was bidirectional
  • 10:50Harlow Internews Bowlby's theories
  • 10:52and his inspiration for the design
  • 10:55of his studies to systematically
  • 10:57test the effects of maternal infant
  • 10:59separation on infant development.
  • 11:01And it's really important to note
  • 11:04Bowlby's influence on comparative
  • 11:06psychology and animal research as a field,
  • 11:09and these are just some of the three major
  • 11:12ways that his thinking influenced our work.
  • 11:20So Harlow then from that point on
  • 11:23made a career of using rhesus monkeys
  • 11:25to study things that were not easily
  • 11:28researched in humans themselves.
  • 11:31He studied the consequences of blocking
  • 11:33the formation of different affectional
  • 11:35systems via isolation rearing,
  • 11:37which I also mentioned,
  • 11:38or which I mentioned earlier,
  • 11:41affectively reproducing psychopathologies
  • 11:42in monkeys that are observable in humans,
  • 11:45namely severe depression,
  • 11:46and these are the controversial
  • 11:48studies that he's most.
  • 11:50Well known for, but it's really important.
  • 11:52This is not all that he focused on,
  • 11:54because later in his career he also
  • 11:57studied the importance of other social
  • 11:59relationships and so with his second wife,
  • 12:01Margaret Harlow,
  • 12:02who developed this really cool apparatus
  • 12:03called nuclear family housing,
  • 12:05in which adults and infants lived
  • 12:07in separate apartments here.
  • 12:08But the adults were confined
  • 12:10to their apartments.
  • 12:11Both the mother and the father
  • 12:12and the infants could go through
  • 12:14this tunnel and play with each
  • 12:16other at will throughout the day.
  • 12:18But at night they would be locked in at home,
  • 12:21in their apartments with each other.
  • 12:24And in this way he could
  • 12:26systematically study the you know,
  • 12:27by manipulating the amount of time that
  • 12:30infants had exposure to each other.
  • 12:32He could study the impacts of peer
  • 12:35relationships on infant development.
  • 12:37And so this whole body of work
  • 12:39started to definitively establish
  • 12:40the overwhelming importance of early
  • 12:43social experiences for the development
  • 12:45of normal behaviors later in life.
  • 12:50And beyond establishing that human
  • 12:52psychopathologies can be reproduced
  • 12:54in primates and that various
  • 12:56social experiences are normal or
  • 12:57important for normal development,
  • 12:59unbeknownst to many,
  • 13:00Harlow also focused on therapeutics,
  • 13:02and one such example are the series
  • 13:05of studies that he and his colleagues
  • 13:08conducted rescuing the social behavior
  • 13:10of those isolates I mentioned earlier,
  • 13:13and so they achieve this through
  • 13:15peer therapy sessions. And So what?
  • 13:18You're looking at the top here is.
  • 13:21On a social isolate,
  • 13:22who until this time had never
  • 13:25been exposed to another monkey,
  • 13:27very clearly very anxious and withdrawn
  • 13:30Hereupon initial contact with another infant.
  • 13:32And importantly, the infants were younger
  • 13:35than the social isolates themselves,
  • 13:37and after several months of a
  • 13:40carefully controlled exposure,
  • 13:41everyday to these peers,
  • 13:43the isolates then went on to develop
  • 13:46pretty typical social behaviors.
  • 13:48And so this whole body of work,
  • 13:51very fondly referred to this
  • 13:53research trilogy as Love, created,
  • 13:54love, destroyed, and Love regained.
  • 13:56And this may sound rather poetic.
  • 13:59And in fact,
  • 14:00Harlow was really a poetic writer.
  • 14:02He actually had a speech
  • 14:04impediment as a child,
  • 14:05and through many years of teaching
  • 14:07as a professor, he overcame that,
  • 14:10and he turned into a prolific and poetic
  • 14:12science communicator whose sense of
  • 14:14humor carried over into his writing,
  • 14:17and let me tell you,
  • 14:19Harlow was.
  • 14:19Hilarious,
  • 14:20so I also stumbled upon in the background
  • 14:22research for this presentation.
  • 14:24This article by Harlow comparing different
  • 14:27cognitive abilities of primates in the
  • 14:29early zoos that he worked in in in Wisconsin.
  • 14:31Now you might notice the publication
  • 14:34date 2008,
  • 14:34but he died in the 80s,
  • 14:36so actually this was published posthumously.
  • 14:38It was found in his archives,
  • 14:41an article he had never published.
  • 14:43An was published in 2008,
  • 14:44so I want to give you a little glimpse
  • 14:47of how he could both entertain.
  • 14:50While educating his audience,
  • 14:51the performance of the two chimpanzees was
  • 14:54no better than the best of The Monkees.
  • 14:56Furthermore,
  • 14:56the chimpanzees never seemed to
  • 14:58really get down to work and put their
  • 15:00hearts into psychological testing.
  • 15:02At the time we attributed their good
  • 15:04natured distract ability to their
  • 15:06age instead of the obvious answer.
  • 15:07The fact that they were chimpanzees.
  • 15:11And then here's one more which
  • 15:13really starts to explain why rhesus
  • 15:15monkeys are such a superior model.
  • 15:17The rhesus monkey lacks the gay
  • 15:19abandoned of the cebus monkey,
  • 15:21the elegant grace and composure
  • 15:22of the spider,
  • 15:23or the buffoonery of the chimpanzee.
  • 15:25But beneath the Gray or grim
  • 15:27exterior of the recess is a central
  • 15:30nervous system waiting to grind
  • 15:31out 100 test trials of day.
  • 15:33The difference between a cebus
  • 15:35monkey and a Reese's is all the
  • 15:37difference between a southern Belle
  • 15:39Anna New England storekeeper.
  • 15:40Our primary interest,
  • 15:42concerned learning and intelligence.
  • 15:43So those are just some examples of
  • 15:46the types of writing that I wish
  • 15:48we saw more of today and beyond
  • 15:50explaining some of the reasons why
  • 15:53rhesus monkeys were great models.
  • 15:55We now know even more why there,
  • 15:57why there's such reliable
  • 15:59models for the study of humans.
  • 16:02One reason is that rhesus
  • 16:03macaques second only to humans,
  • 16:05are second only to humans in
  • 16:07their geographic distribution.
  • 16:08So there's the second most widely
  • 16:10distributed primate in the world,
  • 16:12and you could see their
  • 16:14distribution here on this map.
  • 16:18Rhesus macaque San humans also share
  • 16:20a high degree of genetic similarity,
  • 16:23about 93%, and in fact there are
  • 16:26some polymorphisms that are unique
  • 16:28only to humans and rhesus macaques.
  • 16:33Another limiting factor in human studies is
  • 16:35a very long life span that humans have now.
  • 16:39This makes it really difficult to study
  • 16:41the same people continuously across the
  • 16:43lifespan and to get repeated sampling.
  • 16:46Whether it's biological
  • 16:47or behavioral measures.
  • 16:48But rhesus monkeys develop approximately
  • 16:504 times faster than humans,
  • 16:52and so this makes the study of life
  • 16:54course an intergenerational studies
  • 16:55possible in a shorter time frame.
  • 16:58And the monkeys I'm showing you here
  • 17:00are some of my absolute favorites.
  • 17:03From my work in Maryland,
  • 17:05this is my very favorite monkey
  • 17:06mom of all time, Xena,
  • 17:08with her infant Zinnia in 2008 and
  • 17:10just six years later as India had
  • 17:12her own first infant named Rufus.
  • 17:14And so we were able to study this whole
  • 17:17family together within just a few years time.
  • 17:20And as mentioned earlier,
  • 17:22nonhuman primates,
  • 17:23particularly macaques,
  • 17:24have complex cognitive abilities as well
  • 17:27as very complex social behavioral repertoire,
  • 17:31which are,
  • 17:32which are made possible due to their nerdy
  • 17:36nuro anatomical similarities to humans.
  • 17:40So Harlow's legacy was really far
  • 17:43reaching and it still is to this day
  • 17:46his research has provided an enduring
  • 17:48empirical foundation for future work
  • 17:50because of his foundational studies
  • 17:53into the formation of attachment.
  • 17:55We now as scientists can study the
  • 17:58interactions of childhood experiences,
  • 18:00genetics, biological factors,
  • 18:01and epigenetics just to give some examples.
  • 18:05We're also able and wanting to study
  • 18:07the vulnerability of children or their
  • 18:10resilience and recovery in various
  • 18:12health outcomes across the lifespan,
  • 18:14and scientists today can answer
  • 18:16questions that Harlow could not,
  • 18:18because since his foundational studies,
  • 18:20we've seen an evolution and such
  • 18:23techniques like a neuroimaging
  • 18:25and epigenetics research.
  • 18:27An his legacy of knowledge.
  • 18:31Really illustrate is really illustrated
  • 18:33here in this graphic that shows
  • 18:36just how far reaching his work is.
  • 18:38He's had over 10,000 citations of
  • 18:41his work and almost 7000 articles,
  • 18:43and you can see the disciplines that
  • 18:46his work are cited in here and some
  • 18:48of the more interesting or obscur
  • 18:51ones include educational research,
  • 18:53endocrinology, and metabolism,
  • 18:54general internal medicine among others,
  • 18:56and so this is really a testament
  • 18:59to how science continues to build
  • 19:01upon understanding.
  • 19:02Formed in the past.
  • 19:06Now Harry's legacy, of course,
  • 19:08was embodied in his trainees.
  • 19:10Fun fact, his very first PhD student
  • 19:13was Abraham Maslow who developed
  • 19:16Maslow's hierarchy of needs.
  • 19:18His dissertation,
  • 19:19the role of dominance in
  • 19:21social behavior of primates.
  • 19:23But of course Harlow had other
  • 19:25proteges who carried on his legacy,
  • 19:27and this legacy is continued not in his
  • 19:29original severe deprivation studies,
  • 19:31because no one wants to do those anymore,
  • 19:33and no one has done them for many,
  • 19:35many years.
  • 19:36But he trained his students in the
  • 19:38rigorous and carefully research
  • 19:40techniques to isolate how early life
  • 19:43experiences shape lifelong health,
  • 19:45and some of these trainees include
  • 19:47his postdoc and undergrad Jim Sackett
  • 19:49and Jerry Ruppenthal who moved
  • 19:51together in 1970 from Wisconsin to
  • 19:53launch the infant Primate Research
  • 19:55Lab at the University of Washington,
  • 19:57which still to this day, is the nation.
  • 20:00Only 24 hours staffed infant primate lab.
  • 20:03He also trained Melinda Novak,
  • 20:05who after graduating a retaining her
  • 20:07PhD moved to UMass Amherst where she
  • 20:10recently just retired last year.
  • 20:13But for the duration of her career
  • 20:15ran a small primate lab,
  • 20:18looking at primate cognition
  • 20:20and psychological well being.
  • 20:22And he trained Steve Suming,
  • 20:24who Appan leaving Wisconsin started
  • 20:26the lab of comparative mythology.
  • 20:28Mythology at NIH,
  • 20:29which was an operation for about 35 years,
  • 20:32and these people are very important
  • 20:34because they are the ones who raised me,
  • 20:37at least academically and
  • 20:40professionally speaking.
  • 20:41Andres is right.
  • 20:42I just happened to be sitting in an
  • 20:45animal behavior class when I was an
  • 20:47undergrad in zoology at University,
  • 20:49Washington,
  • 20:50and the professor mentioned
  • 20:51infant primate lab.
  • 20:52So to conducted my undergrad research there,
  • 20:54I obtained my Masters and PhD in
  • 20:57behavioral neuroscience under the
  • 20:58guidance of Melinda Novak, and I did it.
  • 21:01My second postdoctoral fellowship in
  • 21:03Steve Sumes lab and my work broadens
  • 21:05the concept of early attachments
  • 21:07to focus on multiple early life
  • 21:09experiences that shaped lifelong
  • 21:11and intergenerational health.
  • 21:14So my research goals are to identify
  • 21:17causal mechanisms linking variable early,
  • 21:19early life experiences to health
  • 21:21outcomes later in life and
  • 21:23adolescence and all hood and beyond,
  • 21:26and these health outcomes, importantly,
  • 21:28are both behavioral and physical,
  • 21:30and when I say early life experiences,
  • 21:33I really mean a wide ranging of
  • 21:36swath of early life experiences,
  • 21:39from prenatal and post Natal stress to
  • 21:41various variations in maternal experience.
  • 21:44Two naturally occurring variations
  • 21:45and mother's milk composition,
  • 21:46as well as the influence
  • 21:48of early attachments.
  • 21:49And though I would really love to talk
  • 21:52about findings on all of these today,
  • 21:54I'll just be focusing on the
  • 21:57influence of early attachments.
  • 21:58So this body of work I've been
  • 22:01fortunate to conduct because of
  • 22:03my time spent in Steve Sumes lab,
  • 22:06which was an operation from 1983 until
  • 22:08just a couple of years ago when he
  • 22:11retired and this was an intramural
  • 22:14research lab at the NCH Dee Ann.
  • 22:16Its three major goals were to study
  • 22:18the genetic and environmental factors
  • 22:20that shape individual developmental
  • 22:22trajectories and also to examine
  • 22:25both the continuity and the change,
  • 22:27that is to say,
  • 22:28those stability of individual differences.
  • 22:31Cross development and finally of
  • 22:33course to identify the degree of
  • 22:36generalizability to both wild monkeys
  • 22:38and for our purposes here to humans.
  • 22:41And we were able to achieve this
  • 22:43because we could tightly control
  • 22:45the early life experiences that
  • 22:47monkeys were exposed to,
  • 22:48and I'm going to take a few moments
  • 22:51to describe those in details so they
  • 22:53have a firm understanding because
  • 22:55they matter for the rest of the talk.
  • 22:57So for in our lab the rhesus monkeys
  • 22:59were randomly assigned to experience
  • 23:01one of three rearing conditions.
  • 23:03For the first eight to 10 months of life.
  • 23:06Now on average we would have approximately
  • 23:0940 infants per year born into the colony.
  • 23:11Anne,
  • 23:12randomly 50% of these would be mother,
  • 23:14period.
  • 23:14This was our control condition Reardon.
  • 23:16Social groups with many other adult females,
  • 23:18adult males and other infants.
  • 23:20The other 50% would be randomly assigned
  • 23:22to experience a nursery ring for the
  • 23:24first eight to 10 months of life and
  • 23:27so from birth they would either be
  • 23:29one of two nursery reared formats.
  • 23:31The first is peer rearing and
  • 23:32the 2nd is surrogate peering.
  • 23:34So after the first month of life
  • 23:36where infants could Thermo regulate
  • 23:38and self feed in the nursery,
  • 23:40they were then either randomly
  • 23:41assigned to be period where they were.
  • 23:44A house with other peers from
  • 23:46three other peers 24 hours a day.
  • 23:48Or they were housed in a single
  • 23:50cage with a cloth covered surrogate
  • 23:52for the majority of the day.
  • 23:54But given daily play sessions with
  • 23:56other surrogate period animals so they
  • 23:58do not have continual exposure to peers.
  • 24:01Now the research over the decades has has.
  • 24:07Has shown us that period
  • 24:08monkeys actually go on to become
  • 24:10exceptionally dependent on each other,
  • 24:12hyper dependent and the reason for
  • 24:14that is because they both act as the
  • 24:16secure attachment base for each other
  • 24:18and they have a need for attachment
  • 24:21so they're unable to break physically
  • 24:22to break apart from each other.
  • 24:24Starting at very young ages because
  • 24:26of the need of others to cling on to
  • 24:29them and so they just kind of form
  • 24:31these huddling clusters of monkeys
  • 24:33in response to stressful situations.
  • 24:35Surrogate period monkeys actually turn out.
  • 24:37Developmentally to be much closer
  • 24:39to Mother period monkeys,
  • 24:41but they are still different and so
  • 24:44these protocols are substantially
  • 24:45improved over harlows methods because
  • 24:48in addition to providing early social
  • 24:51experience to the nursery monkeys,
  • 24:54which parlors did not?
  • 24:55These monkeys were involved in numerous
  • 24:57experiences that both tested their
  • 25:00cognitive development emotional development,
  • 25:02so they had everyday multiple
  • 25:05variable rich experiences,
  • 25:06not only with researchers.
  • 25:09But stimulating?
  • 25:11Sort of test sessions that would
  • 25:14stimulate their simulate their
  • 25:16brains and also had continually
  • 25:19rotating environmental enrichment.
  • 25:21Now,
  • 25:22after the first eight to 10 months of life,
  • 25:25these monkeys would then be reared
  • 25:26identically in the same housing situation,
  • 25:29therefore mimic well.
  • 25:30We would form them into a
  • 25:31mixed social rearing group.
  • 25:33Now this was a really stressful
  • 25:35event actually,
  • 25:36because what would happen
  • 25:37is literally one day.
  • 25:39All the monkeys,
  • 25:40mother period and nurtured
  • 25:41would be up and relocated,
  • 25:42put into a new room and said OK,
  • 25:45Now you have to get along an imagine
  • 25:47that you're suddenly one day taken to
  • 25:49a new home with new people and you
  • 25:51just have to learn how to get along.
  • 25:55So although this was a majorly
  • 25:57stressful experience,
  • 25:57it was not permanently stressful.
  • 25:59They did adapt as you'll see later,
  • 26:01but it's important to note that they
  • 26:03live in this mixture ING environment
  • 26:05with identical care in treatment,
  • 26:07essentially for the rest of their lives.
  • 26:09Now,
  • 26:09at three years of age,
  • 26:11and we separated them by *** to avoid
  • 26:14unintended pregnancies and females
  • 26:15went back into the breeding colony,
  • 26:17but they still maintain their
  • 26:19mixed rearing groups,
  • 26:19and so any differences that we
  • 26:22observed after the first eight to 10
  • 26:24months of life can be attributed.
  • 26:26To the differential early
  • 26:27rearing experiences in the first
  • 26:29eight to 10 months of life.
  • 26:34Now over the last several years,
  • 26:36we've been able to look at how
  • 26:38these different, tightly controlled,
  • 26:39early life experiences
  • 26:40influence multiple outcomes.
  • 26:41In one of our primary outcomes of
  • 26:43interest is cognitive development,
  • 26:45which we assess in monkeys from
  • 26:47about four to eight months of age,
  • 26:49which is equivalent to approximately
  • 26:51one to three years in humans,
  • 26:53and there's a direct through line
  • 26:55from Harlow's very early work on
  • 26:57cognitive capabilities to our research.
  • 26:58If you remember when I mentioned
  • 27:00earlier the W GTA,
  • 27:02there was constant general testing apparatus.
  • 27:04We use that in the early work and
  • 27:06along with standardized battery of
  • 27:08cognitive assessments that were developed
  • 27:10at the primate lab at Washington,
  • 27:12where I was an undergrad in
  • 27:14which I was trained on.
  • 27:16But up until very recently it was
  • 27:18only possible to administer these
  • 27:20cognitive assessments to nursery
  • 27:21monkeys because of the compounds that
  • 27:23would come along with separating
  • 27:25temporarily an infant from its mother
  • 27:27to give a cognitive tasks well.
  • 27:29A few years ago,
  • 27:31we decided to tackle this and we
  • 27:33devised a method a really simple
  • 27:35apparatus to be able to test socially.
  • 27:37House Mother period infants.
  • 27:39Here on the left,
  • 27:40in their home environments and to
  • 27:42compare them directly with nursery
  • 27:43reared in infants in their home environments.
  • 27:46So we had no need for separating
  • 27:48infants from their mothers.
  • 27:49We got no distress responses and
  • 27:51we weren't moving them to a novel
  • 27:53room to do the testing.
  • 27:55As you saw in this last picture.
  • 27:57And I'm going to show a video of
  • 27:59what this simple yet I I feel
  • 28:01elegant apparatus looked like.
  • 28:02So it was a cage within the
  • 28:04large floor to ceiling cage,
  • 28:06and this cage had a tiny little
  • 28:07tunnel that only the infant was small
  • 28:09enough to fit through so mom could not
  • 28:11prohibit her infant from going in.
  • 28:13And I'm here to tell you.
  • 28:14As you can see from the video,
  • 28:16once the babies learned that they
  • 28:18could go in and access streets
  • 28:19that mom couldn't get,
  • 28:20they were they were all about it.
  • 28:22And so you can see this one waiting
  • 28:24and as soon as the occluder concern
  • 28:26comes in and grabs that treat.
  • 28:32So here are some of the tasks
  • 28:34that we gave her infants.
  • 28:36We gave them a simple training tasks
  • 28:38such as simple training tasks that
  • 28:39simply where they learn to push aside
  • 28:42an object to retrieve a food reward.
  • 28:44We followed that with
  • 28:45the discrimination task,
  • 28:46which was a measure of their ability
  • 28:48to associate an object with reward,
  • 28:50and so the object that was rewarded
  • 28:52would either be a black block or a white
  • 28:54block randomly assigned to each infant,
  • 28:56and the presentation of
  • 28:57those blocks would be.
  • 28:59Randomly switched side to
  • 29:00side across 25 trials per day.
  • 29:03Once they pass that test,
  • 29:04we gave them a reversal task to
  • 29:06measure their cognitive flexibility.
  • 29:08So if Black was previously rewarded,
  • 29:09we then rewarded the white block
  • 29:11and look to see how long it took
  • 29:13them to learn this new Association,
  • 29:15and then finally they don't have time
  • 29:17to go into the details that video you
  • 29:20saw in the last slide was really a
  • 29:23task to measure infants impulsivity.
  • 29:25And so, in our study of Mother P.
  • 29:28Reardon nursery monkeys,
  • 29:29we found no global cognitive
  • 29:31differences when we looked at
  • 29:32cognition in the first year of life,
  • 29:34and in fact on 19 different measures,
  • 29:37we actually found significant
  • 29:38ring effects only for two of them,
  • 29:40and one of them actually was on
  • 29:42this training task,
  • 29:43which does not really.
  • 29:47Impact the cognitive development
  • 29:48or what we know about cognitive
  • 29:50development as a whole.
  • 29:52And so this is just one example of
  • 29:54data showing the performance on the
  • 29:57discrimination and reversal test.
  • 29:59Those black white tan.
  • 30:00Now in this figure,
  • 30:02Mother Pier and monkeys are always
  • 30:04presented on the left of the blue line.
  • 30:06Here nursery rhyme monkeys,
  • 30:08which is both period answer again
  • 30:10because they did not differ.
  • 30:11They're always presented on the
  • 30:13right side of the blue line and what
  • 30:15you're looking at is that there are
  • 30:17no rearing differences in monkeys bulk rate.
  • 30:20That's their refusal to work and
  • 30:22how many days it took them to pass
  • 30:25the test and how long they took per
  • 30:27trial to conduct the tasks or in
  • 30:29the percentage of trials correct.
  • 30:32And we checked this lack of differences
  • 30:34up to the fact that all infants
  • 30:36were very carefully acclimatized to
  • 30:38testing procedures and to the researchers,
  • 30:41and especially that they were
  • 30:43comfortable in their home environments.
  • 30:44And importantly,
  • 30:45we didn't test cognitive capacities
  • 30:47under mildly stressful situations,
  • 30:48but obviously we might expect that we
  • 30:50would see rearing differences there,
  • 30:52and we also have not yet been able
  • 30:55to look for differences in learning
  • 30:57day by day or trial by trial.
  • 31:00This is just a gross overview
  • 31:02of cognitive development.
  • 31:03But looking in that finer detail
  • 31:05is something I hope to do when
  • 31:08I have a little more time.
  • 31:09So with techniques that have
  • 31:11emerged in recent years,
  • 31:13we've also been able to start probing
  • 31:15how early life experiences get under
  • 31:17the skin to affect later health,
  • 31:19and one of the ways we've
  • 31:21looked under the skin
  • 31:22is by developing an assay to
  • 31:24measure chronic stress by measuring
  • 31:26the hormone cortisol in her.
  • 31:28Now, cortisol travels through the
  • 31:30bloodstream and is deposited into
  • 31:32the hair shaft and accumulates
  • 31:34over time so that when you take a
  • 31:36sample here at the base of the skin,
  • 31:38the base close to the skin.
  • 31:40Surface, I mean the amount of
  • 31:43cortisol that you get is an
  • 31:46aggregate representation of.
  • 31:48Hypothalamic pituitary adrenal
  • 31:50axis activity is basically a
  • 31:53phenotypic view of cortisol activity.
  • 31:56And to measure cortisol we simply
  • 31:58shave patches of hair from the backs
  • 32:00of monkeys neck at routine intervals.
  • 32:02And since this procedure was
  • 32:04developed in monkeys,
  • 32:05it's since been adapted for use in humans.
  • 32:08Where no, we don't shave large patches,
  • 32:10but we get very small samples with
  • 32:13scissors and process the samples
  • 32:15for cortisol concentrations.
  • 32:17Now, if you were in our study
  • 32:19of differently reared monkeys,
  • 32:21we looked at her cortisol
  • 32:23concentrations across development
  • 32:24from birth through two years of age.
  • 32:26Now remember any differences we
  • 32:28see after 8 to 10 months should be
  • 32:30attributable to the first months of
  • 32:33different early life experiences,
  • 32:34and this figure is showing you
  • 32:37her cortisol concentrations for
  • 32:38all three rearing groups.
  • 32:40The blue line indicates that stressful
  • 32:42relocation that I mentioned and 1st
  • 32:44I want to draw your attention to the
  • 32:46fact that prior to relocation those
  • 32:48peer weird monkeys, the black squares.
  • 32:50The ones I said to develop hyper attachments.
  • 32:53Those monkeys had higher her
  • 32:55cortisol concentrations than either
  • 32:56of the other two groups of monkeys,
  • 32:58just at baseline.
  • 32:59And these her cortisol concentrations
  • 33:02remained elevated for up to a
  • 33:04year after the relocation.
  • 33:06Surrogate monkeys showed a massive
  • 33:08increase in her cortisol after relocation,
  • 33:10which also stayed elevated.
  • 33:12But importantly by two years of age after
  • 33:15they had had sufficient time to acclimate,
  • 33:18the monkeys were indistinguishable
  • 33:20from one another.
  • 33:22Now when we looked at these
  • 33:25same monkeys anxious behavior,
  • 33:26we found somewhat similar patterns.
  • 33:28The monkeys did not develop any
  • 33:30anxious or show very much anxious
  • 33:32behavior in their home environments.
  • 33:35Preload relocation,
  • 33:35but understandably all monkeys
  • 33:37found this to be a stressful event.
  • 33:40An showed massive increases in anxiety,
  • 33:42especially if they were nursery reared,
  • 33:44but particularly for period monkeys.
  • 33:46Again,
  • 33:47the squares here those levels of
  • 33:49anxious behavior remained elevated
  • 33:51for up to one year after relocation.
  • 33:54However, by two years of age,
  • 33:56again,
  • 33:56they were indistinguishable from one another.
  • 33:58Now we wanted to know does does
  • 34:00her cortisol in infancy before the
  • 34:02relocation predict anxious behavior
  • 34:04after their stressful relocation?
  • 34:06And the answer is yes,
  • 34:08we found a significant correlation
  • 34:10for all monkeys.
  • 34:11But when we broke it down by rearing group.
  • 34:15That correlation maybe no surprise,
  • 34:16was significant only for purity animals,
  • 34:19an this correlation persistent again,
  • 34:20for up to one year post relocation.
  • 34:26Another important outcome of interest
  • 34:27for us is socialrank attainment,
  • 34:29which in this study I'm about to
  • 34:31present we measured or reassessed in
  • 34:33adolescent monkeys age 2 to three years,
  • 34:35which is equivalent to about 8
  • 34:37to 12 years in human children.
  • 34:40Now, rhesus monkeys have a very rigid
  • 34:42varying linear dominance hierarchy,
  • 34:44which is enforced by often subtle but
  • 34:46sometimes not so subtle behaviors.
  • 34:48So sometimes even a sideways glance will be
  • 34:51enough to tell one monkey who's in charge,
  • 34:53by the way, notice in the background who's
  • 34:56watching this interaction and learning
  • 34:59some really important social cues.
  • 35:01Other behaviors that reinforce the
  • 35:03dominance hierarchy are threat displays,
  • 35:05and then if they get a really escalated
  • 35:07monkeys will physically chase and
  • 35:09attack each other.
  • 35:11Now, the way the hierarchy goes is that
  • 35:14monkey A will always win against Monkey B,
  • 35:17who never loses to anyone except a monkey a.
  • 35:21But monkey be never loses to
  • 35:23monkey see and so on and so on.
  • 35:26And once hierarchies are formed very,
  • 35:28they're rather stable.
  • 35:29And so,
  • 35:30although there are important differences,
  • 35:32rhesus monkey social status is a good
  • 35:34proxy of socioeconomic status in humans,
  • 35:36because those at the top have greater
  • 35:39access to resources like social partners,
  • 35:41desirable social partners,
  • 35:42better food,
  • 35:43or first access to food.
  • 35:44The best shelter which for monkeys
  • 35:47is a shady spot on a hot day,
  • 35:49and both monkeys and humans use their
  • 35:52power to access those resources.
  • 35:54So in our study of juvenile monkeys,
  • 35:57we found that nursery reared
  • 35:59juveniles both period and surrogate
  • 36:01reared occupied lower social
  • 36:02ranks than mother reared monkeys.
  • 36:04And this was a finding that held
  • 36:07true for adults as well once they
  • 36:09were eight to eight years or older.
  • 36:12But we had an interesting caveat here.
  • 36:15So we looked to see whether her cortisol,
  • 36:18the chronic stress measure,
  • 36:19could was affiliated with social rank and
  • 36:22in adulthood only for Mother Period monkeys,
  • 36:24which is the solid black line.
  • 36:26Was there significant correlation such
  • 36:28that mother period monkeys with higher
  • 36:30her cortisol indicative of greater stress,
  • 36:32ranked lower on the hierarchy?
  • 36:34But this a filler.
  • 36:36This relationship was not present
  • 36:38for the nursery room monkeys,
  • 36:40so we look back in time to that stressful
  • 36:44relocation and we found that once
  • 36:46again from other peer reared monkeys,
  • 36:49only their ability to regulate cortisol,
  • 36:51their changes in cortisol across
  • 36:53several months following relocation
  • 36:55significantly predicted their dominant
  • 36:57status only if they were mother appeared,
  • 36:59and so this suggests that there
  • 37:02is a stress regulation is.
  • 37:04Is out of wack in nursery monkeys
  • 37:07an that this dysregulation likely
  • 37:08begins in adolescence and maybe a
  • 37:11predictor of adult social status.
  • 37:16So recall that I just showed
  • 37:18you that higher ranking monkeys
  • 37:19are more likely to be mother,
  • 37:22period, and this is a particular
  • 37:24importance for this next study,
  • 37:25which examined differential DNA metalation
  • 37:27in the placenta of high middle ranking
  • 37:30monkeys versus low ranking monkeys.
  • 37:32And remember the high ranking
  • 37:34monkey placentas are more likely to
  • 37:36come from mother period animals.
  • 37:38And so we studied DNA methylation
  • 37:40as another way to look under this
  • 37:43skin because DNA changes the
  • 37:45activity of a DNA sequence without
  • 37:47changing its structure or sequence.
  • 37:50An importantly,
  • 37:51because Miss Metalation typically
  • 37:52acts to alter later gene expression.
  • 37:56And so we found dramatic differences
  • 37:57in DNA methylation in the placenta
  • 37:59of monkeys that were high ranking
  • 38:01versus low ranking,
  • 38:02and that's what this heat map
  • 38:04here is showing you.
  • 38:05All you need to know is the high,
  • 38:07medium, low rank,
  • 38:08high medium ranking monkeys are on
  • 38:10the left side and the low ranking
  • 38:12monkeys are on the right side.
  • 38:14An blue is low levels of metalation
  • 38:16red is high levels of metalation.
  • 38:18You could see that their patterns
  • 38:20of methylations are essentially
  • 38:22opposite each other.
  • 38:23And further,
  • 38:24exploration revealed that the dramatic
  • 38:27differences in Diamet DNA metalation
  • 38:29were particularly located in networks
  • 38:32that are critical to cell morphology,
  • 38:35cell growth and differentiation,
  • 38:37and cell signaling.
  • 38:39All vital,
  • 38:40important, vital,
  • 38:41and functions for placental Physiology.
  • 38:43We also found that the upstream
  • 38:46regulators of these gene networks included
  • 38:49glucocorticoid receptor target genes,
  • 38:51which indicates to us that.
  • 38:54The stress stress response is
  • 38:56particularly important here,
  • 38:57and so the broad scope of
  • 39:00methylation differences here,
  • 39:01as well as the fact that these
  • 39:04metalation differences affected tissue.
  • 39:05That's vital for fetal development,
  • 39:07really illustrates the profound biological
  • 39:09effect that socialrank likely achieved,
  • 39:11partly by early rearing may
  • 39:13have on the next generation,
  • 39:15and this is important because
  • 39:17in translating it to humans,
  • 39:19these have really important implications
  • 39:22for our understanding of the impacts
  • 39:24of social equality or inequality.
  • 39:26Of the well being and care of pregnant women,
  • 39:30and it underscores the need for
  • 39:32appropriate interventions for women,
  • 39:34particularly those who may be
  • 39:36disadvantaged and more likely
  • 39:38to suffer from social stress.
  • 39:40And so this body of work represented
  • 39:42one of our first forays into
  • 39:45the intergenerational effects.
  • 39:46Of early secure attachments.
  • 39:48Because the placenta is of fetal
  • 39:50origin and so therefore we may be
  • 39:53starting to see here in this study
  • 39:55the impacts of secure attachments
  • 39:57on the next generation.
  • 40:02So only now has it been possible to
  • 40:05extend the findings of Goon Park.
  • 40:07Beyond offspring development an
  • 40:09into multiple generations and
  • 40:10we can do this now because we
  • 40:13have transferred the entire NCH,
  • 40:15the rhesus monkey data set.
  • 40:16Both biological and behavioral
  • 40:18to the Yale Child Study Center.
  • 40:20So we now hold longitudinal perspective
  • 40:23and comprehensive data collected over
  • 40:25the course of 25 years or more on
  • 40:27multiple generations of monkeys in
  • 40:29sufficient numbers to be able to begin.
  • 40:32To rigorously test the causal
  • 40:34influences of disruptions to
  • 40:36early secure attachments and some
  • 40:38of the data we hold include.
  • 40:40Thousands like 25,000 or more
  • 40:43biological samples, blood plasma,
  • 40:45Cerebro, spinal fluid saliva,
  • 40:47extracted DNA RNA,
  • 40:49white blood samples and hair samples,
  • 40:52among others.
  • 40:53We hold extensive neonatal assessments,
  • 40:56measuring neurological development
  • 40:58and function across the first month
  • 41:01of life on multiple generations.
  • 41:04And we have health records,
  • 41:06weight and BMI data,
  • 41:08pregnancy outcomes, social behavior,
  • 41:09rank data and much more.
  • 41:14And one of the aspects that makes this
  • 41:17archival data set such a goldmine and so
  • 41:19unique is that we have data on multiple
  • 41:22generations of monkeys that were reared
  • 41:24in one of four different sequences.
  • 41:27So we help now hold intergenerational
  • 41:29data on mother offspring pairs that were
  • 41:32either mother, period, mother period.
  • 41:34So the mothers themselves were
  • 41:37weird with their mothers and their
  • 41:39infants were mother reared.
  • 41:41Or we have a data on monkeys who
  • 41:44were reared by their mothers,
  • 41:45but their infants reared in the nursery.
  • 41:49We have data on monkeys who were who
  • 41:50were the mothers were nursery reared,
  • 41:52and their infants were also nursery.
  • 41:54Weird and where mothers were nurtured.
  • 41:56But they reared their own infants.
  • 41:59And so, several years ago we forged a
  • 42:02unique collaboration with behavioral
  • 42:04economists at the University of Chicago,
  • 42:06led by Doctor James Heckman, who,
  • 42:09as Andres mentioned at the outset,
  • 42:11was the Nobel Laureate in Economics in 2000,
  • 42:15and is best known for his birth to
  • 42:18five research and policy advocacy now.
  • 42:20This collaboration,
  • 42:21previously found single generation effects
  • 42:23of different early life experiences,
  • 42:25so the work found that that
  • 42:28monkeys experiencing adverse.
  • 42:29Adversity early in life via lack of a
  • 42:32secure attachment had adverse health
  • 42:34outcomes in adolescence and adulthood.
  • 42:37But we're now extending this work with
  • 42:40the collaborators of pictured here,
  • 42:42to study the causal effects of different
  • 42:44early attachments on subsequent generations,
  • 42:46and this is the work I'm going
  • 42:49to finish out with today.
  • 42:51So with funding from the National
  • 42:52Science Foundation in this
  • 42:54first intergenerational study,
  • 42:55we capitalized on the randomization of
  • 42:58early life experiences in monkeys to
  • 43:00quantify the degree of the impact that
  • 43:02maternal presence has on offspring health.
  • 43:05And we published these findings recently.
  • 43:07In a working paper at the National
  • 43:10Bureau of Economic Research,
  • 43:11and we address a major limitation in
  • 43:13human studies by being able to carefully
  • 43:16control the early life experiences of
  • 43:18monkeys and study them across generations.
  • 43:20And so these four rearing sequences
  • 43:23that I just showed you allow us
  • 43:25to test on the one hand,
  • 43:27the potential benefit of sustained,
  • 43:29secure attachment across generations
  • 43:30here in the Blue Square and on
  • 43:33the other hand we can also test
  • 43:35whether it's possible to compensate.
  • 43:37For a lack of secure attachment.
  • 43:40Here in the red, on the right,
  • 43:43and by comparing the differences in
  • 43:46outcomes between the intergenerational
  • 43:48groups here in the orange,
  • 43:49we can describe how the effects
  • 43:52on the offspring depend on the
  • 43:54parents early rearing environments.
  • 43:56An this comparison is something that
  • 43:59economists call intergenerational
  • 44:00complementarity,
  • 44:00and this analysis allows us to
  • 44:03learn more than just the benefit
  • 44:05of secure attachments in a single
  • 44:07generation the mother's generation.
  • 44:10We can now learn about the
  • 44:12importance of sustained,
  • 44:14secure attachments across generations,
  • 44:15as well as the importance of investing
  • 44:19early in the parents generation versus
  • 44:21trying to compare or to compensate
  • 44:24in the offspring's generation.
  • 44:26So our behavioral economics economist
  • 44:28friends use their sophisticated equation
  • 44:30modeling to conduct all these tests,
  • 44:33which I call fancy voodoo magic
  • 44:35and to calculate these benefits.
  • 44:38So we relied on archival data from
  • 44:41over 650 mother offspring pairs
  • 44:43with roughly equal representation
  • 44:45across the four rearing sequences.
  • 44:48An we first focused on initial
  • 44:50intergenerational impacts,
  • 44:51namely pregnancy outcomes did the infant's
  • 44:53birth result in a live birth yes or no?
  • 44:56Did the infant survive past the first
  • 44:59month and what was its birth weight?
  • 45:01And these can be considered pre
  • 45:03treatment outcomes since most of them
  • 45:05occur prior to randomize conditions.
  • 45:09Now we found no intergenerational
  • 45:11effect on the probability of an
  • 45:14offspring being born live versus
  • 45:16stillborn or on its birth weight.
  • 45:18But we did find significant intergenerational
  • 45:20effect on infant survivability,
  • 45:22so infants of moms who were reared with
  • 45:25their mothers had a survival rate that
  • 45:28was 2.9% higher than infants reared
  • 45:30by mothers who were nursery rears.
  • 45:32I'll say that one more time the infants and
  • 45:36mothers whose own mothers were mother weird.
  • 45:40Had a 2.9% higher survival rate than
  • 45:43infants whose mothers were nursery reared.
  • 45:47So we then examine long term
  • 45:50intergenerational impacts,
  • 45:50both of which occurred after the
  • 45:53experimental rearing conditions ended.
  • 45:55After that relocation,
  • 45:56and unlike the previous results,
  • 45:58these outcomes can be affected both
  • 46:00by the mother's ring condition and
  • 46:02the offspring zone rearing condition.
  • 46:05One of these outcomes was adolescent health,
  • 46:08measured at one to three years,
  • 46:10which is approximately 4 to 4
  • 46:13to 12 years in humans.
  • 46:15And for this we looked at the
  • 46:18percentage of quarterly vet exams in
  • 46:20which the animal was in good health,
  • 46:23meaning the animal did not require treatment
  • 46:25for a problem like wounding diarrhea,
  • 46:28which is common in monkey colonies,
  • 46:30skin rash,
  • 46:31dermatitis surgeries, etc.
  • 46:32And in adulthood we measured
  • 46:34their social rank attainment
  • 46:35and this was actually earlier.
  • 46:37The age span range from 2 to 15 years,
  • 46:40but the majority of animals were
  • 46:43in adulthood.
  • 46:45An the hear their relative rank in
  • 46:48their social groups range from 0.05
  • 46:50lowest on the totem pole to 1.0 highest.
  • 46:54Alright,
  • 46:54so for both health and social status,
  • 46:57the only significant effects again
  • 46:59war for Mother period offspring
  • 47:01whose mothers were themselves
  • 47:02mother reared for good health.
  • 47:04This resulted in an increase of 6.7
  • 47:07percentage points and for social rank.
  • 47:10This resulted in an increase of .17 points,
  • 47:13which is pretty remarkable
  • 47:14considering the scope of the scale.
  • 47:17Here I should say there were
  • 47:19no no significant effects for
  • 47:22other rearing sequences.
  • 47:24So the big question is,
  • 47:26is the difference in
  • 47:27intergenerational effects Mother
  • 47:28period versus nursery offspring?
  • 47:30Is that statistically significant?
  • 47:32Well,
  • 47:32we formally tested this again
  • 47:35by measuring intergenerational
  • 47:37complimentarity and the answer is yes.
  • 47:39We found a strong and significant
  • 47:42intergenerational complementarity such
  • 47:44that the effect of secure attachment
  • 47:47is more beneficial for offspring whose
  • 47:50mothers also had a secure attachment.
  • 47:52And the effect was as follows.
  • 47:55Mother periods,
  • 47:56offspring time and good health was
  • 47:58increased by 9.7% versus nurture
  • 48:00offspring and socialrank was
  • 48:02increased by .24 points more and
  • 48:05then for nursery reared offspring.
  • 48:07Now because only mother period offspring
  • 48:09can interact with their mothers.
  • 48:11But nursery monkeys cannot.
  • 48:13Any positive effects on Mother Period.
  • 48:16Offspring here must be post needle
  • 48:18because the in uteral prenatal
  • 48:21effects were the same for both groups.
  • 48:24So we therefore conclude that
  • 48:26the improved parenting received
  • 48:27by Mother Period offspring,
  • 48:29because their own mothers had
  • 48:31secure attachments early in life.
  • 48:33This improved parenting is the
  • 48:35primary channel through which
  • 48:36early life advantage in the
  • 48:38parents generation in the form of
  • 48:40a secure attachment is transmitted to
  • 48:43the next generation versus prenatal in
  • 48:45utero effects an it's clear now that
  • 48:48these affect start very early in life,
  • 48:50as evidenced by survival rates
  • 48:52and persist until adulthood,
  • 48:54as evidenced by socialrank.
  • 48:58So these first findings on intergenerational
  • 49:01effects of early advantage have
  • 49:04really important implications.
  • 49:05There is strong importance to ensure that
  • 49:09all children experience secure attachments,
  • 49:11particularly children who start
  • 49:14out life in with disadvantage.
  • 49:17These findings also really underscore
  • 49:19the need for investments into parents
  • 49:22and families not just into children or
  • 49:25infants so that families can engage
  • 49:27in forming secure attachments with
  • 49:29their children and when parents and
  • 49:32caregivers and families are denied,
  • 49:34denied. Access to.
  • 49:37Abilities to enable them to
  • 49:40meet their basic needs,
  • 49:42they just don't have the
  • 49:44resources to Additionally be able
  • 49:46to form the secure bonds.
  • 49:47So when they're denied
  • 49:49extended paid parental leave,
  • 49:50both mothers and fathers when they
  • 49:52don't have access to high quality early
  • 49:55childhood education an also by the way,
  • 49:58when early educators are
  • 50:00not paid appropriately.
  • 50:01When people don't have a living
  • 50:03wage and when they lack food,
  • 50:05housing and health security,
  • 50:06you can begin to see how it could be
  • 50:09much harder to invest the resources
  • 50:11needed in forming secure attachments that
  • 50:14those of us with more privileged means have.
  • 50:18So what's next?
  • 50:19Well,
  • 50:20with current and future funding,
  • 50:21we aim to identify the biological
  • 50:24mechanisms of secure attachments.
  • 50:25At least some of them,
  • 50:27and so with this current NSF funding
  • 50:30in collaboration with Michael Kobor
  • 50:31at University of British Columbia,
  • 50:34we're going to look at DNA
  • 50:36metalation across generations.
  • 50:37As one of these under the skin
  • 50:39mechanisms an we're also going to begin
  • 50:42probing in inflammation both within a
  • 50:44single generation an across generations,
  • 50:46and I've just recently been awarded.
  • 50:49This pilot award from the animal
  • 50:51models for the social dimensions of
  • 50:53health and Aging Research Network,
  • 50:55which is a National Institute
  • 50:57of Aging Research Network,
  • 50:58to start looking at a chronic
  • 51:00inflammation across the life course,
  • 51:02and I plan in the next few months to
  • 51:05apply for further funding from the
  • 51:07foundations and institutes you see here,
  • 51:10including one opportunity.
  • 51:11I'm really excited about.
  • 51:12Again from NIH.
  • 51:13It's for early stage investigators
  • 51:15using nonhuman primate models.
  • 51:17And in addition to these two DNA,
  • 51:20metalation information will
  • 51:22obviously also be probing for
  • 51:24other biological mechanisms.
  • 51:26I also want to take a moment to talk
  • 51:29about how we're expanding our hair.
  • 51:32Cortisol or chronic stress studies
  • 51:34so we're acquiring equipment now to
  • 51:37set up a lab to be able to run her
  • 51:39cortisol assays here in house at Yale,
  • 51:41and I'm really bummed because
  • 51:43right before Pandemic hit and we
  • 51:45were required to go into lockdown,
  • 51:47I was just preparing an IRB protocol
  • 51:49to study with a colleague and
  • 51:52collaborator University of Virginia.
  • 51:53A different type of early life
  • 51:55experience and that is bearable.
  • 51:57Educational environments,
  • 51:58so we're going to compare a children
  • 52:01in public Montessori schools
  • 52:04with standard public education.
  • 52:06Pedagogy and look at see how their
  • 52:08hair cortisol change across the
  • 52:10year and how that influenced their
  • 52:12academic and social emotional outcomes.
  • 52:14So we hope to resume this once
  • 52:16it's safe to do so with additional
  • 52:18colleagues at University of Chicago,
  • 52:20we just submitted a research grant,
  • 52:22a small pilot award to look at
  • 52:25the hair cortisol of black mothers
  • 52:27whose prenatal and post Natal care
  • 52:29has been disrupted due to covid.
  • 52:32And with colleagues here at
  • 52:34the Yale School of Nursing,
  • 52:35we're about to submit a grant
  • 52:38to the National Heart, Lung,
  • 52:40and Blood Institute to examine
  • 52:42how chronic stress may be,
  • 52:44what role it may have in a sleep
  • 52:46disorders and cardiovascular function.
  • 52:48But one could also envision
  • 52:50studying other hormones in her.
  • 52:52It's possible to measure,
  • 52:54for example, progesterone,
  • 52:55estradiol and testosterone,
  • 52:56and maybe some of you have an interest
  • 53:00in studying other elements in her.
  • 53:02That might measure environmental
  • 53:03exposures like trace elements or
  • 53:05heavy metals or tobacco exposure.
  • 53:07So if any of these sound interesting to you,
  • 53:10please get in touch with me after the talk.
  • 53:15And so with that I would like to
  • 53:17conclude by acknowledging our funders
  • 53:19have made all of this work possible
  • 53:21today that I've shared with you,
  • 53:23along with the numerous people who work
  • 53:25behind the scenes to present these data.
  • 53:28And I would be remiss if I did not
  • 53:30acknowledge my own caregivers who gave
  • 53:32me the best form of secure attachment
  • 53:34from the moment I was born two months
  • 53:37early as a preemie who could fit in
  • 53:39a shoe box all the way up until now.
  • 53:42Not only do they provide me with
  • 53:44the secure loving base, but.
  • 53:46Pretty good peer interactions
  • 53:47with my sister too.
  • 53:49So with that I'm going to leave you
  • 53:51with one more quote by Harry Harlow,
  • 53:54perhaps, and in which he was describing.
  • 53:57An anecdotal behavior he had seen where
  • 54:00a monkey used a stick as a weapon.
  • 54:03Perhaps we should be criticized for
  • 54:05not reporting these behaviors earlier,
  • 54:06but in my defense,
  • 54:08let me state that in the mid 30s,
  • 54:10comparative psychology had attained
  • 54:11a level of objectivity that made
  • 54:13reporting such behavior a matter
  • 54:15of grave risk to the reputation
  • 54:16of any ambitious young scientists.
  • 54:18And it is better to become famous
  • 54:21slowly than to be right all at once.
  • 54:24Alright, so that is it.
  • 54:26Thank you very much all for being
  • 54:29here and and especially for you know,
  • 54:32enlightening me in here and let me
  • 54:34regale you with my monkey tails.
  • 54:37So thank you. Amanda, thank you so much.
  • 54:42We are out of time,
  • 54:43but we're going to leave them open and
  • 54:45I'll stick around and whomever wants
  • 54:47to stick around Papparazi's questions
  • 54:48if you need to leave, that's fine.
  • 54:50If you want to stay hang out but Amanda,
  • 54:53thank you. I will be so much.
  • 54:55Yes,
  • 54:55thank you so much for the invitation on
  • 54:57race and I really hope to see as many of
  • 54:59you as possible in person again soon.
  • 55:02Thank you.