Skip to Main Content

Population Neuroscience and Public Health

May 03, 2023

YCSC Grand Rounds April 18, 2023

Henning Tiemeier, MA, MD, PhD, Professor of Social and Behavioral Science and the Sumner and Esther Feldberg Chair of Maternal and Child Health - Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health

ID
9893

Transcript

  • 00:05Good afternoon, everyone, and welcome
  • 00:07to Grand Ryans and especially to
  • 00:09everyone joining us on Zoom.
  • 00:11And I'd like to remind you that for the Q&A,
  • 00:13please feel free to put on your video
  • 00:15cameras and we'll project you here on
  • 00:17our screens here in the Cohen Auditorium
  • 00:20and we'll hope for a lively discussion.
  • 00:22Now as usual, we just want to preview
  • 00:23a couple of our presentations that are
  • 00:25coming up over the next couple of weeks.
  • 00:26And so next Tuesday,
  • 00:28we will hear from Doctor Jessica Cardena.
  • 00:30And this is a very special by
  • 00:32Ola Barnard lecture series.
  • 00:33And so Doctor Cardeno will be talking to
  • 00:36us about what we can learn from Latino
  • 00:38mothers and what Latino mothers can
  • 00:40teach clinicians about trauma and recovery.
  • 00:43And then a special date for
  • 00:45your diary on Monday.
  • 00:46And we have Doctor Tracy Bale
  • 00:48coming to give a seminar in the
  • 00:51Division of Reproductive Sciences.
  • 00:53So that's in the department of OB GYN,
  • 00:55my other home department.
  • 00:56And so on Monday from 12:00 to 1:00,
  • 00:59Doctor Bale will be coming to talk
  • 01:01to us about extracellular vesicles
  • 01:02as a novel form of communication
  • 01:05between the mother and the fetus.
  • 01:07And as you'll all know,
  • 01:07Doctor Bale has done some seminal
  • 01:09work trying to uncover the molecular
  • 01:11mechanisms that underpin the
  • 01:13intergenerational transmission of stress.
  • 01:15And now to our speaker for today,
  • 01:17it is my distinct pleasure to welcome
  • 01:19Doctor Hennington Meyer to the Child
  • 01:21Study Center for the very first time,
  • 01:22I'm told.
  • 01:23And we did a little bit of history of
  • 01:25the Child Study Center earlier on and a.
  • 01:26Tour So Doctor Timmeyer is joining us
  • 01:29from the Harvard School of Public Health,
  • 01:31where he is the Professor of Social
  • 01:33and Behavioral Science and holds
  • 01:35the Sumner and Esther Feldberg
  • 01:36Chair of Maternal and Child Health,
  • 01:38where he also directs the Maternal
  • 01:40and Child Center for Excellence at
  • 01:42Harvard School of Public Health.
  • 01:44And, of course, Dr.
  • 01:45Timmeyer also holds a professorship at
  • 01:47the Erasmus University in Rotterdam,
  • 01:50where, as many of you know,
  • 01:51he set up the Generation Rotterdam cohort,
  • 01:54the Gen. R cohort.
  • 01:56Which has made a tremendous
  • 01:58contribution to our understanding of
  • 02:00how the environment shapes individual
  • 02:02differences in child development.
  • 02:03And I hope we'll hear a little
  • 02:04bit about that today,
  • 02:05as well as many of the other
  • 02:07initiatives that Doctor Tiamar is
  • 02:09involved in since moving to Harvard.
  • 02:11And of course,
  • 02:12he has published prolifically and is
  • 02:14regarded as a ISI highly cited researcher.
  • 02:16So please join me in giving
  • 02:18a warm child study.
  • 02:19Welcome to Doctor Tiameyer.
  • 02:20Thank
  • 02:24you. Thank you very much.
  • 02:27Let me put on my mic and thank
  • 02:29you very much for the kind,
  • 02:31very kind and warm introduction
  • 02:34and the invitation to come here.
  • 02:36Indeed, I'm quite proud to talk here.
  • 02:38I should say that because just teach
  • 02:41currently again the the course child
  • 02:44Psychiatric EPI at Harvard and on
  • 02:47my third slide there I show the Yale
  • 02:49Study Center and the work of gazelle,
  • 02:52which I think shaped longitudinal studies.
  • 02:56More than many others or anybody
  • 02:59else was that introduction.
  • 03:00For those that are also interested in
  • 03:03more recent work I'm doing or more other
  • 03:05work on the maternal child space space,
  • 03:07I must disappoint you or focus on
  • 03:10generation R still doing much of my work.
  • 03:12What I did is I okay is I selected work
  • 03:18from ongoing studies or older studies even
  • 03:22because I do much population or imaging.
  • 03:24I'll show you.
  • 03:25And the theme I thought was answering
  • 03:28an all discussion saying this work
  • 03:30should do now that you're at the
  • 03:31School of public health is not really
  • 03:33relevant to public health at all.
  • 03:35And after 20 years or 30 years of imaging
  • 03:38research, it's still not relevant.
  • 03:41And that doesn't insult me.
  • 03:43I think it's a fair critique,
  • 03:44but at least I have to live with
  • 03:45it and address it.
  • 03:46And that's what I'm trying to do
  • 03:47with you today. Discuss it with you.
  • 03:49Could it be relevant?
  • 03:50It's not so obvious.
  • 03:53So yes,
  • 03:54they asked me to do learning objectives.
  • 03:56So here you are a bit,
  • 03:57it's a bit about the prenet exposures,
  • 03:59which I'll Kieran is working on.
  • 04:01So I'll focus on that.
  • 04:02And the question really is,
  • 04:04is it identified?
  • 04:05I don't think that's the
  • 04:07learning objective to be honest.
  • 04:08It would be discussed with me how
  • 04:10child imaging might possibly in
  • 04:13theory a bit impact public health.
  • 04:16What am I talking about?
  • 04:18I see Euroscience population of science
  • 04:20not as broad as somebody like Thomas Powells.
  • 04:23Thomas Powers, I would see.
  • 04:24It's really the intersection of,
  • 04:26if you wish,
  • 04:28population research or etymology
  • 04:29and neuroscience.
  • 04:30Essentially that's what happened in genetics,
  • 04:32that genetics has been now 1520 years really
  • 04:35infused with genetics as we just talked,
  • 04:38epidemiology,
  • 04:38but now also influences
  • 04:40epidemiology with new methods.
  • 04:43And then I'll focus on prenatal exposures,
  • 04:45psychosocial or chemicals.
  • 04:46I've got one more chemical
  • 04:48exposure pull that up after I met.
  • 04:51Somebody yesterday night,
  • 04:53I thought that's a good one,
  • 04:57how that impacts child development.
  • 04:59I'll start with what I think
  • 05:01is not public health relevant.
  • 05:02So I thought I'll start with
  • 05:04something where I think it's not
  • 05:08what imaging research is not and I
  • 05:09start with not other people's work.
  • 05:11That's not very cool.
  • 05:12I start with my own work,
  • 05:14so I'll show you my.
  • 05:16Were my best publication last
  • 05:17year or one of my nicest,
  • 05:19but I don't think it is any
  • 05:21public health relevance.
  • 05:22It's answering the question child psychiatry.
  • 05:26Really it's giving you an example
  • 05:27of that because much of my work
  • 05:29or all of my work was funded under
  • 05:32the premise that it will inform in
  • 05:34the prediction and the causality
  • 05:36of child psychiatric disorders.
  • 05:37And now 20, not 15 years later,
  • 05:41what have we delivered?
  • 05:43It's this type of work.
  • 05:45Can we really predict adolescent
  • 05:47hallucinations with imaging?
  • 05:49Does it add anything?
  • 05:50So last year we published work on
  • 05:53this question, Public Health Relevant.
  • 05:55You ask yourself,
  • 05:56can we predict adolescent hallucinations
  • 05:58would be very, very important.
  • 06:00We measured that in Generation
  • 06:02R at 10 and 14 years.
  • 06:05It's actually quite easy to measure.
  • 06:07You can ask the adolescents themselves,
  • 06:09You can ask them to hear voices.
  • 06:12You have strange thoughts.
  • 06:15I don't know if anybody here has an idea
  • 06:17how prevalent that is at age 10, At 14,
  • 06:20Any idea if it's a fringe thing happening
  • 06:22at 2% of the population or 10 or 15%?
  • 06:25But actually if you ask them,
  • 06:29do you hear voices, it's 25% easily.
  • 06:32And that is not just waking up
  • 06:35after dreaming, it is really work of
  • 06:39Keleha and Mary Cannon in Ireland.
  • 06:42Has shown it's somewhat less frequent,
  • 06:44so it goes down to 15% if you wish.
  • 06:46If you really get them bothered by voices,
  • 06:5029 is really what you get with these
  • 06:52population assessments if you do it crudely.
  • 06:54But trust me, it is easily 15% at
  • 06:57age 10 and then it drops to 12%.
  • 06:59And again, if you do it more carefully,
  • 07:01it would probably be six, 7% at age 14.
  • 07:03That hear voices,
  • 07:05which is huge, don't forget.
  • 07:07Don't confuse that with schizophrenia.
  • 07:09That's nowhere near schizophrenia.
  • 07:10Actually, if you know their work,
  • 07:12it predicts depression, anxiety,
  • 07:15borderline much more than schizophrenia.
  • 07:18And we did,
  • 07:19so that's the special thing.
  • 07:20We did repeated imaging at age 10 and age
  • 07:2414 so we can show does the brain change.
  • 07:27We can also say, can we predict it?
  • 07:30In the paper in Biological
  • 07:32Psychiatry last year we showed
  • 07:33something after all lots of studies,
  • 07:37different approaches,
  • 07:38different work with the brains.
  • 07:40What we found really is if you hire voices,
  • 07:44then the typical decline,
  • 07:47this is sort of exaggerated.
  • 07:48This is a bad curve.
  • 07:49It should be much more than sort
  • 07:50of trajectory curve.
  • 07:51But forgive me for that that the
  • 07:53decline in Gray matter which.
  • 07:55Originates probably much earlier than age 10,
  • 07:57probably age 6 onwards is a tiny
  • 08:00bit far faster in those that have
  • 08:09new onset hallucinations at age 14.
  • 08:15I'm showing this.
  • 08:17It is an association.
  • 08:18It has a tiny effect size.
  • 08:21You need a few thousands 2000s to
  • 08:25find it as a tiny effect size.
  • 08:28It is a specific,
  • 08:30it is much of your Gray matter,
  • 08:33and actually it also maps on
  • 08:35other psychiatric problems,
  • 08:36so it would not be that very
  • 08:39specific for hallucinations.
  • 08:40You can also zoom in and find other
  • 08:42structures, of course we did that.
  • 08:43And the hippocampus,
  • 08:46that's what the small A says
  • 08:48it's The effect is again small.
  • 08:51It survives multiple testing,
  • 08:52correction for other structures.
  • 08:53It's a tiny effect.
  • 08:54Again, it is unspecific.
  • 08:56The conclusion here is useless.
  • 08:59As a predictor,
  • 09:00I have little doubt over and above any
  • 09:03prediction model which we published.
  • 09:06These brain imaging does nothing.
  • 09:07You can better do predict
  • 09:10with socioeconomic factors,
  • 09:12better predict with clinical factors.
  • 09:14You can better predict with well-being.
  • 09:17It does not predict and this is
  • 09:19the biggest imaging study so far.
  • 09:21So it may be that one day all of
  • 09:23you will search for more specific
  • 09:25markers and we'll do resting
  • 09:26state analysis and whatever.
  • 09:28But we had this unique data
  • 09:30set with repeated imaging and
  • 09:32repeated hallucinations over
  • 09:33the really relevant period.
  • 09:35I would give this a one out of five
  • 09:38in population public health relevance.
  • 09:41It does not add to any child
  • 09:43psychiatric clinicians.
  • 09:47Addiction, therapeutic,
  • 09:49understanding model.
  • 09:50I would say we've done
  • 09:53lots of this type of work.
  • 09:54It's fascinating, it's fun.
  • 09:55I think it's important to understand
  • 09:58that the brain can predict,
  • 10:00but it is not clinically useful.
  • 10:02Let me go on with transition to the work.
  • 10:05I'm going to show where I think we can
  • 10:08discuss public health relevance And again,
  • 10:12this is a crude analysis, I know that.
  • 10:14Actually more fine grain didn't predict,
  • 10:16more if you do multiple testing
  • 10:18correction and the prediction was small.
  • 10:21This is a paper I'm not going to discuss,
  • 10:22I'm just going to recommend it for your read.
  • 10:24From last year we said thought
  • 10:26it harder that the population of
  • 10:28science is the best paper of the year
  • 10:30and so it got our prize for that,
  • 10:33whoever cares.
  • 10:35And what it does is it uses the biggest
  • 10:40databases like the UK Biobank and
  • 10:42the ABCD studies and others to show.
  • 10:44That for a
  • 10:49if you don't zoom in on our ones
  • 10:52but you take a broader approach
  • 10:55for resting state and for volumes
  • 10:59that you need to find anything.
  • 11:02They say it's three to 6000 people in
  • 11:05the general population to find anything
  • 11:08you can argue in your clinical sample
  • 11:10it's different there's a letter or.
  • 11:12An answer to nature arguing that very
  • 11:15recently I actually fundamentally
  • 11:16disagree with that letter.
  • 11:18I think they have it right.
  • 11:20It's my own experience too,
  • 11:23and the only thing I'm not so sure,
  • 11:26and that's the judgment that's out.
  • 11:28This analysis is clearly only cross-sectional
  • 11:33and actually I'm not so interested
  • 11:36in cross-sectional prediction.
  • 11:37So we would have to move to longitudinal
  • 11:39and if you've got repeated brain measures,
  • 11:41I would argue because you control
  • 11:44for quite a bit and you have change
  • 11:47that could be different, although we
  • 11:49don't know what's the interval change.
  • 11:51Secondly, they have very poor phenotypes.
  • 11:54You could argue,
  • 11:55I think they should have used multiple
  • 11:57informant and other approaches, but anyway,
  • 11:59it was all that critique I think.
  • 12:02It's very humbling that all of a sudden
  • 12:04after so many years where we had studies
  • 12:06of 1520 people and found big effects,
  • 12:09we now have people that say if we want
  • 12:11to do it well, we need 3 to 5000.
  • 12:15So I would argue in child psychiatry so far
  • 12:18without very few exceptions you can think of,
  • 12:21you know, but very rare syndromes,
  • 12:24not so sure OCD, there's some debate that
  • 12:26that's quite specific, but otherwise.
  • 12:29I think it's poor discrimination,
  • 12:31poor specificity, poor sensitivity.
  • 12:33We've done machine learning.
  • 12:35I'm not talking about that.
  • 12:36To overcome that,
  • 12:37what we find is a very small signal and
  • 12:39actually something we already knew.
  • 12:41It's quite broad changes in
  • 12:44externalizing behaviors,
  • 12:45nothing very specific either.
  • 12:47So I think in sharp psychiatry,
  • 12:49my research has not contributed
  • 12:51that much for public health.
  • 12:56That does not mean it's useless, of course.
  • 12:58I would like to discuss prenatal exposures,
  • 13:00some old work and then zoom in more
  • 13:02recent work, ongoing work even.
  • 13:05And we identify important introduction
  • 13:07influences on the Turtle Shine house.
  • 13:09And we've done a lot over the years.
  • 13:11We sort of in generation are measured as
  • 13:14much as we could and we were quite creative.
  • 13:17We've got environmental toxins,
  • 13:18we've got thyroid poverty.
  • 13:20That's the recent thing that I added to
  • 13:22the list because I was interested in that.
  • 13:24Depression.
  • 13:25So the bold ones are zooming on today.
  • 13:27One or two of you will know earlier work,
  • 13:30but poverty is very recent and the
  • 13:33environmental stuff is just out last year.
  • 13:37So that's cool.
  • 13:38Discuss with me what you think is the
  • 13:41role of imaging, which is so well funded.
  • 13:43You know, if you take the European
  • 13:45funding in the neuroscience,
  • 13:47probably 1/2 goes to brain imaging,
  • 13:50which is shoot.
  • 13:52Not as much in the US
  • 13:54interestingly relatively speaking,
  • 13:55but a lot Okay just a bit about
  • 13:59Generation R as a prospective cohort.
  • 14:00It started in early fetal life but
  • 14:02early the inclusion we promised
  • 14:04and we had funding for 10,000,
  • 14:06I don't know for whatever reason the
  • 14:07end of the year came and we had to stop.
  • 14:08So we didn't manage the 10,000
  • 14:10but we got close,
  • 14:11it's 10,000 if you know who's active.
  • 14:13It's still more than 5000 are
  • 14:15contributing participating 6000 which
  • 14:17is very good if you start prenatally.
  • 14:19I think it's much better in a way
  • 14:21than ABCD because they have a 15%
  • 14:23response rate at baseline or lower.
  • 14:25So this is a 62% response rate
  • 14:28and then the Dutch majority group,
  • 14:30it's actually 70%.
  • 14:31So it's more selective in minorities,
  • 14:34it's urban and multiethnic and I do
  • 14:36because I have a slide later on this
  • 14:38ethnicity normally I sort of gloss over it.
  • 14:40Note that if you're on Rotterdam,
  • 14:42it's not much different than in many of
  • 14:46the Americans cities that about half.
  • 14:48Of the population is Dutch means
  • 14:53that has Dutch ancestry origin
  • 14:5610% would be other Europeans,
  • 14:57So that's expats largely.
  • 15:00And then you've got both migrant
  • 15:02or guest worker I should say,
  • 15:04which are the Turkish for example,
  • 15:06and the Moroccans.
  • 15:07And then you've got colonial history,
  • 15:09people like tsunamis, Cape roses,
  • 15:11also guest workers,
  • 15:12but Dutch Antilles.
  • 15:13Are ex colonies of the Netherlands where
  • 15:16people could migrate easily into meaning.
  • 15:19It's a very dangerous city.
  • 15:20And yeah, that's important because we'll
  • 15:24talk about poverty just about the measures.
  • 15:28I have no pointer,
  • 15:29but I have a cursor,
  • 15:30I'm told,
  • 15:31so I don't want to go through measures.
  • 15:33Nothing is more boring than telling
  • 15:34you what we measured in the study,
  • 15:36but we measured a lot ultrasound
  • 15:37in the beginning questionnaires,
  • 15:39lots of motor development was exciting.
  • 15:41We have IQ measures but also actually
  • 15:43of the parents which are the mother.
  • 15:44It's very important to control
  • 15:46for baseline confounding.
  • 15:48If you have intrauterine infectors,
  • 15:49what is sort of genetic
  • 15:52background and then the imaging,
  • 15:54I'll focus much of my talk.
  • 15:56On the imaging at age 9 to
  • 15:5810 which is at 4000 people.
  • 16:01I have one study later where we do a
  • 16:03follow up of the imaging which we have.
  • 16:05This is actually a typo.
  • 16:06It shouldn't be 4050.
  • 16:08This should be 3 thousands
  • 16:10and 52 hundred 3200.
  • 16:11Just copy pasted the wrong thing here.
  • 16:14So we have now three wave
  • 16:16completed and the 4th wave ongoing.
  • 16:17In total it would be 6000 different
  • 16:20individuals that have been scanned
  • 16:22of 5500 and the overlap is not that
  • 16:24big but it is there to do nice multi
  • 16:27level analysis over three waves already.
  • 16:30I would like to start with my
  • 16:31classical one of my classical
  • 16:33papers maternal depression.
  • 16:34So I think there we can learn a
  • 16:36bit about public health relevance
  • 16:37and actually I'm saying that also
  • 16:40because it informed a study or
  • 16:42work that I'm doing currently.
  • 16:43Maternal depression from fetal life forward.
  • 16:46What I'm trying to show you is
  • 16:48that we've measured maternal
  • 16:50depressive symptoms at three time
  • 16:53points during pregnancy.
  • 16:54This would have been forgive me
  • 16:56that the error is not quite good.
  • 16:58It should be after birth at two months,
  • 17:00after birth at three years.
  • 17:03We didn't use that and we used
  • 17:04it at 9 to 10 years.
  • 17:06So 4 measures of maternal depression 1/2
  • 17:09just after birth in the early childhood and.
  • 17:13At 10,
  • 17:14why is 10 interesting?
  • 17:15That is interesting because that
  • 17:16is cross-sectional if you wish.
  • 17:17Was the brain imaging,
  • 17:19focusing on the brain imaging at
  • 17:2110 years when we measured 4000
  • 17:23children and not all in study
  • 17:24at the end there will always be
  • 17:26only 2000 or 3000 in the study,
  • 17:28but that's at that time a very big study.
  • 17:31Certainly the biggest study was
  • 17:34prenatal exposure assessment,
  • 17:36prospective prenatal exposure assessment.
  • 17:40And I always ask when I see the
  • 17:42slides and those who have not seen it,
  • 17:44what time is there a strongest relation
  • 17:49of maternal depressive symptoms to
  • 17:51the brain of a child measured at age 10?
  • 17:57So we've got it at during pregnancy,
  • 18:00just after birth, early childhood
  • 18:04and cross-sectional with the MRI.
  • 18:08And the question is when?
  • 18:11Is there a relation between the
  • 18:15maternal depressive symptoms
  • 18:17and the volume and connectivity
  • 18:21of the child brain at age 10?
  • 18:24So is there a long term influence
  • 18:27from prenatal life forward?
  • 18:29Is there an influence of early
  • 18:32after birth perinatal depression?
  • 18:36Is an influence of childhood depression
  • 18:39or an influence of cross-sectional just
  • 18:44concurrent depression to the mother?
  • 18:47Talk about structure of the
  • 18:49brain of the child at age 10.
  • 18:55So I'm not, as I sometimes do,
  • 18:56pull somebody up and say what
  • 18:57do you think I'll do it myself.
  • 18:59You can think many people would think
  • 19:03it's either prenatal depression.
  • 19:05That has a big effect because that's
  • 19:07when the child is in the womb.
  • 19:09So you would think that the mother's
  • 19:12depression influences her Physiology,
  • 19:15and that impacts the child.
  • 19:18You could argue for just after the birth,
  • 19:21because that's a key period of attachment.
  • 19:25You could even argue somewhat
  • 19:26less for the childhood,
  • 19:28but you could argue for that because it's a
  • 19:30long period of childhood upbringing anyway.
  • 19:33If you look at this,
  • 19:35this is just very broad.
  • 19:36Total measures, Total white measure.
  • 19:38Total Gray measure.
  • 19:39Because we start with
  • 19:41hierarchical approaches,
  • 19:41doing big parts of the brain
  • 19:43and then zooming in on specific
  • 19:45regions if we find something.
  • 19:47You can look at these small effects.
  • 19:49They're actually translatable in centimeters.
  • 19:52Cubic.
  • 19:55You can see nothing was
  • 19:57the white matter much.
  • 19:58And if you look at the Gray matter.
  • 20:01There is a period,
  • 20:02two months where there is an effect and
  • 20:04again that survives multiple testing.
  • 20:06So if you want an answer from this,
  • 20:09it is not.
  • 20:10And I've said that many times,
  • 20:11for me, this is one of the
  • 20:12big it's a few years old now,
  • 20:14four years ago we published it.
  • 20:16It's
  • 20:19not the prenatal exposure that is most
  • 20:22important and we see that in some of this,
  • 20:24it is actually just after birth.
  • 20:28Where we see an effect and
  • 20:30that's actually very consistent.
  • 20:32So there's two ways to look at the data.
  • 20:36One is prenatal is not everything and
  • 20:39sometimes the Doha people would tell you.
  • 20:42Secondly, effects are small and if anything
  • 20:47that is a small effect postnatal depression,
  • 20:50which makes sense if you know
  • 20:53the literature and attachment,
  • 20:55maternal bonding and how important it is.
  • 20:57To have and how that is impacted
  • 20:59in clinically depressed mothers.
  • 21:01If you look at and I'll show later some,
  • 21:03I think some more DTI.
  • 21:04This is a slide of how we look at DTI.
  • 21:06We sort of don't integrate it all.
  • 21:08We look at different tracts
  • 21:10which we then sometimes sum.
  • 21:11So this would be the connectivity
  • 21:13in the white matter.
  • 21:14You measure that with two measures FA or MD,
  • 21:17but it essentially shows you
  • 21:18the integrity of the in these.
  • 21:21Different tracts.
  • 21:22We measured that. Well,
  • 21:23that was just the global brain measures.
  • 21:26And I can tell you this effect is
  • 21:28quite broad across parts of the brain.
  • 21:31So it's not just in the temporal
  • 21:32lobe or the frontal lobe.
  • 21:33We find it a global effect.
  • 21:35And then we also looked at the
  • 21:37DTI and what is interesting,
  • 21:39that's not so surprising.
  • 21:40Well, there was nothing in the white matter.
  • 21:42We saw that the tracts, the general tracts,
  • 21:46the integrity of the tracts.
  • 21:48Again, depression at 2:00.
  • 21:51Months Postnatally there was
  • 21:54less integrity of these tracts
  • 21:56together and trust me there's not
  • 21:58a single track that does it.
  • 21:59These global integrity of tracts is less,
  • 22:03is less clear is there's less
  • 22:06integrity in these tracts.
  • 22:08And then in fact was the depression at
  • 22:11two months on the child brain of 10 years.
  • 22:14So it's different exposure times was
  • 22:16one outcome time always at 10 years.
  • 22:18So you see the effect and there's
  • 22:19nothing Again,
  • 22:20there's prenatal
  • 22:25If we discuss public health relevance,
  • 22:29you will not want me to say we now
  • 22:33found that maternal depression is
  • 22:35important because there's fifty years
  • 22:37or 100 years of research to show that.
  • 22:39You might want to say, wow, he has a
  • 22:44way of finding sensitive periods.
  • 22:48And that's why I would think perhaps,
  • 22:51but really, honestly, I don't think so.
  • 22:54And I'll tell you why I tell you that.
  • 22:58And I know people in everywhere think
  • 23:01differently that you can with Social
  • 23:03adversity study sensitive periods.
  • 23:07I actually have tried to do that
  • 23:10now with measure of homelessness
  • 23:11and other work in my group.
  • 23:13We feel that is largely flawed.
  • 23:17Because of the following thing,
  • 23:19depression in mothers does not
  • 23:21occur in isolate meaning over time.
  • 23:24What I mean is that is a mother
  • 23:26that is depressed at two months
  • 23:28after birth has likely some elevated
  • 23:31symptoms already during pregnancy.
  • 23:33Not only likely, very likely,
  • 23:36meaning that all these poverty, abuse,
  • 23:41depression, all these risk factors,
  • 23:43all these social adversities are studying.
  • 23:46Have a high carry over and we cannot
  • 23:50validly or have seen very little studies
  • 23:54to validly study the period specific
  • 23:56exposure because then you would have
  • 23:58to have people that have it only in
  • 24:00this period and not in the others.
  • 24:01And if you see how carefully they
  • 24:04account for the other periods,
  • 24:06I can tell you in most models I've
  • 24:08seen that is flawed, including my own.
  • 24:11So I'll show you why it's flawed
  • 24:13and this is the trajectories.
  • 24:16It's flawed because the mothers who
  • 24:17have that peak of depressive symptoms
  • 24:19at two months were actually those
  • 24:21that were on average as a group.
  • 24:23If we just do these trajectories
  • 24:25and we classify them in groups
  • 24:26and we forget about that,
  • 24:27this is of course a continuum, this,
  • 24:29this series of continuum on that level.
  • 24:31But if we do them in four groups,
  • 24:32we see this group that actually I
  • 24:34can tell you carries the results,
  • 24:36has high levels here,
  • 24:37super high levels here and then keeps
  • 24:39on in the all these ten years after.
  • 24:44Assessments to be reasonably high because
  • 24:46this is 0.7 is exactly where the clinical
  • 24:49line of clinical severity would have been,
  • 24:51meaning that there is a group that has
  • 24:53clinical symptoms but they're high all over.
  • 24:55And of course there are
  • 24:56some that have only high.
  • 24:57When the children get older, only in
  • 24:59sort of childhood life they develop it.
  • 25:01It's a small group actually,
  • 25:03but the important thing is that.
  • 25:07These are so tied,
  • 25:08So to say that this is the unique effect
  • 25:11of this episode when they're far be above
  • 25:14clinical levels and others makes no sense.
  • 25:17It is because it's not like an infection.
  • 25:19It's not like a COVID infection where
  • 25:21you can say that during pregnancy
  • 25:23because you don't have continuous COVID,
  • 25:25well, not the infection probably
  • 25:27over 10 years is different.
  • 25:29I think it doesn't work.
  • 25:30We've done it with homelessness and then we
  • 25:32have a set where people experience only.
  • 25:34Short time and then find housing again
  • 25:36and if you have very detailed data I
  • 25:38think you can do that with poverty.
  • 25:40But people who are really below the
  • 25:43poverty line will have been mostly
  • 25:45in a tough spot a year or two later
  • 25:49or a year or two before.
  • 25:51So indeed that was consistent.
  • 25:53So there is this carry over effects,
  • 25:55there is these.
  • 25:57Perhaps there's a biological
  • 25:59rapid development post natally
  • 26:00there are sensitive peers,
  • 26:02there's good ideas.
  • 26:02I think we might be able to do that.
  • 26:04I'll show you later something with
  • 26:06the thyroid hormones where we managed
  • 26:07to do that with sensitive peers.
  • 26:08I think with social adversities
  • 26:11we cannot do that.
  • 26:12So if you judge this study
  • 26:15against public health relevance,
  • 26:17give me a two out of five because I
  • 26:20think the carry sort of the sensitive
  • 26:23period effects which I marketed as.
  • 26:25Don't convince me myself,
  • 26:28and I hope I don't convince you.
  • 26:31It's interesting,
  • 26:32but I don't think it should guide.
  • 26:35It did for a while,
  • 26:36influenced me that I thought, you know,
  • 26:37I have to put more of my research
  • 26:40time into very early depression.
  • 26:41I think that's still valid,
  • 26:43but I'm not so sure that we need
  • 26:46imaging research to show that.
  • 26:48I'll show you because it's very
  • 26:49popular now to do imaging and poverty.
  • 26:52I'll show you a bit of that result
  • 26:54and then an angle I tried to take
  • 26:56and I'm trying to hear your thoughts
  • 26:58or at least look at you whether it
  • 27:00might convince you what we did there.
  • 27:02So household income has been
  • 27:05associated with brain morphology.
  • 27:07We had this data prospectively from it.
  • 27:10Life again that's a sort of marketing trick.
  • 27:13So we show you that we did that and.
  • 27:16I was interested in two things, the timing.
  • 27:18So is it different if it's prenatal or later?
  • 27:21And I was also interested if it's
  • 27:23different in minority majority
  • 27:24and I'll come to that why I'm so
  • 27:27interested in that in a minute.
  • 27:28So if we have 2000 children against
  • 27:30imaging at 10 years poverty defined as
  • 27:33national low income threshold in the
  • 27:35Netherlands, that's nicely defined.
  • 27:36So you get different analyses, you can do it.
  • 27:40Never low income and ever low income.
  • 27:42Note that we have repeatedly assessed income,
  • 27:44So what people? You can just simply do it.
  • 27:46Have you ever in any period been
  • 27:49poor and we can do that chronic
  • 27:51or for example in pregnancy only.
  • 27:53And what you see is just the
  • 27:57distribution which made it for me,
  • 27:59made this a very complicated distribution
  • 28:03because in the Netherlands and you
  • 28:07see a very similar pattern in the US,
  • 28:09it's just not immigrant.
  • 28:12Or non western,
  • 28:14it's just classified as white and non white.
  • 28:17You would see a very similar pattern
  • 28:20that those that are poor are very often
  • 28:24from here from a non western background.
  • 28:26So there is a racial ethnic patterning
  • 28:29of poverty in the Netherlands.
  • 28:31There's a racial ethnic pattern
  • 28:34of poverty in the in America.
  • 28:38So you see that of the four hundreds,
  • 28:40quite a few that were poor,
  • 28:41so 20% were poor at one time a
  • 28:45majority would have been from
  • 28:47long Western and then we have,
  • 28:49you can see the numbers 100 people
  • 28:50that were poor in pregnancy,
  • 28:52100 and 200 that were poor at any one time.
  • 28:54So you can see the breakdown
  • 28:56of these numbers.
  • 28:59Here is so how it looks at truth.
  • 29:02You can see all the different
  • 29:03results that you know.
  • 29:04If you analyze, you get,
  • 29:05even if you take this broad approach of
  • 29:07total brain volume and Gray volume and
  • 29:09then the typical hippocampus, amygdala.
  • 29:11If you do this mix of global and
  • 29:14to specific areas, researchers,
  • 29:16regions of interest, you see with
  • 29:19these many poverty categorizations,
  • 29:21you see all these patterns and then you
  • 29:24can look where there's significance.
  • 29:27And honestly, you could find,
  • 29:29that's why I had it in red,
  • 29:31some association between the
  • 29:32amygdala volume and poverty.
  • 29:34And if you look at it carefully,
  • 29:35this is the reference group.
  • 29:37Never. Then you see what's this?
  • 29:38This is the low income childhood only.
  • 29:40There seems to be no effect.
  • 29:41But if you're chronically poor,
  • 29:43if you're chronic poor, or if you're.
  • 29:47So ever low income is cost,
  • 29:48a combination,
  • 29:48but it's really by low income
  • 29:50and pregnancy or chronic force.
  • 29:51So it really seems to be, if anything,
  • 29:53the pregnancy that might drive it,
  • 29:56the amygdala.
  • 29:57But I can truthfully tell you that this
  • 29:59does not survive multiple testing.
  • 30:01So there would be,
  • 30:03if anything overall in the total group,
  • 30:05no real.
  • 30:11Convincing or strong consistent effect?
  • 30:13Not on the global measures for sure
  • 30:15and on these regions of interest.
  • 30:16Well, if you find it somewhere
  • 30:18just borderline significant,
  • 30:20you should probably discount it.
  • 30:23However, we had very good data
  • 30:25from Child IQ that certainly the
  • 30:28pregnancy was very different in
  • 30:30minority groups and majority groups,
  • 30:33so we had reason from that
  • 30:35paper to stratify a sample in.
  • 30:38Let me call it Western or Nonwestern.
  • 30:40That's the Dutch, Dutch language in America.
  • 30:42Western on Western is not
  • 30:43really cool anymore,
  • 30:44So I'd rather should say
  • 30:48it's not immigrants,
  • 30:49it's people whose ancestors were born
  • 30:52in probably not high income countries
  • 30:55and came to the Netherlands for colonial
  • 30:57history reasons or work reasons,
  • 30:59and the Dutch and the Dutch and European.
  • 31:04Community on the other hand,
  • 31:07and why do I think that's a very
  • 31:09important difference in poverty?
  • 31:11Not only did we have some prior results,
  • 31:13but also we know that if you're
  • 31:15financially strained and you
  • 31:16have a network in the country,
  • 31:18that's a different thing if your
  • 31:20family lives there than if you come
  • 31:22as an immigrant from the Cape Verin
  • 31:24Islands to work in the harbour.
  • 31:25If you're then out of job then you're really,
  • 31:28it really is tough.
  • 31:30So that's why we stratified
  • 31:32for these groups and then.
  • 31:34We see if we do that and
  • 31:36this is only the Dutch,
  • 31:37we actually all of a sudden saw
  • 31:39very broad effects on cerebral and
  • 31:42other broad parameters of the brain.
  • 31:44So taking out this big group of
  • 31:48non Dutch ancestry participants,
  • 31:50let's call it that way, not Dutch ancestry,
  • 31:54but taking them out shoulders all of
  • 31:56a sudden we had a strong effect in.
  • 32:00The overall brain volume.
  • 32:02But what was perhaps more interesting
  • 32:04in the numb Dutch.
  • 32:05So this was the Dutch.
  • 32:06This is the numb Dutch.
  • 32:08We didn't see any global parameters,
  • 32:09but we see very consistently
  • 32:14the effects of in pregnancy or chronic,
  • 32:17which also means in pregnancy
  • 32:18and later on as well.
  • 32:20If you pull that group to sort of
  • 32:22ever in pregnancy, we get a very.
  • 32:24Very significant effect on consistent because
  • 32:27it's significant in both of the subgroupings.
  • 32:29If you pull it, it gets very
  • 32:31significant in effect on the amygdala.
  • 32:33So we get a very different pattern.
  • 32:35So we get a much more stress related
  • 32:38grain poverty pattern in the non Dutch
  • 32:41ancestry group and a very global effect.
  • 32:44It's very hard to think what that means.
  • 32:47Does that valid?
  • 32:47I can tell you I immediately
  • 32:49looked at the ABCD data.
  • 32:50Does it also fall apart in similar
  • 32:52patterning and again of course that would be.
  • 32:54Would be none, white and white,
  • 32:56probably what you could do.
  • 32:58And it's interesting,
  • 32:59we saw the same similar different complicated
  • 33:01pattern for the behavioral outcomes,
  • 33:02but not so much for the brain.
  • 33:04So there is some reason to think that if
  • 33:07poverty comes with different stresses,
  • 33:10it could have a different
  • 33:11meaning for the brain.
  • 33:12We see that very clearly for
  • 33:14the behaviour also in ABCD,
  • 33:15but not for the brain.
  • 33:17And I haven't looked at the amygdala.
  • 33:19As they make that and actually in the
  • 33:21Dutch this really predicted school
  • 33:22performance so it was meaningful.
  • 33:24So if you summarize early in life poverty
  • 33:27and pre adolescent brain morphology,
  • 33:30there is an association but they really
  • 33:32differ from majority and minority groups.
  • 33:34And was all the caveats that you
  • 33:36hate this subtyping of majority and
  • 33:37minority that's up to you to dislike it.
  • 33:40I think that is some evidence that we do it.
  • 33:42In America,
  • 33:43I would say we should do it to
  • 33:44some extent because poverty and
  • 33:46discrimination go together,
  • 33:47which makes a very different terrible mix.
  • 33:50In the Netherlands it is also discrimination
  • 33:52and stress of surviving financial strength.
  • 33:54So there is some reason to do that
  • 33:56and this I think what that reflects,
  • 33:59I'll be very,
  • 33:59very careful to speculate about that.
  • 34:01I think it could also be
  • 34:03genetically associated,
  • 34:04we don't know,
  • 34:06but in the in the minority groups
  • 34:09as I call them here or.
  • 34:11If you want the real nice terminology,
  • 34:13I think the exact terminology the
  • 34:16non Dutch and ancestry group.
  • 34:18I think it is likely stressed
  • 34:19by discrimination and because we
  • 34:21have that variable in the model I
  • 34:23can tell you pull it in and it's
  • 34:26substantially weakened the association.
  • 34:28So it's not a real mediation analysis,
  • 34:30but there is.
  • 34:31About 30% of the association and
  • 34:33that was a very crude measure of
  • 34:35discrimination disappeared once
  • 34:36we put that in the model.
  • 34:37So I think there's real reason to
  • 34:39think that could be different and we
  • 34:41have to think more carefully about
  • 34:43our neurodevelopmental measures.
  • 34:44I'll do the thyroid and then another,
  • 34:47I'll do that quickly.
  • 34:49I've presented that for many times.
  • 34:51So what would that be in if you give me.
  • 34:56My sort of scale,
  • 34:57my own scales is rating your
  • 34:59own work. But let's do it critical.
  • 35:01I think we're still only at a three out
  • 35:03of five of public health because to
  • 35:06think that poverty measures mentioned
  • 35:09poverty matters for the brain,
  • 35:11I don't think we need too much brain imaging.
  • 35:14But, you know, to carefully dissect the
  • 35:17effects of minority groups again, Well,
  • 35:20really, do we need the imaging for that?
  • 35:24Although I think that it has
  • 35:26lasting effects on child brains,
  • 35:27that it what is it affects is
  • 35:30associated with child brains may be
  • 35:32very different How it associates
  • 35:34with the brain where you come from,
  • 35:37It's at least makes us think so give
  • 35:39me a three out of five perhaps we want
  • 35:42to go to four out of five, don't we?
  • 35:44So here's thyroid where I think
  • 35:46we can manage thyroid.
  • 35:48Old work of mine and it was one recent
  • 35:51update which is I think quite spectacular.
  • 35:53Thyroid of the brain,
  • 35:55So note that the maternal thyroid brain.
  • 35:57So maternal thyroid hormones are very
  • 35:59important for the brain development.
  • 36:01Animal work has shown convincingly
  • 36:03that actually it's fascinating
  • 36:04that the neurogenesis and it's
  • 36:07particularly the neuro neuromigration
  • 36:08which actually comes from around,
  • 36:10you know the central,
  • 36:12the ventricles and then the neurons
  • 36:15migrate out to your cortex.
  • 36:17Obviously that's where they are
  • 36:19in our brains that is guided by.
  • 36:22Thyroid hormones that happens in
  • 36:26early embryonic life when the embryo
  • 36:30is reliant on the maternal thyroid.
  • 36:35So much of the neurodevelopment.
  • 36:37So the nature is seem sort of
  • 36:39very pasimonious.
  • 36:40It has only 10 mechanisms
  • 36:41and what does you know,
  • 36:43vitamin D does something and
  • 36:45serotonin do something very different
  • 36:46in the fetal life they're much
  • 36:49more neurodevelopmental than in
  • 36:50us where they have.
  • 36:51Very new endocrine function,
  • 36:52but they have very new
  • 36:54developmental functions.
  • 36:55All these systems and in particular
  • 36:57thyroid in pregnancy and only in
  • 37:00week 14 does then the fetus produce
  • 37:03its own thyroid and only by week 20,
  • 37:06so sometime later does it produce
  • 37:08somewhat sufficient levels and takes over.
  • 37:11So in that time the mother
  • 37:13supplies the thyroid.
  • 37:15In that time many women who have a low
  • 37:17thyroid function actually become a bit
  • 37:19hyperthyroid because they need more.
  • 37:21There's very good graphs.
  • 37:22I haven't got them with me because
  • 37:24I do a short version of this talk.
  • 37:25But trust me, there's very good work,
  • 37:27mostly animal work,
  • 37:28very consistent that we need the
  • 37:30thyroid levels for a brain development.
  • 37:33And what we showed in the very early
  • 37:35publications, nearly ten years ago now,
  • 37:37is that if you take the total sample,
  • 37:39and this is the measure of s s.
  • 37:44SRS,
  • 37:44the social responsiveness of good
  • 37:46population trait measure of autism,
  • 37:48you see that the people with
  • 37:49good levels of the mothers,
  • 37:51the offspring of mothers with normal
  • 37:53levels of thyroid hormone have much
  • 37:56lower levels than those that have subautimal.
  • 37:59And this is subclinical,
  • 38:00we're not talking about a clinical,
  • 38:01this is untreated hypothics,
  • 38:04thyroxinemia, you can do severe,
  • 38:05you can do less severe,
  • 38:06but it's all subclinical,
  • 38:08so it's just low levels.
  • 38:10Of thyroid hormone in the mother
  • 38:12and you saw that association which
  • 38:14we showed and then we move on
  • 38:16to more recent work,
  • 38:19a first Lancet endocrinology paper where
  • 38:22we showed that if we take the levels
  • 38:26continuous now FT-4 that's the thyroid.
  • 38:29So this means more thyroid,
  • 38:30this means less thyroid.
  • 38:32We showed actually a a
  • 38:34curvilinear association with IQ.
  • 38:36It's most robust in the
  • 38:38low thyroid levels here.
  • 38:40And then this is a quite
  • 38:41a wide confident role,
  • 38:41but you see some significant down decline.
  • 38:44So there is a tightly regulated level
  • 38:46and that's where most mothers are.
  • 38:48If you see the distribution of hormones,
  • 38:50it would be just most people are in
  • 38:52this space, some are in the low,
  • 38:55some are in the high.
  • 38:56And we saw a very robust relation with IQ.
  • 39:00And later we've replicated this in 2-3
  • 39:02other cohorts where I must be honest,
  • 39:04this ups this low levels of and the
  • 39:08relation to low IQ is extremely robust.
  • 39:10This in other cars looks more like this,
  • 39:12going sort of much more flat.
  • 39:13There's not such a decline,
  • 39:15but there is a very robust association
  • 39:19between prenatal thyroid hormones and I Q.
  • 39:23And then we move to another
  • 39:26hormone thyroid parameter.
  • 39:28So be careful.
  • 39:28This is now thyroid,
  • 39:29thyroid stimulating hormone.
  • 39:31This means that now you'd beware that
  • 39:33higher levels of the stimulating hormones
  • 39:36means lower levels of thyroid hormone.
  • 39:38It's flipped.
  • 39:39I think you have to be a doctor or
  • 39:41an endocrinologist or physiologist
  • 39:42who immediately get it.
  • 39:44But trust me whereas we had easy more
  • 39:47hormone is we thought better but this
  • 39:49is not the case because it gets worse here.
  • 39:51But this is more hormones.
  • 39:53This is less hormones and
  • 39:55less hormones means lower IQ.
  • 39:56Here it is two different things modeled.
  • 39:59It's not IQ, it's Gray matter.
  • 40:01So it's not a brain parameter.
  • 40:02And you see this is essentially flipped.
  • 40:06So this means.
  • 40:07Less hormones.
  • 40:08This would mean more hormones,
  • 40:10but I'm presenting at the stimulating
  • 40:12axis hormone and what you see is
  • 40:15the same similar inverted U-shaped
  • 40:18curve tightly regulated on all
  • 40:21levels of the thyroid between the
  • 40:24brain and the between the brain.
  • 40:27And the thyroid hormone and
  • 40:29it's highly significant.
  • 40:31So it's 2000 children at age 10,
  • 40:33it's their prenatal,
  • 40:35their mothers in the early
  • 40:37mostly around week 10 to 14,
  • 40:39it's their thyroid hormone levels.
  • 40:41And this has led to some guidelines
  • 40:43and discussion and guidelines.
  • 40:44Should we measure more thyroid
  • 40:46hormones in women that have no
  • 40:48symptoms and no history of and there
  • 40:51have been trials based on this work
  • 40:52which it have to have been largely
  • 40:54negative or very small effects.
  • 40:55So they're sort of.
  • 40:57Equivocal trials have been done,
  • 40:59so we don't know,
  • 41:00but there is some evidence that it is
  • 41:02a very important parameter to regulate.
  • 41:04And now comes the recent work.
  • 41:07I don't know,
  • 41:08I don't have a date when that
  • 41:09was published 2 years or so ago,
  • 41:11which is very fascinating.
  • 41:13We did that.
  • 41:14We just realized this data because
  • 41:16we had the idea what actually we
  • 41:19included the women at different ages.
  • 41:21So we can model always in about 200 women.
  • 41:25The curve essentially continuously
  • 41:28moving the curve with a time interaction
  • 41:32variable across the inclusion period.
  • 41:34So the first women came to
  • 41:37generation out to be included and
  • 41:39we took the blood at week seven.
  • 41:41The latest that we included
  • 41:43were week eighteen.
  • 41:44Note these are not the same women.
  • 41:46This is the first blood assessment we
  • 41:49had where we did the thyroid hormones.
  • 41:52So what we modeled it as a
  • 41:55sort of continuous model,
  • 41:56but then cut it for the doing
  • 41:59essentially the intercept for
  • 42:01the different week 7 to 18.
  • 42:03And what we see is that this curvy linear
  • 42:08pattern which is very remarked up to age,
  • 42:11then sort of disappears at the
  • 42:13end of this inclusion period.
  • 42:15And this was still 200 women on average.
  • 42:20Time period per week and what this
  • 42:22shows you I think is convincingly
  • 42:25a sensitive period because it
  • 42:27is in the same study measured at
  • 42:31different time points specifically.
  • 42:33And why is that so credible?
  • 42:34Because the reviews they sort
  • 42:36of were extremely excited.
  • 42:37I've never got anything in that sort
  • 42:39of Lancet like paper that easily
  • 42:41because as in chronologist said,
  • 42:43I've done animal work and I showed
  • 42:45by week 15 the child produces
  • 42:47on thyroid and thus the mother.
  • 42:49Thyroid is just not informative anymore.
  • 42:53So while I marketed as a final we
  • 42:56got their sensitive period study,
  • 43:00the reviewer toned it down to saying
  • 43:02it's really showing that the measure
  • 43:04is not informative At age 15.
  • 43:06It may still influence the brain,
  • 43:08but you're measuring the wrong parameter.
  • 43:10So this is getting closer to the
  • 43:12sensitive period. Holy Grail.
  • 43:14That's all these Doha epinologists
  • 43:15want to get to, but even there,
  • 43:17a very careful reviewer can tell.
  • 43:19Tell you you're not there.
  • 43:21It just means that from week 14 onwards
  • 43:23you're measuring the wrong person,
  • 43:25essentially like having the wrong informant.
  • 43:27But what does it tell you?
  • 43:30It tells you what.
  • 43:32I think that this is valid because how
  • 43:36could it's if you then have the right
  • 43:39measure and you find what you expected,
  • 43:41perhaps that's sort of a circumvential say.
  • 43:44I don't think it proves causality.
  • 43:46But it's getting better that
  • 43:48this is quite credible.
  • 43:49So I do think in all honesty there
  • 43:51is a true curvilinear relationship
  • 43:54between thyroid hormone and the brain.
  • 43:57I do think given the biology
  • 43:59it is likely to be causal.
  • 44:00Whether that's amenable for
  • 44:02intervention is another study.
  • 44:04I've got the wrong slides.
  • 44:06I was going to ask you so
  • 44:08transition to new results.
  • 44:10I missed my transition slide because I
  • 44:12pulled it up yesterday night after the.
  • 44:15Chemical exposure for the colleague.
  • 44:17I hope she's there on the zoom.
  • 44:21Does anybody know what trans fatty acids are?
  • 44:25Take a sip of coffee while you tell me.
  • 44:31Is that forgotten? You're not bisphosals and
  • 44:38organophosphates?
  • 44:39Which are about what?
  • 44:40Does anybody still know
  • 44:41what trans fatty acids are?
  • 44:47I'll tell you, trans fatty assets
  • 44:51in the Netherlands were a big scandal,
  • 44:55a public health scandal of big proportions.
  • 44:57Why? Because in the 1990s eighties,
  • 45:01I don't know, to that time,
  • 45:03your grandparents wouldn't have eaten butter.
  • 45:06Which they would have
  • 45:06lived in these countries.
  • 45:07And then there comes the introduction of
  • 45:09margarines which is better for public health?
  • 45:11Okay, it's better for your fat because
  • 45:14it's unsaturated and saturated.
  • 45:16Fatty Acids in butter versus margarine.
  • 45:18And these are people that eat,
  • 45:19you know bread butter and I don't know cheese
  • 45:24on twice a day.
  • 45:27And so the problem was that these
  • 45:30margarines where fatty acids,
  • 45:32but they also had trans fatty acids.
  • 45:35Meaning these are industrial fatty
  • 45:37acids which come with the production
  • 45:39of fat and essentially if you produce
  • 45:41fat and if you have sort of a if you
  • 45:44fry your French fries and you have very
  • 45:46poor fat in one of these, I don't know,
  • 45:49I don't want to point at any cart here,
  • 45:51but if you have very poor,
  • 45:52you get trans fatty acids in them.
  • 45:54And those were really,
  • 45:56it turned out to be terrible
  • 45:58for cardiovascular health,
  • 46:00actually so bad so that the whole
  • 46:02benefit of eating margarine was offset
  • 46:05by the effect of trans fatty assets.
  • 46:08It was a real scandal in the 1990s.
  • 46:11OK, It's sort of forgotten.
  • 46:13And I don't know,
  • 46:14and I don't know anything much about America.
  • 46:15My work is mostly from Europe.
  • 46:17So there what happened is
  • 46:19there were countries.
  • 46:20It's already interesting to see what
  • 46:21happens in countries once that's,
  • 46:23you know.
  • 46:23Detected that Hans very as a mouse
  • 46:26models and humans and observational and
  • 46:28is really bad and sort of kills you
  • 46:31the there's countries that forbid it.
  • 46:33Okay Denmark said gone two years
  • 46:35and we phase it out of production.
  • 46:38It's easy you can just make
  • 46:39a bit more expensive oils.
  • 46:42The Dutch you might not know them
  • 46:45are sort of compromising country
  • 46:47so they say to the industry
  • 46:49you know it would be good.
  • 46:52If you reduced it in your
  • 46:53products in the next five years,
  • 46:55we do that on a voluntary basis and we
  • 46:57will also do a bit of shaming and naming.
  • 46:59So there is some pressure.
  • 47:01That's the Dutch approach.
  • 47:02Now you would laugh about the Dutch,
  • 47:04but they do get it done so slowly, by slowly.
  • 47:07Uni Lever,
  • 47:08whom you know from the Dove products,
  • 47:10is a real big you know you Lever
  • 47:12is the modern maker in that time.
  • 47:15I don't know if they still do it and
  • 47:17they phased it out, which leaves.
  • 47:19Other products like um,
  • 47:22cheap bakery products where it's still
  • 47:24used because they couldn't care less.
  • 47:26You know, that's the fringe market
  • 47:28and they couldn't care.
  • 47:28It's cheap to do that.
  • 47:30So what we found, um, so here is.
  • 47:34If you want to know trans and sis
  • 47:36fatty acids, so this is the big difference.
  • 47:38Industrial fatty acids like trans would have
  • 47:41the hydrogen here instead of like this.
  • 47:43Wow, you think that's the difference?
  • 47:45That's it.
  • 47:45Yes, that's the difference.
  • 47:47That's it.
  • 47:47And where are they?
  • 47:48They're found in fried foods,
  • 47:50commercial bakers and processions.
  • 47:51But what happens in the statcha pros?
  • 47:53There was not a law to stop them,
  • 47:55but really,
  • 47:56in the early 2000s in this country, this.
  • 47:59And the changes in the Netherlands went down
  • 48:02in vegetable oils and fat in those years.
  • 48:05The production went down dramatically.
  • 48:08So without,
  • 48:09I'm not saying that's the best approach,
  • 48:10but both in the Netherlands
  • 48:12and Denmark and other countries
  • 48:14in Europe, they reduce these fatty acids.
  • 48:17And why is that? Why am I telling
  • 48:19you all this in an imaging study?
  • 48:21I'll tell you why.
  • 48:23It's really fascinating.
  • 48:24I saw this once and I thought these are.
  • 48:28The inclusion years of the Generation R study
  • 48:32we included from 2003 to actually to 2007,
  • 48:40we included in exactly the years when
  • 48:43trans fatty acids disappeared in the in
  • 48:49the food industry in the Netherlands.
  • 48:52That means we can look at.
  • 48:55The blood levels of women who came
  • 48:58at different times in those years,
  • 49:00and we did. And you know,
  • 49:01you'd think you'd see the same exact curve,
  • 49:03But you know,
  • 49:04we saw a 10/10 a quarter of decline,
  • 49:09which for anything in
  • 49:10biology is quite dramatic.
  • 49:12So in 2000,
  • 49:14the people who included in 2005 had only 3/4
  • 49:17of the levels of those included in 2002.
  • 49:21So it indeed related to a.
  • 49:25Reduction in the blood of a women and
  • 49:29I don't know if they knew and they
  • 49:30didn't change their eating behavior,
  • 49:32they just ate the same bread
  • 49:34and French fries as before,
  • 49:36but they got less of this.
  • 49:38Which means if we can relate
  • 49:41this to a child outcome,
  • 49:43we have something which we call
  • 49:46instrumental barrel approach
  • 49:47because it is a policy change.
  • 49:49That is related to biology
  • 49:51in the blood of people.
  • 49:53And so we published that last
  • 49:55year after sort of after I had
  • 49:56what I don't know how I came.
  • 49:57I come from a Baker's family to be honest,
  • 49:59I was, I don't know,
  • 50:00reading this in the sort of Baker thing
  • 50:02digest and I was quite fascinated
  • 50:04and I thought, yes, you can do that.
  • 50:06And so we did the.
  • 50:08Trans fatty acids in the blood and
  • 50:10then we showed and you know you
  • 50:12this is a very bad slides taken
  • 50:14directly from the publication.
  • 50:16But you can see a highly,
  • 50:17highly significant association of
  • 50:20trans fatty acids with fetal head growth.
  • 50:24And this is true head growth.
  • 50:25This is the change from fetal head
  • 50:27size from one point to the other.
  • 50:29It's not just growth and you say birth
  • 50:31weight is a measure of fetal growth.
  • 50:33This is really fetal growth
  • 50:34as it is a change from.
  • 50:362nd to 3rd trimester,
  • 50:38there was no effect when the head is very,
  • 50:41very small, but when it expands,
  • 50:42when it gets big,
  • 50:43that's where all the growth is.
  • 50:44And that second to third end of
  • 50:47trimester and 6000 children.
  • 50:48So that's a good inclusion.
  • 50:50We see super significant associations and
  • 50:52then we can actually do the same trick.
  • 50:54We can only do the, the,
  • 50:56the, the TFA measures.
  • 50:58We can do that with the high
  • 51:00and see very clear patterns,
  • 51:01but we can see that this calendar time.
  • 51:05There is an association of
  • 51:08calendar time with fetal growth,
  • 51:11meaning that in the course of
  • 51:15that studies the the heads of
  • 51:18the children became a tiny bit.
  • 51:21I must admit it's a tiny bit,
  • 51:22but fetal measures in 6000 are very precise,
  • 51:25bigger and we think and we can show that
  • 51:28was an instrumental viral approach,
  • 51:30which is a different sort
  • 51:32of statistical technique.
  • 51:33We can show that the association is
  • 51:37driven by the reduction and the policy
  • 51:41change and that is something I've been
  • 51:44working 20 years towards and never got done.
  • 51:46So that we show that policy translates
  • 51:50into biology and sad thing is we didn't
  • 51:54get it to behaviour, so bigger heads.
  • 51:55And I'm not really much related to behavior
  • 51:57and then it becomes very messy and noisy.
  • 51:59But you know,
  • 52:00the journal loved it that it was.
  • 52:02And why does it have
  • 52:04clinical health relevance?
  • 52:06Well, first of all, it does.
  • 52:07This is causality,
  • 52:08not only policy,
  • 52:10it is quite a causal approach,
  • 52:11but really interesting.
  • 52:12If you look at the production
  • 52:14of East Europe and South Asia,
  • 52:15that's the Indian region and
  • 52:17the East European region where
  • 52:19there's nobody cares about this,
  • 52:21The levels are still shockingly high.
  • 52:24So I think it's still relevant,
  • 52:25although for us it's a historic
  • 52:28study to be honest.
  • 52:30And do I do one more or should I do
  • 52:32for questions. This is a good ending.
  • 52:34So I could do a physical activity
  • 52:36in the brain,
  • 52:37but
  • 52:41good, then I'll wrap up.
  • 52:42So I'll leave away that
  • 52:44there is an association with.
  • 52:46Brain change that I should
  • 52:47I just do one slide?
  • 52:49No, I don't do one slide.
  • 52:50It doesn't work.
  • 52:50I do the, I do the IT doesn't work.
  • 52:53I just tell you it is we show that
  • 52:56would have been the last one.
  • 52:57I sort of did too much
  • 52:58fatty acids carried away.
  • 53:02I was going to show you that
  • 53:04we can show that physical
  • 53:06activity is related not just to
  • 53:08brain size and brain volume,
  • 53:10but it is related to the volume
  • 53:13change over adolescence.
  • 53:15Which is quite a bit more and that's
  • 53:17essentially an answer to the,
  • 53:18you know we need bigger studies
  • 53:20or we need studies of change.
  • 53:22We've now got the first studies of change.
  • 53:24If you want to show,
  • 53:24just show the result,
  • 53:28it's total physical activity really,
  • 53:30not just the the also quite a
  • 53:33bit of the hippocampus grows or.
  • 53:37Grows a bit faster if you do physical
  • 53:39activity and it's interesting
  • 53:41because it's consistent across
  • 53:43parent and child reported physical
  • 53:45activity reports Okay I'll wrap up.
  • 53:47So the dominant force in research is
  • 53:51the is the know you're imaging a lot
  • 53:54in autism and a DHDI would challenge
  • 53:57or like to discuss with people who say
  • 54:00it's made a change in our clinical
  • 54:02treatment or in our public health
  • 54:04understanding of autism and brain I think.
  • 54:06It did a lot for understanding the disease.
  • 54:09I'm not so sure it did a lot for how we
  • 54:12treat disease, which is a big difference.
  • 54:15I would say The effect sizes are often
  • 54:17small and often correlational and not causal.
  • 54:20There's a real problem which I didn't show
  • 54:22you, but we've struggled with that a lot.
  • 54:24Can we reproduce imaging results?
  • 54:28Anybody who might talk today was talking
  • 54:30about the heterogeneity of populations.
  • 54:31That's the same.
  • 54:32And I showed you that was the
  • 54:35minority majority is one example.
  • 54:37I think we have to, and that was my
  • 54:40first talk this morning was Kerim.
  • 54:41I think he's there in the back row.
  • 54:43We should really go to developmental
  • 54:45approaches and longitudinal trajectories.
  • 54:46I think that's the only way forward.
  • 54:49I fell short of showing you
  • 54:50that was the physical activity,
  • 54:52but I think that's what matters.
  • 54:55I'd like to wrap up,
  • 54:56it's not a diagnostic or prognostic tool.
  • 54:58It does have some public health relevance,
  • 55:01but I would say.
  • 55:03Occasionally,
  • 55:03and sometimes even sort of coincidentally,
  • 55:07but it does as many other fancy techniques.
  • 55:11These are the students that helped
  • 55:13Ryan Mitzler.
  • 55:14I want to mention him because he does
  • 55:15much of my imaging in the Netherlands
  • 55:17and students who did these papers,
  • 55:19and of course the participants.
  • 55:20Thank you very much.
  • 55:27Thank you so much, honey.
  • 55:28I will just say that we do have
  • 55:30time after the presentation.
  • 55:31So if anyone would like to stay in the
  • 55:33room and continue the conversation,
  • 55:35we're free until 2:30.
  • 55:36And but any burning
  • 55:38questions for Doctor Tamar
  • 55:49that was that was pretty interesting to me.
  • 55:51And I just wonder your thoughts
  • 55:53about how far do you go on
  • 55:56restrictive public policy to?
  • 56:00Get the good for for young children
  • 56:04who can't protect themselves.
  • 56:06So for instance you know it's good to
  • 56:10keep lead away from babies and and young
  • 56:13children but when you start talking
  • 56:15you know dietary and cultural things,
  • 56:18just your thoughts.
  • 56:19How how far do you go with this?
  • 56:20Do you do you, you know say that's it,
  • 56:23fruits and vegetables and
  • 56:25Mediterranean diet for everyone or.
  • 56:28That's an interesting one.
  • 56:29So that's sort of the whole public health
  • 56:32school of Harvard debates that every day
  • 56:36and seriously does if it's good to to
  • 56:38zoom in on an example because otherwise
  • 56:40I'm going to give a sort of overreaching,
  • 56:43I would be struggling.
  • 56:44That's a little evening thing debate.
  • 56:46If you take the dietary example,
  • 56:48I am in favor. Of restricting
  • 56:52soda and sweet drinks in schools.
  • 56:55We have seen now that
  • 56:57that is really so obesity,
  • 57:00making so much diabetes down the road.
  • 57:02I think we should go there.
  • 57:05Many of the others like no sweets,
  • 57:07which are also, you know,
  • 57:08sugar is bad, but I would be very,
  • 57:10very hesitant.
  • 57:12I think the best way to do it
  • 57:15is to think carefully with the
  • 57:17schools should sell them but.
  • 57:19To forbid them, perhaps a sugar tax.
  • 57:21But other than that I think
  • 57:23very little is evidence based.
  • 57:24So much of these things are not causal.
  • 57:26We changes.
  • 57:27You know look at the history of
  • 57:29the Harvard schools of department
  • 57:31of petition advice for diet.
  • 57:32You know that's a funny changing thing.
  • 57:34You know nuts and that and oils
  • 57:36and meat and alcohol.
  • 57:38Just look at the alcohol.
  • 57:39You know 20 years ago I was
  • 57:41taught in Rotterdam alcohol is
  • 57:42better than any concentration.
  • 57:44You come to Harvard and they say no,
  • 57:45but of alcohol is very good.
  • 57:47Now they have to sort of change that,
  • 57:49but it took them 15 years to
  • 57:52really come to a conclusion there.
  • 57:54So that encouraging of your
  • 57:57glass of red wine
  • 57:58is now gone. You know, you have to
  • 58:00have it was a bad conscience tonight,
  • 58:03but I think still think,
  • 58:04still think you should.
  • 58:05So I'd be very, very restrictive,
  • 58:07very, very cautious,
  • 58:09but I wouldn't shy away from a few measures,
  • 58:12very, very cautious.
  • 58:13But sometimes I think soda, we got it.
  • 58:16Sugars, we've got it.
  • 58:17So restrict the sugars in a
  • 58:19creative way and for not forbid
  • 58:21but tax it and don't have it. Yeah,
  • 58:25just really quickly. Perhaps relatedly,
  • 58:28you know when you talk about your
  • 58:30trans fatty acid decline over time,
  • 58:31I was thinking about PER and
  • 58:33polyfluoroloco substances.
  • 58:34You know, these forever chemicals and and.
  • 58:36You know, what we've seen in with
  • 58:38some pilot data there is that
  • 58:40there's a patterning by income level,
  • 58:41a patterning by income level per country.
  • 58:44I'm just wondering the decline in
  • 58:46trans fatty acids that you described,
  • 58:48was there a patterning by SES?
  • 58:50Did you observe A steeper? No.
  • 58:52We see much less of that patterning
  • 58:53in the US than in the US.
  • 58:55In the US, every environmental exposure
  • 58:58is highly socially patterned to an
  • 59:00extent that sometimes escapes me.
  • 59:02I don't quite know, you know,
  • 59:03why are they having so much more?
  • 59:05And then I hear they have different
  • 59:06hair products and this and that.
  • 59:08It's very hard for me to understand.
  • 59:10In the Netherlands, for example,
  • 59:11I'll tell you, organo phosphates,
  • 59:14which is pesticides,
  • 59:15were higher in the high SES
  • 59:18because they ate more fruit.
  • 59:19So in in the US we looked at the
  • 59:22same thing and lo and behold,
  • 59:24organo phosphates are lower in high SES.
  • 59:29I don't understand the US
  • 59:30enough to understand why that is
  • 59:32such a ubiquitous pattern.
  • 59:34In the Netherlands,
  • 59:35it's much less so people live.
  • 59:39I don't know as many reasons.
  • 59:42I don't quite understand that.
  • 59:43So in the Netherlands?
  • 59:44No, not always,
  • 59:45although some of some of the chemicals,
  • 59:48yes, very much so,
  • 59:49but not as not as dramatic as here.
  • 59:53I think you're trans fatty policy
  • 59:55example is one of the most profound
  • 59:58statements in support of integrating
  • 01:00:00the research and policy says
  • 01:00:01Thank you so much for sharing.
  • 01:00:03I definitely want to find out more
  • 01:00:04about that and track that
  • 01:00:06and try to replicate that.
  • 01:00:07My question for you is building
  • 01:00:09on all that you've done,
  • 01:00:10especially in the area of policy,
  • 01:00:12what do you see next?
  • 01:00:13What do you see is the next area that
  • 01:00:16you could be pursuing building out?
  • 01:00:19What does policy mean?
  • 01:00:20Because when I when I looked at the data,
  • 01:00:22I thought back to let.
  • 01:00:24Because in the United States there's
  • 01:00:26definitely an association with with
  • 01:00:28low income and and lead in your
  • 01:00:30pipes and in your drinking water.
  • 01:00:32So what is on your horizon
  • 01:00:34next in the space of of poverty
  • 01:00:38and research and policy?
  • 01:00:42Yeah there's there's in my school and in
  • 01:00:45my world thinking too 2 lines of research.
  • 01:00:48One is always which we have.
  • 01:00:50You know can you dissect. Why poverty?
  • 01:00:56What makes poverty relate to behavioral
  • 01:00:58and new developmental cognitive school
  • 01:01:00achievement problems or do you just
  • 01:01:02think you know it's money. That's it.
  • 01:01:08You know I am it's I think those
  • 01:01:12two are are totally separate.
  • 01:01:14I think we should fight LED
  • 01:01:17and environmental things really
  • 01:01:18more better and full force.
  • 01:01:22Lead is just I can't yeah we've
  • 01:01:23discussed that I don't need to say that
  • 01:01:24no none of us can believe that it's
  • 01:01:26still around as a public house hazard.
  • 01:01:28It should be gone.
  • 01:01:29It's just not acceptable.
  • 01:01:31At the same time I think make very
  • 01:01:34clear that as long as we have these
  • 01:01:37substantial poverty gradients that
  • 01:01:41that is a policy taxing and that's you
  • 01:01:44know beyond me to to do much about the
  • 01:01:46but it's clearly something that has
  • 01:01:48to be addressed because I think with.
  • 01:01:50Addressing LED, you will not
  • 01:01:53substantially address the poverty
  • 01:01:54inequality in this country as
  • 01:01:56much as I think it's important,
  • 01:01:58but it's completely different thing.
  • 01:02:00And you see that in
  • 01:02:04we all know that, you know homelessness.
  • 01:02:08I'm yeah, the the the extent of homelessness
  • 01:02:11in Boston and other areas is just so
  • 01:02:14dramatic and such a health hazard.
  • 01:02:17I don't know why that's not addressed.
  • 01:02:18I really fail to see that could
  • 01:02:20easily be addressed. Wonderful.
  • 01:02:22Well, just in the interest
  • 01:02:23of everyone's time,
  • 01:02:23if anyone would like to stay on,
  • 01:02:25please do wait in the room.
  • 01:02:26We can continue this conversation.
  • 01:02:28And but just please do join
  • 01:02:29me again in thanking Dr.
  • 01:02:31Kmar for his presentation.
  • 01:02:34Yeah. Sorry to talk so
  • 01:02:36long and see you later.