Skip to Main Content

Child Study Center Grand Rounds 02.02.2020

March 23, 2021

Never too early to Intervene Neurodevelopmental Perspective of Schizophrenia

ID
6323

Transcript

  • 00:00Hello and welcome to grand rounds.
  • 00:03Today is the second of three lectures
  • 00:07honoring our late director and friend,
  • 00:10Doctor Donald J Cohen.
  • 00:11Before we go to the main event
  • 00:14with Kartik's presentation,
  • 00:16I want to share some images and
  • 00:20thoughts about Doctor Cohen,
  • 00:22recognizing that many of us here today,
  • 00:25especially our younger trainees,
  • 00:27may not have known Donald Donald Cohen.
  • 00:31To remind you who lived between 1940 and
  • 00:332001 was a Sterling professor of child
  • 00:36psychiatry psychology in Pediatrics,
  • 00:38and he was the director of the Deal
  • 00:41Child Study Center from 1983 to 2001.
  • 00:44He was one of the most renowned
  • 00:47child psychiatrists in the world
  • 00:49and those of us who came under his
  • 00:52influence remember him not only fondly,
  • 00:55but our lives were really changed by him.
  • 00:59As I talked the first time during
  • 01:01Emily Olsen's presentation,
  • 01:03I commented on Donald the builder.
  • 01:05And when we talk when we have
  • 01:07the third talk by Tom Fernandez,
  • 01:09I will focus on Donald Cohen, the mentor.
  • 01:12But today with Kartik's presentation,
  • 01:15I think it's very fitting that
  • 01:17I'll talk about Donald Cohen,
  • 01:19the scientist,
  • 01:19and one thing that Donald and
  • 01:21Kartik had in common is that they
  • 01:24both got science early on.
  • 01:26They got the bug of science very early on.
  • 01:29Karthik is the son of scientists.
  • 01:32His early years were marinated
  • 01:33in science and he is really the
  • 01:36consummate scientist and like Donald,
  • 01:38as I say he got the bug early on.
  • 01:43Let me share with you how early
  • 01:45Donald got this bug.
  • 01:47This is an image from Donald in the
  • 01:504th grade, 3rd grade, 2nd grade.
  • 01:52I'm sorry and these are words
  • 01:54that he wrote maybe a year later
  • 01:57after this photograph was taken.
  • 01:59So let me quote.
  • 02:02Even at age 8,
  • 02:03I was curious about relationships
  • 02:04and thought about thinking,
  • 02:06especially how we think about each other.
  • 02:08This was the context of my
  • 02:11first formal interview.
  • 02:12Michael and I often went to
  • 02:14the Garfield Park Conservatory,
  • 02:16a wonderful institution available
  • 02:17to youngsters in Chicago.
  • 02:19We would wander through the rooms
  • 02:21filled with the tall tropical
  • 02:23trees and exotic Flowers,
  • 02:25taking in the beauty and Misty musty smells.
  • 02:28I became especially aware of one
  • 02:30man who would always be standing
  • 02:32quietly and watering the plants.
  • 02:34He patiently did his job with a sense of
  • 02:37calm and a gentle smile for the newspaper.
  • 02:40I thought he would be the ideal
  • 02:42person to interview and he consented.
  • 02:44The interview was then published in
  • 02:46our school newspaper and constitutes
  • 02:48one of the earlier reports,
  • 02:50though less widely.
  • 02:51Circulated than that of Kanner and Asperger,
  • 02:54of the central phenomena that
  • 02:56still intrigue our field.
  • 02:58Let me quote the full
  • 03:00article published in 1948,
  • 03:02at this time as a historic.
  • 03:06And the documentation of the launching
  • 03:08of a lifetime career in autism research.
  • 03:11This is a verbatim interview.
  • 03:13I was a shy and frail child.
  • 03:17Therefore,
  • 03:18I decided to become a gardener.
  • 03:22This early reporting is socially
  • 03:24dysfunctional.
  • 03:25Adult identifies constitutional factors,
  • 03:27shines,
  • 03:27and possible biological coral.
  • 03:30Its frailty with long term prognosis
  • 03:33in a career that was socially isolated,
  • 03:36the Gardner represents an optimistic
  • 03:39adaptation to an underlying
  • 03:41disability in social orientation.
  • 03:44So that's not. Donald, of course.
  • 03:46Went on to build a career focus on
  • 03:49autism work that has continued with
  • 03:52the likes of Red Oak Mark of Ami,
  • 03:54Klin of Jane McParland and so
  • 03:57many other of our colleagues.
  • 04:00Donna was also a psychoanalyst.
  • 04:02We're delighted that Phyllis Cohen
  • 04:04is joining us today and here you see,
  • 04:07Phyllis and Donald hanging out
  • 04:10with Anna Freud.
  • 04:11And this work in psychoanalysis and
  • 04:13bringing analysis and science together,
  • 04:16continues thinking of the
  • 04:17individual and the science,
  • 04:19not as two separate things,
  • 04:21but together the importance of
  • 04:24the lived experience.
  • 04:26And this is work that also continues
  • 04:28with Linda and the work at the
  • 04:31unemployed Center in London today.
  • 04:33Donald also was a pioneer
  • 04:35in the use of medication.
  • 04:37This is the persons of quantity
  • 04:401979 for Tourette syndrome.
  • 04:42And the rest, as we say, is history.
  • 04:45This is a work that has continued with,
  • 04:47I mean Amy Arnsten here at Yale with
  • 04:50Larry Scale now at Emory and medications
  • 04:53that have really revolutionized the
  • 04:56way that we treat ADHD Tourettes.
  • 04:58Here you have Donald in the 1970s
  • 05:02working on the molecule of quantity.
  • 05:05And that works.
  • 05:06That focus on threats continues most
  • 05:09notably with the work of Jim Lechman
  • 05:12and the incredible influence that
  • 05:14Donald had on Jim and the incredible
  • 05:16influence the gym in turn has had on.
  • 05:20Like a block on me personally,
  • 05:23and certainly on our hero of
  • 05:26the day on Karthik.
  • 05:28So without further ado,
  • 05:30let's see what the legacy of Donald
  • 05:33and the funds from his chair have
  • 05:35gone to do in the work of wonderful
  • 05:38young scientists like Emily Olson and
  • 05:40today Kartick Kartick take it away.
  • 05:42The grades were dropping and then
  • 05:44they started to notice that the child
  • 05:47was spending more time in their
  • 05:49room and eventually the mother was
  • 05:51trying to concern and brought him
  • 05:53into the emergency room because she
  • 05:55noted that this child was talking to.
  • 05:58Someone that wasn't there.
  • 06:00Eventually the child is diagnosed with
  • 06:02schizophrenia and from there schizophrenia
  • 06:03is a progressive disorder where there
  • 06:06is worsening psychotic symptoms.
  • 06:07There's worse than negative
  • 06:09and cognitive symptoms.
  • 06:13And so I think this is an important
  • 06:15point that schizophrenia is not just the
  • 06:17psychotic symptoms or the madness, which is,
  • 06:20I think is really focused on in a lot
  • 06:22of different menu, specifically media.
  • 06:24So there are positive symptoms
  • 06:26which are hallucinations,
  • 06:27delusions, or disorganized thought.
  • 06:28But there's also negative symptoms.
  • 06:30Which are Anna Donia, a motivation?
  • 06:32Social withdrawal?
  • 06:32I think that one of the initial
  • 06:34terms autoset almost looks like
  • 06:36autism as well as a flat affect.
  • 06:38And finally,
  • 06:39their cognitive symptoms can be
  • 06:41quite debilitating for the patients.
  • 06:42It's poor short term in working
  • 06:44memory as well as difficult and
  • 06:47difficulty in communication.
  • 06:48And so once this child was
  • 06:50admitted to our unit,
  • 06:52we decided to start a medication
  • 06:54for this child, an antipsychotic.
  • 06:55You know everyone has their
  • 06:57their choice and despite having
  • 06:58medications to treat schizophrenia,
  • 07:00the outcomes are still not great.
  • 07:02In this.
  • 07:03Once they show that 10% of patients
  • 07:05with schizophrenia die by sight,
  • 07:07suicide,
  • 07:07which is greatly elevated from
  • 07:09the rest of the population.
  • 07:1125% have chronic severe symptoms,
  • 07:12while most patients have some
  • 07:14level of symptoms and they're
  • 07:16just looking at symptoms,
  • 07:17functional outcomes are actually much worse.
  • 07:19I think part of the issue is that it
  • 07:22was kind of highlighted in the Katy
  • 07:25trial that was done early in the 2000s,
  • 07:27where they showed in two at least.
  • 07:30They followed patients with
  • 07:31schizophrenia over two years and only
  • 07:3326% of them actually stayed on their
  • 07:35medication through this whole trial.
  • 07:37And due to this progressive illness
  • 07:38as well as the difficulty of keeping
  • 07:41patients on their medications,
  • 07:42the focus has been on prevention and
  • 07:45this has been work done starting in the
  • 07:47late 80s by various groups instead of.
  • 07:50Anything including a strong group
  • 07:51here in adult psychiatry with the
  • 07:54focus has been as is intervening
  • 07:56during this prodromal stage.
  • 07:57So when our honor student is slowly
  • 07:59starting to academically decline,
  • 08:01can we interview at that point intervene?
  • 08:03Sorry at that point to change the
  • 08:05trajectory of schizophrenia and maybe
  • 08:07prevent the transition to psychosis.
  • 08:09And there's been a lot of
  • 08:11great work done in that area,
  • 08:13especially the focus on psychoeducation
  • 08:15teaching the family what to expect,
  • 08:17as well as reducing the stress
  • 08:19that the child experiences
  • 08:20as it's thought that stress.
  • 08:22Can worsen the trajectory of the disorder
  • 08:25and despite all these great studies,
  • 08:27kinda bleak meta analysis came out
  • 08:29July of 2020 and then I'll just
  • 08:32highlight that first sentence.
  • 08:33No evidence was found that that favored
  • 08:36any indicated intervention over another,
  • 08:38including control conditions.
  • 08:39So despite all this work,
  • 08:41we're not really preventing the transition
  • 08:43to psychosis and later on they talk.
  • 08:45There's actually no real obvious improvement
  • 08:47in a lot of these other functional measures,
  • 08:51So what gives?
  • 08:52How can we prevent schizophrenia?
  • 08:54And I think part of the issue
  • 08:56has been this psychosis driven
  • 08:58formulation of the disorder that
  • 09:00prior to this prodromal phase
  • 09:02there's no symptoms or few symptoms,
  • 09:04and so the focus has been on the prodrome.
  • 09:08But actually data shows something different.
  • 09:11This was one of the seminal
  • 09:13works that came out in 1994,
  • 09:14led by Doctor Elaine Walker and what she
  • 09:17showed along with when it came out in 1994.
  • 09:20I thought it was kind of funny.
  • 09:22Was that this is correlated.
  • 09:24But 20 years after like home video
  • 09:26cameras became popular with this study,
  • 09:28did was they looked at the life they
  • 09:30recruited patients with schizophrenia
  • 09:31and then looked at their home videos
  • 09:33from their childhood and they looked for
  • 09:36neuromotor abnormalities and what they
  • 09:37found specifically in infants is that.
  • 09:39And so this population that
  • 09:41later developed schizophrenia.
  • 09:42Has schizophrenia as a child they showed
  • 09:44increased abnormal motor movements
  • 09:46while their healthy siblings did not.
  • 09:47Just things that did not have
  • 09:49schizophrenia and patients with
  • 09:50other affective disorders also
  • 09:52didn't have a significant difference
  • 09:53from the patients of schizophrenia.
  • 09:58Later, Lin walked.
  • 09:59Walker was part of this other study
  • 10:02that was more thoughtful 'cause
  • 10:04they had no more time to design
  • 10:07this study and what they did was
  • 10:09they videotaped 1311 through 13
  • 10:11year olds in Scandinavia that have
  • 10:12an increased risk of schizophrenia.
  • 10:14Specifically a parent with
  • 10:16schizophrenia and they found that
  • 10:17when they went and then waited to
  • 10:19see which one of these children
  • 10:21would later develop schizophrenia,
  • 10:23they found that the children that
  • 10:25later developed schizophrenia show
  • 10:26deficits and social behaviors.
  • 10:28Around 11:13 there wasn't an obviously
  • 10:30significant different in motor.
  • 10:31Even though it trended in boys
  • 10:33towards having some motor atypical,
  • 10:35atypical motor behaviors,
  • 10:36and this more recent paper was
  • 10:39something that I was really drawn to,
  • 10:41and this was part of the Avon
  • 10:43Perspective study where they just
  • 10:45tracked children that were born in two
  • 10:48years in this city of Avon and England,
  • 10:50and then just did a bunch of like just
  • 10:53neuro psychological battery on them
  • 10:55and they found that children that
  • 10:57later developed psychotic disorders,
  • 10:59that's that red line around the age of.
  • 11:0218 months somewhere to 18 months
  • 11:04in four years start to show a
  • 11:07decline in cognitive function.
  • 11:08This is seen in full scale IQ,
  • 11:11verbal IQ and nonverbal IQ and
  • 11:12so I think these findings along
  • 11:14with various other studies suggest
  • 11:16maybe we're missing something.
  • 11:18Maybe schizophrenia actually
  • 11:20starts earlier than we thought.
  • 11:23And so this has been led to kind
  • 11:25of a new version of schizophrenia,
  • 11:28which I feel is much more child psychiatry.
  • 11:31Child psychology focused where
  • 11:32initially their cognitive motor
  • 11:34and social impairments and these
  • 11:36progressively moved towards psychosis,
  • 11:37and I think this is a really
  • 11:40interesting theory because it kind
  • 11:42of gives us a longer runway, right?
  • 11:45Can we intervene much earlier and to
  • 11:47later prevent this transition to psychosis?
  • 11:50However,
  • 11:50in order to turn this model into real?
  • 11:53Real clinical intervention.
  • 11:54We have to kind of understand the
  • 11:56neurobiological underpinnings of this data.
  • 11:58So what are the neurobiological
  • 11:59correlates of the neuro developmental
  • 12:01theory of schizophrenia?
  • 12:02Which is something that I'm very
  • 12:04interested and so the first thing we
  • 12:07can do is look to our colleagues in
  • 12:10psychiatric genetics such as my good friend.
  • 12:13Emily Olson,
  • 12:13who's like,
  • 12:14does a brilliant work and gave an
  • 12:16amazing presentation two weeks ago.
  • 12:18As Doctor Martin mentioned and what
  • 12:20she kind of talked about was the
  • 12:23current state of psychiatric genetics,
  • 12:24and in the last 10 years there's
  • 12:27been amazing progress in the field,
  • 12:29and schizophrenia is also seen
  • 12:31great progress in schizophrenia,
  • 12:32genetics,
  • 12:32where they have identified several
  • 12:34genes associated or enriched
  • 12:36in mutations in several genes
  • 12:37associated with schizophrenia,
  • 12:38and what a recent study has done.
  • 12:41And this is kind of.
  • 12:43Where several labs are doing this
  • 12:44where you have so many genes,
  • 12:46how can we turn these jeans into
  • 12:48specific targets or regions or time
  • 12:50periods of development to study?
  • 12:52And what this study did was they
  • 12:54looked for regions in the brain
  • 12:56in specific periods.
  • 12:57They looked at fetal infancy,
  • 12:58an adult adolescent stages and they
  • 13:01looked at different regions of the
  • 13:02brain and they looked for a place and a
  • 13:05time where there was an enrichment of
  • 13:07genes associated with schizophrenia.
  • 13:08Almost like you have people
  • 13:10involved in a crime.
  • 13:11Are they in the same place together?
  • 13:13As a way of maybe giving you a time
  • 13:16point in an area to specifically
  • 13:18dig deeper into to understand what
  • 13:21they found was an enrichment.
  • 13:22These alleles in the fetal cortex,
  • 13:25specifically in the frontal lobe,
  • 13:26and this is very similar findings to
  • 13:28work done by Matt States Group and
  • 13:31he was here at Yale in autism and
  • 13:33this also coincides really well with
  • 13:36earlier epidemiological data that
  • 13:38shows that stress during pregnancy,
  • 13:39specifically during the 2nd trimester
  • 13:41increase the chance of having a child.
  • 13:44That develops schizophrenia.
  • 13:47So this led to our more focused
  • 13:49question how does disruption of
  • 13:51neurodevelopmental events in the mid
  • 13:52fetal frontal cortex lead to the
  • 13:55complex symptoms of schizophrenia?
  • 13:56And so this is where the story starts.
  • 14:00And So what we did is we use that
  • 14:02same data set that these previous
  • 14:04groups use is called psych encoder.
  • 14:06Brain sandwiches generated in this
  • 14:08test in lab to look for genes that
  • 14:10were uniquely enriched in the frontal
  • 14:12lobe during mid field development.
  • 14:14We're looking for something specific
  • 14:15to the mid function.
  • 14:17The midfielder frontal lobe and that's
  • 14:19why we looked for genes are enriched there.
  • 14:21And so we identified 125 genes enriched
  • 14:23specifically in the frontal lobe.
  • 14:25During this period,
  • 14:26Anna's Emily showed in her previous talk.
  • 14:28We looked at what do these jeans?
  • 14:30Share this click gene ontogeny analysis and
  • 14:33what we found was that these genes are in.
  • 14:35This list is enriched for genes that
  • 14:37are involved in circuit formation so
  • 14:39synapse assembly Axon development in
  • 14:41circuits are kind of the functional unit.
  • 14:43I think I, uh,
  • 14:45I guess eventually unit.
  • 14:46That's like high truly focuses on the brain,
  • 14:49but we also saw something that
  • 14:50was unexpected which was jeans
  • 14:52that are responsive to rent.
  • 14:54No gas or jeans that are regulated
  • 14:56by retinoic acid and this was very
  • 14:58exciting for us. For several reasons.
  • 15:00There was a basic science.
  • 15:02Perspective which I want talking
  • 15:04or evolutionary perspective that
  • 15:05I want to talk about.
  • 15:07But there was a very interesting
  • 15:09clinical perspective.
  • 15:10So disruption of retinoic acid signaling
  • 15:11has been implicated in schizophrenia.
  • 15:14And it seems that the your polygenic
  • 15:16risk score or how many genes associated
  • 15:18with retinoic acid's are affected seem
  • 15:20to be elevated in patients schizophrenia
  • 15:23with severe cognitive symptoms.
  • 15:24So this was something very
  • 15:26interesting and very exciting to us,
  • 15:28so we postulated that disruption
  • 15:30of RA regulated or related genes.
  • 15:32Causes some change in brain development.
  • 15:34Likely circuit formation as
  • 15:36those are what those genes are.
  • 15:38Also enriched and and that can lead
  • 15:40to the early cognitive symptoms
  • 15:42of schizophrenia.
  • 15:44So to study this,
  • 15:45we turn to a mouse model and
  • 15:47we asked what is retinoic acid
  • 15:49doing specifically in
  • 15:50the frontal cortex.
  • 15:52So we generated a mutant mouse line
  • 15:54where we're able to specifically
  • 15:55reduce retinoic acid signaling Justin,
  • 15:57the mouse equivalent of
  • 15:59the prefrontal cortex,
  • 16:00and So what you see here is a PO
  • 16:02mouse brain which is equivalent to
  • 16:05mid fetal about post Conception Week
  • 16:0724 in the human and these purple
  • 16:09dots are cells where there's active
  • 16:11retinoic acid signaling going on,
  • 16:13and so this is the control.
  • 16:15And this is the mutant where we've
  • 16:17modified retinoic acid signaling,
  • 16:18and you can see a dramatic
  • 16:20reduction in purple cells which
  • 16:22is quantified here on the right.
  • 16:23And so now that we've reduced
  • 16:25retinoic acid saying we looked at
  • 16:27what happens to circuit formation in
  • 16:28the frontal cortex or specific middle
  • 16:30prefrontal cortex in the mouse and
  • 16:32what we found was a couple things.
  • 16:34But I'll highlight just a few phenotypes.
  • 16:36First,
  • 16:36we found a reduction in dendritic spines
  • 16:38which are kind of excitatory connections
  • 16:40in the brain which is quantified here.
  • 16:42This is done in the adult,
  • 16:44and so you can see a
  • 16:46reduction in total spines.
  • 16:47But also,
  • 16:47reduction in mushroom bodies,
  • 16:49which are mature spines and we replicate
  • 16:51this finding both during the equivalent of
  • 16:53midfield development in the human at PO.
  • 16:56So this occurs very early.
  • 16:57But we also saw this
  • 16:59finding in adults as well,
  • 17:01so this seems like this deficit in
  • 17:03excitatory connections starts very early,
  • 17:05but the finding that I want to focus
  • 17:07on is this very interesting deficit
  • 17:09that we found that got us all excited.
  • 17:12We first found this data,
  • 17:14so this is a study using this mutant
  • 17:16mouse line using diffusion tensor.
  • 17:18Which looks at connectivity in the brain,
  • 17:21and so this is a five day old
  • 17:23mouse brain which you can see
  • 17:25kind of as a Gray fuzzy outline,
  • 17:28and so about five days equivalent to
  • 17:30birth in the human and what we found
  • 17:32was a dramatic reduction between
  • 17:34connections which are shown by these.
  • 17:36These red lines between the
  • 17:38prefrontal cortex and the thalamus
  • 17:40on the right is the control,
  • 17:41so you can see this thick bundle
  • 17:43and on the left is the mutant where
  • 17:46you see a dramatic reduction in
  • 17:48connectivity which is quantified.
  • 17:50Here on the left,
  • 17:51and so we found this deficit very early,
  • 17:54but it seems like this may.
  • 17:55This deficit is maintained during
  • 17:57adolescence, so we checked it P.
  • 17:5921 but we also checked it P.
  • 18:0116 The adult.
  • 18:02So this isn't, you know,
  • 18:03a delay in development.
  • 18:05This is a complete disruption of
  • 18:06the circuit in our mouse model.
  • 18:11And this was kind of fits what
  • 18:13we've been thinking about with
  • 18:14schizophrenia, so this connection,
  • 18:16the connection between the medial
  • 18:18prefrontal cortex and the thalamus
  • 18:19is essential for cognition,
  • 18:20specifically working memory.
  • 18:21This is a study.
  • 18:23This is a reviewed written by
  • 18:25Doctor Gordon Josh Gordon,
  • 18:26who is now the head of the MH,
  • 18:29who showed that the connection
  • 18:30between the mediodorsal thalamus
  • 18:32and the prefrontal cortex,
  • 18:33and there's typical connection
  • 18:34between the prefrontal cortex
  • 18:36and the mediodorsal thalamus is
  • 18:38essential for the delay in the
  • 18:40retrieval steps of working memory.
  • 18:41The circuits also been
  • 18:43implicated in social behavior,
  • 18:45so it's a circuit that's very it's
  • 18:48highly studied in psychiatric biology.
  • 18:54And so our proposed model is that
  • 18:56disruption of retinoic acid,
  • 18:58associated and regulated genes lead
  • 19:00to reduce connectivity between the
  • 19:02prefrontal cortex in the thalamus
  • 19:03and that leads to the early
  • 19:05cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia.
  • 19:07This deficit occurs during the 2nd
  • 19:10trimester of development in scene
  • 19:13right at birth in our mouse model.
  • 19:16So this led to a lot of important
  • 19:18questions of future directions,
  • 19:19which I'll talk about.
  • 19:21So first this is the mouse model.
  • 19:23This is have any relevance to
  • 19:25patients with schizophrenia?
  • 19:25And so this in this case we turn
  • 19:28to another set of colleagues,
  • 19:29colleagues that are involved
  • 19:31in functional imaging,
  • 19:32functional neural humanit,
  • 19:33functional imaging.
  • 19:34And this is a study done in 2015 and
  • 19:36what they found they were specifically
  • 19:38looking at the lambic cortical
  • 19:40networks in healthy subjects that
  • 19:42compare them to patients within four
  • 19:44weeks of their diagnosis of schizophrenia.
  • 19:46And then patients with chronic
  • 19:48schizophrenia chronic psychosis.
  • 19:49So this has been over a couple years.
  • 19:53Of being diagnosed with schizophrenia
  • 19:54and we can see in the blue bar,
  • 19:56so this is functional connectivity so
  • 19:58it's looking at just connection between.
  • 20:00Different regions,
  • 20:01specifically the almacen regions
  • 20:02of the cortex,
  • 20:03and they showed that compared to
  • 20:05the control which is the blue bar,
  • 20:07there is reduced connectivity
  • 20:08both at diagnosis,
  • 20:09which is shown at the yellow bar
  • 20:11and both in the chronic stage.
  • 20:13So that seems like this disruption
  • 20:15is present,
  • 20:16and at least this population
  • 20:17of patients with schizophrenia,
  • 20:19something else that was really interesting
  • 20:21was a study done by Alan and Tisha Vich,
  • 20:23who is in I guess now apiai in
  • 20:26adult psychiatry.
  • 20:27But this is when he was working
  • 20:29in Doctor Tyrone Cannons Lab.
  • 20:31And they looked at patients with high
  • 20:33risk of developing schizophrenia.
  • 20:34They had a certain set of criteria
  • 20:36to kind of identify these patients
  • 20:38and what they showed was this
  • 20:41disconnection between Hypo connection
  • 20:42with they called it between the medial
  • 20:45frontal cortex or the frontal cortex,
  • 20:47and the thalamus was present even
  • 20:49prior to conversion in two psychosis.
  • 20:51Specifically,
  • 20:52that then I guess in addition
  • 20:54to what they showed,
  • 20:55was that the worst the psychotic symptoms,
  • 20:58the more the reduced connectivity
  • 21:00there was between.
  • 21:01These two regions,
  • 21:01so it seems like even prior to conversion,
  • 21:04there seems to be this disconnection
  • 21:06between the thalamus in the cortex,
  • 21:07and so I think this is an interesting
  • 21:10circuit to study in and further work from.
  • 21:12Functional neurology is important
  • 21:13to kind of see if this is a much
  • 21:16more generalizable etiology.
  • 21:17We also plan to study this in
  • 21:20various mouse models as well.
  • 21:22So the second question,
  • 21:23which I think is something that
  • 21:25I'm very excited about and I think
  • 21:27is an essential question to really
  • 21:29understand how to understand the
  • 21:31complex symptoms of schizophrenia,
  • 21:33is how can a specific deficit in
  • 21:34one circuit that's involved in
  • 21:36cognition lead to the complex
  • 21:38symptoms of schizophrenia?
  • 21:39Just as a quick refresher we talk
  • 21:41about we're talking about a circuit
  • 21:43that's important for cognition
  • 21:44and maybe some
  • 21:45social behavior,
  • 21:46but there's also the hallucinations,
  • 21:48delusions, the various other
  • 21:49symptoms of schizophrenia,
  • 21:50so we turn back to those studies that
  • 21:52I just mentioned from functional.
  • 21:54Human neuroimaging and what we see is in,
  • 21:58so I showed you this panel here,
  • 22:01which is the connectivity between
  • 22:03the prefrontal cortex and the family
  • 22:05showed reduced connectivity during
  • 22:07chronic psychosis and early stages,
  • 22:09but we actually see a commensurate
  • 22:12hyper connectivity between the
  • 22:14thalamus in the motor cortex and the
  • 22:16thalamus in the somato sensory cortex.
  • 22:19What's interesting is that that the
  • 22:21study done by Allen Tiffin Teacher
  • 22:24***** so that same hyperconnectivity
  • 22:26as well in these prodromal patients.
  • 22:31And so that in this kind of I turned
  • 22:34back to kind of some of our mouse works.
  • 22:37So we not only looked at connections
  • 22:39between the thalamus and the frontal
  • 22:41cortex in our mutant mouse time,
  • 22:42we looked at various other
  • 22:44connectivity at specifically at P4,
  • 22:46which is equivalent to birth,
  • 22:47and at this stage the only deficit that
  • 22:49we found was a connection between the
  • 22:52thalamus and the medial prefrontal cortex,
  • 22:54and we did not see reduced connectivity
  • 22:56between the thalamus in the motor cortex or
  • 22:59the thalamus in the somatosensory cortex.
  • 23:01So what we're interested in doing is
  • 23:03seeing whether there's a change in
  • 23:05this kind of diagram in connectivity.
  • 23:06As the mouse gets older,
  • 23:08do we see a change in adolescence,
  • 23:10and do we see a change in adulthood?
  • 23:12We know that the connection between
  • 23:14the medial prefrontal cortex,
  • 23:15the prefrontal cortex in the thalamus,
  • 23:17is lossed all the way through.
  • 23:19But what happens to other circuits?
  • 23:21Are there progressive change and
  • 23:22so kind of fleshing out this model,
  • 23:24which is something that we need
  • 23:26to continue to test and study?
  • 23:28Is that retinoic acid associated
  • 23:30genes disruption in these genes
  • 23:31lead to reduced medial?
  • 23:32The frontal cortex,
  • 23:33medial dorsal thalamus and medial prefrontal
  • 23:36cortex connectivity and that leads
  • 23:38specifically to early cognitive symptoms.
  • 23:40However,
  • 23:40the brain now has to compensate so this this
  • 23:43disruption of a circuit occurs very early.
  • 23:46So during childhood the brain is
  • 23:48trying to compensate for this deficit.
  • 23:51As we know,
  • 23:52the brain can do through neuroplasticity,
  • 23:54and maybe these secondary changes
  • 23:56may underlie the positive and
  • 23:57negative symptoms of schizophrenia.
  • 23:59Abig critique of the
  • 24:01neurodevelopmental theory is what?
  • 24:02Explains that big gap between what's
  • 24:04supposed to be the deficit during
  • 24:06development and the positive symptoms later,
  • 24:08and what what it could be is
  • 24:11that the brain is compensating
  • 24:12it's compensating it's working.
  • 24:14It's kind of figuring out different ways to
  • 24:16try to make up for this cognitive deficit,
  • 24:19and eventually as expectations get higher,
  • 24:21there's a breakdown in the system,
  • 24:23and that leads to the psychotic symptoms.
  • 24:25This can also kind of dovetail well with
  • 24:27kind of the current driving force kind
  • 24:29of driving neurodevelopmental theory that
  • 24:31it's a specific synaptic pruning deficit.
  • 24:34During adolescence,
  • 24:34and that's where you see the break then.
  • 24:37So maybe as you prune more,
  • 24:38you've lost some of these
  • 24:40compens atory mechanisms,
  • 24:41leading to a psychotic break.
  • 24:42And this is something that I'm really
  • 24:44interested in testing in a mouse model.
  • 24:46What happens to our mouse when we have
  • 24:48a specific deficit in this mediodorsal
  • 24:50prefrontal cortex connection,
  • 24:51and how does it change the rest of the brain?
  • 24:54And how does it change behavior
  • 24:56progressively through time?
  • 24:58And I think this also gives us
  • 25:00an opportunity to think about
  • 25:02interventions or future interventions.
  • 25:04If this is the primary insult,
  • 25:06a connection between the mediodorsal
  • 25:07down in the prefrontal cortex,
  • 25:09can we try to strengthen this
  • 25:11connection using in the few?
  • 25:13It's very futuristic at this point,
  • 25:15but maybe deep brain stimulation
  • 25:17or TMS or other processes.
  • 25:19Or we could just work on cognitive skills.
  • 25:22Could we do brain training apps
  • 25:24almost like a burst to three?
  • 25:26However, for patients that are at high risk?
  • 25:29Of schizophrenia and the thought is
  • 25:31by not only by treating this trying
  • 25:34to strengthen this circuit can we
  • 25:36improve these early cognitive symptoms?
  • 25:39We can altogether prevent the secondary
  • 25:41circuit changes and maybe prevent
  • 25:43positive and negatives symptoms.
  • 25:45And this is something I'm just
  • 25:47generally interested in studying.
  • 25:49Currently circuits neuroscience,
  • 25:50which is, I think,
  • 25:52a big crux of adult psychiatry has
  • 25:54an adult focus of what they do.
  • 25:57Is they disrupt relevant circuits?
  • 25:59Like the study I showed that
  • 26:01was done by Josh Gordon's lab.
  • 26:03They just daily disrupt these
  • 26:04circuits during adult in adults or
  • 26:06they disrupted during adolescence.
  • 26:08And then they look, went to disrupt it.
  • 26:10How does that affect behavior?
  • 26:12But what I'm interested in is I think
  • 26:14more relevant to disorders like autism,
  • 26:16schizophrenia,
  • 26:16where we see this disruption occurs
  • 26:18very early and so we can then study
  • 26:20if we disrupt in the development
  • 26:22of specific circuits early.
  • 26:24How does that affect the rest
  • 26:25of brain dump development?
  • 26:27I think this could generate a more.
  • 26:30A better version or better model of
  • 26:34neuro psychiatric disease in our
  • 26:37model systems to further study.
  • 26:39So this is a summary of the talk.
  • 26:42I think the main points I want
  • 26:44everyone to take away within some
  • 26:46that we can further discuss is that
  • 26:49the cognitive and motor symptoms
  • 26:51of schizophrenia may precede the
  • 26:53positive symptoms in schizophrenia.
  • 26:54This is underlying the neuro
  • 26:56developmental theory of schizophrenia
  • 26:57and disruption of frontal cortex
  • 26:59development during the 2nd trimester
  • 27:01has been associated with schizophrenia,
  • 27:02both genetically as well as from
  • 27:04epidemiological data and then
  • 27:06finally prefrontal cortex,
  • 27:07Islamic connectivity.
  • 27:08Maybe the underlying developmental
  • 27:09etiology of schizophrenia and a target.
  • 27:11For early intervention.
  • 27:14So this was the data that I showed
  • 27:16was part of a as netted,
  • 27:18mentioned a large collaborative
  • 27:19effort at specifically like to thank
  • 27:22men and who's been an excellent
  • 27:24mentoring is really giving me a
  • 27:25great opportunity in the lab and
  • 27:27as well as a Mickey to Shibata,
  • 27:29who we worked very closely in
  • 27:31leading this project.
  • 27:31But as I said,
  • 27:33there were so many different people in
  • 27:35the lab that were involved in this project.
  • 27:37Doing experiments giving feedback.
  • 27:38It's just been really amazing process,
  • 27:40but I also like to thank all
  • 27:43of our collaborators.
  • 27:44They helped us with both the generating
  • 27:46the mice as well as the neuroimaging.
  • 27:48The diffusion tensor imaging
  • 27:49in the mouse model,
  • 27:51as well as our funding sources
  • 27:53there retinoic acid choice,
  • 27:54specifically was funded by Safari
  • 27:55Assignments Foundation for Autism Research,
  • 27:57and then it is funded by the NIH,
  • 27:59and I received some money from
  • 28:01the Riva Ariella Ritvo endowment,
  • 28:03which I'm using towards kind of
  • 28:05answering that last question that I'm
  • 28:07really interested to and then finally
  • 28:09like to thank the Solar Program.
  • 28:11The Child Study Center this other
  • 28:12program has been really amazing
  • 28:14opportunity to really kind of get
  • 28:16into lab and really kind of bridge.
  • 28:18The clinic as well as my
  • 28:20basic science interests,
  • 28:21so I'm really thankful to doctor Block,
  • 28:24Doctor Stube and Tiny Cologne
  • 28:25who really do the heavy lifting.
  • 28:27The program as well as the T 32 fellowship,
  • 28:30which funds my last two years in the program.
  • 28:35And that's the talk.
  • 28:46I have a question if I can
  • 28:48jump in and it's amazing data,
  • 28:51I just think it's so important.
  • 28:55I had a question about connectivity.
  • 28:57Were you able to see reduced
  • 28:59connectivity and other pathways,
  • 29:01either longranger or local?
  • 29:02I know it's very challenging
  • 29:04in the mouse where there isn't
  • 29:07that much cortical cortical,
  • 29:08but I'm guessing that this is and
  • 29:11the the thalamic mouse prefrontal
  • 29:13connection is extra important in
  • 29:15a mouse compared to a primate.
  • 29:17I think 'cause of that were
  • 29:19you able to see others here,
  • 29:22so we definitely looked
  • 29:23at that. We wanted to make sure.
  • 29:26Whether this was a specific
  • 29:27lesion and before we started,
  • 29:29we were kind of agnostic about the
  • 29:31circuit that we were expecting,
  • 29:32so we did a couple things.
  • 29:34First, I kind of went quickly to this side,
  • 29:37so this is very diffusion
  • 29:38tensor imaging data from P5.
  • 29:40So we kind of basically
  • 29:41parcel atede using an Atlas.
  • 29:43Different regions of the brain,
  • 29:44and so we didn't see any deficits
  • 29:46in inflamma cortical connections
  • 29:47besides the connection to the
  • 29:49medial prefrontal cortex.
  • 29:50We did see a little bit of a
  • 29:52deficit in the orbitofrontal cortex,
  • 29:54which isn't shown here.
  • 29:56What was really interesting
  • 29:57is that it seems like both of
  • 29:58these tracks ride together,
  • 29:59and so we were one day.
  • 30:00Weather DTI was aimed really had
  • 30:02some issues separating them,
  • 30:03but we didn't see any and I'll show
  • 30:05some later date a couple slides
  • 30:07down but we shot so no deficits
  • 30:08in cortical cortical connectivity
  • 30:10between the somatosensory cortex
  • 30:12with the motor cortex as well
  • 30:14as the prefrontal cortex,
  • 30:15and we didn't see any changes
  • 30:17in cortical amic tracks.
  • 30:18We also looked at dopaminergic input.
  • 30:20We actually initially thought we would
  • 30:21see a deficit in dopaminergic input
  • 30:23and made that underlies the positive
  • 30:25symptoms of schizophrenia and at
  • 30:27least at the ages that we looked at,
  • 30:29we didn't see any.
  • 30:30Gross abnormalities in dopaminergic input,
  • 30:32and I know times aren't senior
  • 30:34interest in the prefrontal cortex,
  • 30:36so we also did.
  • 30:40Yeah, so we also didn't show this slide,
  • 30:43but we also did viral tracing
  • 30:45from the prefrontal cortex and
  • 30:47so this was in the so we did.
  • 30:49This was in the adult where
  • 30:51we and this was nabbed car.
  • 30:53Doctor Carr was a postdoc in the
  • 30:55lab who did an injection in the
  • 30:57prefrontal cortex and then we
  • 30:59looked at just inputs into the
  • 31:01prefrontal cortex and whether
  • 31:03there are any specific deficits.
  • 31:04We saw a little bit of reduction
  • 31:06connectivity between in the insula,
  • 31:09but still once again the primary deficit was.
  • 31:11The mediodorsal doubtful amic
  • 31:13input into the prefrontal cortex.
  • 31:18And I'm copying
  • 31:20that slide so I can look at it is.
  • 31:23Is that also a reduced connectivity
  • 31:25with is that medial orbital?
  • 31:30We didn't see
  • 31:31that much, so I called that date.
  • 31:33We call that the insula.
  • 31:36There wasn't much connectivity that we
  • 31:37could see in our tracing experiment,
  • 31:39but I can look more closely over the data.
  • 31:42I can definitely email you some
  • 31:43of the slides so we can look
  • 31:45at it more closely together.
  • 31:46Great if I may interject.
  • 31:48Amy, I think you are right.
  • 31:49Actually, we do suspended that,
  • 31:51but you know, the problem is that we just.
  • 31:54Our goal for us not to claim too much.
  • 31:58I totally understand
  • 31:59that for publication in particular, but.
  • 32:04Especially, you know, given the cortical
  • 32:08cortical connections and and the.
  • 32:11Between hemispheres was that one
  • 32:13also not affected that was
  • 32:16not affected as well. The closel.
  • 32:18I just imagine that if it's
  • 32:21more general than the thalamic,
  • 32:24we really see that in the human, and
  • 32:27that it would be tremendously important.
  • 32:32Ask your question.
  • 32:34Kept up or you know about 30 years ago,
  • 32:38Danny Weinberger at NIMH had
  • 32:41a developmental theory about
  • 32:43schizophrenia and that the
  • 32:45reason that it sort of showed
  • 32:48up at around age 20 or so.
  • 32:51Why is that? The
  • 32:53kind of prefrontal inhibitory
  • 32:55functions that usually
  • 32:57began to come online it around that time?
  • 33:01Somehow words and I was just curious.
  • 33:05This is a naive question in terms of
  • 33:09my understanding of DTI and maybe this
  • 33:12retrograde study shows something.
  • 33:14But what is the directionality?
  • 33:16You talk about defects in
  • 33:18connectivity between the
  • 33:20prefrontal cortex and the thalamus?
  • 33:22Are those simply reciprocal,
  • 33:24or are they mostly kind of top down? And
  • 33:30then the other question is
  • 33:32that you know going way back
  • 33:35to the 70s and 80s with work by
  • 33:39Barbara Fish and Kornblatt and
  • 33:41other people who are studying
  • 33:43children of skits, high risk
  • 33:46offspring of schizophrenic patients.
  • 33:47I mean they showed a lot of
  • 33:51early kind of minor ADHD
  • 33:53inhibitory difficulties,
  • 33:54subtle neuropsychological deficits,
  • 33:55and I was wondering if
  • 33:58you had thoughts about what
  • 34:00the? Underlying neural correlate's
  • 34:02of those are. Definitely yeah,
  • 34:06and I think that Danny Weinberger's work.
  • 34:08I have a Erica. That's a slide from his.
  • 34:11So he really was the creator of this
  • 34:13theory of the neuro developmental theory.
  • 34:15And this is actually from one of
  • 34:17his more recent reviews and I
  • 34:19think that's really interesting.
  • 34:20And so I think the focus has
  • 34:22been really in that adolescence,
  • 34:24and it makes sense, right?
  • 34:25Maybe there.
  • 34:26I think it combines two theories together.
  • 34:28I tried to touch on that and that what
  • 34:31we see is that the brain is trying to
  • 34:33compensate the brains trying to compensate.
  • 34:36As I mentioned.
  • 34:37And then, once the.
  • 34:38The normal processes of development
  • 34:40that occur during adolescence,
  • 34:41which strips away these
  • 34:43connections at reduced plasticity.
  • 34:44With the closure of these neural Nets,
  • 34:47you lose that compens atory mechanisms,
  • 34:49and then that kind of breaks
  • 34:52through and leads to the symptoms.
  • 34:56Maybe that's something that needs
  • 34:57to be really studied, right?
  • 34:59I mean, I think this is still something
  • 35:01that we're looking at in a mouse model.
  • 35:03I kind of pull on a little adult
  • 35:05data to try to give it some support.
  • 35:07You're actually,
  • 35:08I'm not familiar with the papers like ADHD,
  • 35:10so definitely to look those up,
  • 35:11but they kind of make sense.
  • 35:13ADHD is also distorted.
  • 35:14The frontal cortex,
  • 35:15something that I have been interested in is,
  • 35:17you know,
  • 35:18it's thought to be like a dopaminergic
  • 35:20deficit that occurs in ADHD.
  • 35:21That's why stimulant works so well.
  • 35:23And so I wonder what would
  • 35:24be the secondary changes in
  • 35:25dopaminergic innervation?
  • 35:26We looked kinda early,
  • 35:27but.
  • 35:28Is there some subtle change and
  • 35:29that's why antipsychotics work in
  • 35:31schizophrenia despite focusing?
  • 35:32I'm focusing on a non dopaminergic circuit,
  • 35:34so that's something that I probably
  • 35:36have to read into a lot more,
  • 35:38but it's really interesting
  • 35:40of like 80s ADHD symptoms.
  • 35:43There's a lot of evidence
  • 35:45out of Judie Rapoport's group,
  • 35:47that ADHD involves impaired development
  • 35:50of the right inferior frontal,
  • 35:52and one wonders if somehow that's
  • 35:55associated with reduced retinoic
  • 35:57acid in a mosaic sort of way.
  • 36:00I also wonder if is there any
  • 36:03evidence that inflammation reduces
  • 36:05retinoic acid since there's so
  • 36:08many ties between inflammatory
  • 36:10insults in utero and schizophrenia.
  • 36:13Definitely. I mean,
  • 36:13I think the inflammation thing
  • 36:14is is quite interesting, right?
  • 36:16How can we coincide both?
  • 36:17I mean, I just read a recent paper by
  • 36:19archive of more about the compliment pathway,
  • 36:21so that's something that could
  • 36:22be quite interesting.
  • 36:23And what I didn't touch it.
  • 36:25I think we talked a little
  • 36:26bit before awhile ago.
  • 36:27We were talking about the RA paper
  • 36:29is what we show in the human,
  • 36:31at least compared to the mouse.
  • 36:32And there's a great expansion of retinoic
  • 36:34acid signaling in the frontal cortex,
  • 36:35so you could see similar deficits may
  • 36:37be in the human that you don't see
  • 36:39in the mouse of these connectivity
  • 36:40changes in other regions of the
  • 36:42Association aspects of the frontal lobe.
  • 36:46Arctic, this was real.
  • 36:47Thank you.
  • 36:47This was really great and one
  • 36:49of the things I love when you
  • 36:51talk is that you always get us
  • 36:53to think about things in a
  • 36:55very different way. So I just had a
  • 36:57question about your idea about
  • 36:59stress and if you think of you know,
  • 37:01as you suggested, that you know
  • 37:03later around the skits of the brain.
  • 37:06Of the schizophrenic,
  • 37:07you know just can't manage all
  • 37:09the all the stress and all the the
  • 37:12the bombardment of demands on it.
  • 37:14Could you think
  • 37:15of those you know? Individuals early
  • 37:19on in childhood who are doing better
  • 37:21not being exposed to as much stress
  • 37:23and having much more early supports.
  • 37:25You know the the work of us in the
  • 37:28field of child psychiatry that would
  • 37:30somehow prepare them in a way to have
  • 37:33kind of a more resilient brain for
  • 37:35later on and hopes of doing better.
  • 37:37Does that make any
  • 37:39sense to you? I
  • 37:40really like that approach.
  • 37:41I think you know, it's really easy
  • 37:44to get very sized by in science.
  • 37:46You know, like we're going to virally
  • 37:48infect the brain changes gene
  • 37:50expression on this other thing, but.
  • 37:52I think we're quite far from that,
  • 37:54so building these skills very
  • 37:55early in these high risk children,
  • 37:57especially something like you know
  • 37:59resilience, skills or cognitive skills.
  • 38:00Now what I like about these types
  • 38:02of interventions is that the risk
  • 38:04profile is really low, right?
  • 38:06If we're stimming a circuit,
  • 38:07or we're giving retinoic acid or work
  • 38:09virally infecting the brain, right?
  • 38:11You have to be pretty confident
  • 38:13that this child is going to develop
  • 38:15a disorder that will be, you know,
  • 38:17quite dramatically, life altering.
  • 38:18And so I like building on these skills
  • 38:20because if this child doesn't convert
  • 38:22to psychosis even in the prodromal stage.
  • 38:24Data about a third of these high risk
  • 38:27patients actually develop schizophrenia,
  • 38:28and so building skills that I think
  • 38:31could help this child leaders and
  • 38:33help manage the stress which is
  • 38:35clearly been shown to affect kind
  • 38:37of severity of symptoms.
  • 38:38I think it just makes a ton of sense.
  • 38:42It just fits
  • 38:43so nicely with her model of lowering.
  • 38:45You know inflammatory insult
  • 38:46from stress and things.
  • 38:47So thank you very much.
  • 38:49I really enjoyed this very much.
  • 38:52Hi Christine.
  • 38:53Hi, this is Flora vaccarino.
  • 38:54I have a question very interesting so can
  • 38:57you go a little bit into the mechanism?
  • 39:00What do you think is happening here?
  • 39:02Do you think that there is a relative
  • 39:06deficit over 18 OIC acid that?
  • 39:08Somehow, directly or indirectly
  • 39:10stunts the growth of this connection
  • 39:12downstream connection to that Alamos.
  • 39:14Do you think there is a chemoattractant
  • 39:17role for 18 OIC acid in attracting
  • 39:20those connections in that alamaze or?
  • 39:22Or what do you think is going on?
  • 39:25Because retinoic acid is thought to
  • 39:27be a very important more for John
  • 39:30earlier on to write much earlier,
  • 39:31but it seems to be more in
  • 39:34posterior regions of the brain,
  • 39:36where here you're talking about something
  • 39:38specific for prefrontal cortex,
  • 39:39yeah, is that right? I mean can you?
  • 39:43Something that netted an in the lab is
  • 39:46talked about, kind of retinoic acid.
  • 39:47Historically we have been thought
  • 39:49of as a post theorizing factor,
  • 39:51especially in the hindbrain,
  • 39:52but I think then it has a more
  • 39:55complex theory of how it goes,
  • 39:56but I think your point is well taken overall,
  • 39:59that retinoic acid.
  • 40:00Is pleiotropic.
  • 40:01It's involved in almost every
  • 40:03process of brain development and
  • 40:04development overall and so I guess
  • 40:06to answer the couple questions and
  • 40:08remind me if I missed some of this.
  • 40:10First, what we show in the paper is
  • 40:12that retinoic acid during mid field
  • 40:14development is tightly regulated
  • 40:16to just the prefrontal cortex.
  • 40:18So there's actually the sink,
  • 40:19so there's 626B1 which is a retinoic
  • 40:21acid degrading enzyme which is
  • 40:23great enriched in the medial pre
  • 40:25installed in the motor cortex.
  • 40:26So that kind of restricts it to
  • 40:28and then we also showed various
  • 40:30different sources of synthesizing.
  • 40:32AXA of genes of both intrinsic
  • 40:34to the parenchyma itself as well
  • 40:36as inputs that are coming in from
  • 40:38other regions of the cortex that
  • 40:40seem to be expressing a retinoic
  • 40:42acid synthesizing enzyme.
  • 40:43So it seems to tightly restrict
  • 40:45it that mechanism that first
  • 40:46question you asked is something
  • 40:48that I'm very interested in.
  • 40:49So what is retinoic acid actually
  • 40:51doing in making this circuit?
  • 40:53And So what we looked at is so we have
  • 40:55we did RNA seek in our mouse model where
  • 40:58we reduced retinoic acid signaling a
  • 41:00specific the medial prefrontal cortex.
  • 41:02So we microdissected the
  • 41:04medial prefrontal cortex.
  • 41:05Orbital frontal cortex in the motor
  • 41:07cortex and looked at what's different
  • 41:08between the control and the wild type.
  • 41:10And we found a lot of target genes,
  • 41:13specifically adhesion molecules
  • 41:14instead of chemo attractants that
  • 41:16are seem to be at the synapse.
  • 41:17And so we're thinking that my kind of
  • 41:19general interest is in cortical connectivity,
  • 41:21especially long range connectivity,
  • 41:23and so we're I'm thinking is that
  • 41:25these axons are able to navigate the
  • 41:27striatum and all these cues there to
  • 41:29get to the doorstep of the cortex,
  • 41:31but they're not getting that final
  • 41:33chemoattractant that retinoic
  • 41:33acid is specifically regulating,
  • 41:35and so it's getting there and not.
  • 41:37To form the connection and then
  • 41:39actually kind of loses it and I forgot
  • 41:42something that Doctor King as it we see.
  • 41:45Reciprocal loss of glamo,
  • 41:46cortical prefrontal connections,
  • 41:47both Pfc to Mia dorsal thalamus
  • 41:49about Mr prefrontal cortex.
  • 41:51Yeah doctrina, that's a vacuum.
  • 41:52That's a great question,
  • 41:54and that's something that we're
  • 41:56really careful to show in the paper
  • 41:58that we're not altering cell number
  • 42:00or brain size or all these other
  • 42:02kind of factors that retinoic acid
  • 42:04is involved in seems to be very
  • 42:06selectively involved in kind of circuit.
  • 42:09Formation if you reduce it just to that
  • 42:11period, I'm just asking how you doing
  • 42:13there asking the obvious question.
  • 42:15Organize stuff.
  • 42:17You can easily test this in organoids
  • 42:18from Cortex entoloma schizophrenic,
  • 42:20personally definitely.
  • 42:20So this is something that
  • 42:22we're really interested in.
  • 42:23I I know that you're so far ahead in
  • 42:25the field and we've been reading a
  • 42:27lot of assembly papers in the paper.
  • 42:29I was really like the Pasco paper
  • 42:31that showed kind of like this
  • 42:33cortical striatal connection.
  • 42:34It's really interesting that
  • 42:35that connectivity can then maybe
  • 42:36alter the properties,
  • 42:37so I think that's a great mechanism
  • 42:39to study specifically human if
  • 42:41this is relevant in a human.
  • 42:42So I agree, yeah,
  • 42:43I should have actually figured it out.
  • 42:47Find the results and the way we're
  • 42:50developing a system where you can develop,
  • 42:53organize from different brain regions
  • 42:55using gradients of morphogens,
  • 42:57so that could be also used to actually
  • 43:00specifically do at Alamo Alamo
  • 43:02particles system that you could use.
  • 43:05That sounds great.
  • 43:06Yeah, 'cause it is. Great
  • 43:09idea. Yeah thank
  • 43:10can I can I? I'm delighted to see how
  • 43:14much interest your talk has taken and
  • 43:17I want to go in another direction.
  • 43:19I want to go from organoid to humanoid
  • 43:21because one of the things then Karthik.
  • 43:24We've had the pleasure of working
  • 43:26together and I've seen you as a
  • 43:28clinician and your clinician as well.
  • 43:30And you know the the experience of
  • 43:32individuals with schizophrenia,
  • 43:33which is something that we see
  • 43:35and I just want to in the chat,
  • 43:37which has been very active. Angie,
  • 43:39thank you for mentioning gating systems.
  • 43:41You know there's a question about gating.
  • 43:44I want to call your attention and to
  • 43:46everyone and there's this wonderful
  • 43:48book called Hidden Valley Rd that
  • 43:51came out last year about a family that
  • 43:53had 7 / 12 siblings had schizophrenia
  • 43:55and what's interesting is both
  • 43:57the experience of schizophrenia.
  • 43:58But this is a family that was followed very,
  • 44:02very closely by Robert Friedman.
  • 44:03You know,
  • 44:04the great psychiatrist in Colorado
  • 44:06who was the pioneer of gating
  • 44:08so highly recommended.
  • 44:09But if you could comment something
  • 44:11about gating and then moving
  • 44:13in the humanoid direction.
  • 44:14I wanna after you answer that I want to
  • 44:16call our attention to Doctor Yankee Yazgan,
  • 44:18who is our colleague from Turkey
  • 44:20who is joining us here and he
  • 44:21will have a comment after you met.
  • 44:23Maybe you mentioned that thank
  • 44:24you for great great talk Arctic.
  • 44:26Yeah,
  • 44:26I think getting
  • 44:27it first before I actually reflect on
  • 44:28some of my time back on when he wanted
  • 44:31something that did strike me when I
  • 44:33was there 'cause we were actually doing
  • 44:34the work while I was on when he won.
  • 44:36My last run. Was that some of the kids
  • 44:39that come in with early onset psychosis
  • 44:40or a couple of stick in my head.
  • 44:42I won't give the names obviously.
  • 44:44But they showed distinct low IQ's.
  • 44:46I think they were running in the
  • 44:4860s and 70s and so that kind of was
  • 44:51something that I always kind of brewing
  • 44:53that idea as as I was going there.
  • 44:55Yet gaining is a really interesting
  • 44:56question right?
  • 44:57Especially for positive symptoms, right?
  • 44:58Can you gate sensory information?
  • 45:00That's going into the cortex right?
  • 45:02And and there's great work by Al Powers.
  • 45:04Who's in the adult Department?
  • 45:05He's a young faculty of the question.
  • 45:07It is actually the information that's going
  • 45:09in from the founders of the cortex incorrect?
  • 45:12Or is the cortex processing
  • 45:13that information incorrectly?
  • 45:14And I think that kind of.
  • 45:16Really eludes to kind of what I was
  • 45:18saying without trying to put my foot
  • 45:20down that as the you're getting abnormal
  • 45:22extra connectivity with these sensory
  • 45:24areas between the thalamus in the cortex,
  • 45:27so maybe you're getting too much
  • 45:29information going in and you're not able
  • 45:31to really parse what's truly occurring
  • 45:33as well as a kind of background noise,
  • 45:35and that could be a nice kind of circuit.
  • 45:38Kind of psychobiology kind of thought
  • 45:40about how you create a hallucination or a
  • 45:43delusion or disorganized thought as well.
  • 45:45Any of the positive symptoms.
  • 45:46To be honest,
  • 45:47so on an international angle,
  • 45:49we're going first to Turkey and
  • 45:51then to France. So Yankee asking,
  • 45:53and then Lilia Benoit Yankee, please.
  • 45:57Just hi there everybody.
  • 45:59So glad to be back in New
  • 46:01Haven, at least for a couple
  • 46:04of weeks after 25 years.
  • 46:06So I've been following what's
  • 46:08going on at child study closely,
  • 46:10but haven't had the chance to be in
  • 46:13grand rounds for awhile. Thanks for.
  • 46:17Including me and great to see
  • 46:20friend friendly faces and friends.
  • 46:22I'm really impressed by the
  • 46:24quality of the work that says that
  • 46:28seemed to that will be changing
  • 46:31the way we deal with these cases,
  • 46:34but so if you questions or
  • 46:37minor questions probably.
  • 46:38I'm also I have been interested
  • 46:41in the neuromotor signs as an
  • 46:44indicator of future psychopathology
  • 46:46an my my question would be.
  • 46:49How would how is the the diagnostic
  • 46:53specifity established either
  • 46:55schizophrenia or aspergers or
  • 46:58whatever based on these findings?
  • 47:01I mean can we replace schizophrenia
  • 47:04with ASD or with another
  • 47:07new developmental disorder
  • 47:09on neurological terms? Thank you party, nice
  • 47:13nice meeting you by the way.
  • 47:18That's a great question.
  • 47:19Even the initial paper by Elaine
  • 47:21Walker specifically said that
  • 47:22this is not specific, right?
  • 47:24Like there, other disorders ASD that
  • 47:26showed these same type of things.
  • 47:28So I think the diagnostic.
  • 47:31Capacity of these things
  • 47:33is probably quite for,
  • 47:34I think, over all right.
  • 47:35I think there might be
  • 47:37something subtle I mean,
  • 47:38and so if you look at the kind of the listed,
  • 47:41all the criteria they looked
  • 47:43at through motor right?
  • 47:44So it was like increased tone
  • 47:46like abnormal hand movements.
  • 47:47All these things that we could see in a
  • 47:50lot of developmental disorders because
  • 47:51they if we think of cerebral palsy,
  • 47:53is like the classic developmental disorder.
  • 47:55Some sort of deficit in
  • 47:57circuit formation early.
  • 47:58They show these signs very distinctly,
  • 48:00so I think the.
  • 48:01Specificity is probably extremely
  • 48:02low for these types of things,
  • 48:04but I think that as we start turning
  • 48:07our focus to early in earlier stages,
  • 48:09which is what these perspective
  • 48:11studies are so exciting to me,
  • 48:13I think maybe we can narrow it
  • 48:15down to something specific.
  • 48:17One thing I didn't know in these
  • 48:19studies is that patients that later
  • 48:21developed bipolar disorder which
  • 48:22have you know a similar psychotic
  • 48:25E type presentation untreated,
  • 48:26those time control don't show
  • 48:28these motor signs,
  • 48:29and so that's I thought quite interesting.
  • 48:31But I think ASD and Sara
  • 48:33Palsy and schizophrenic.
  • 48:34There's a lot of overlap and
  • 48:36I think disease pathology.
  • 48:37You know,
  • 48:38both in the time period that specifically
  • 48:40sensitive the cells that are affected,
  • 48:42the jeans that are effective.
  • 48:44There seems to be similar processes
  • 48:45that are affected or altered and why
  • 48:48they manifest is different disorders.
  • 48:49I think that's still a long way away,
  • 48:52and it needs a lot of research to
  • 48:54make sense that thanks very much.
  • 48:58Paid Lilia and
  • 48:59then Ellen Hoffman. High thank
  • 49:01you very much. Much chaotic.
  • 49:03So just a comment.
  • 49:05We had just seeing that the 30 years
  • 49:08of research carried on clinical
  • 49:10transition by our Australian colleagues,
  • 49:13for instance, Patrick Mcgorry trying to
  • 49:16to build clinical tools like the cars
  • 49:19and following clinical subjects like
  • 49:21we see with the drama that it's not
  • 49:24really working as as we would have reached 2.
  • 49:28Because it was a great hope to
  • 49:30to have those clinical tools and
  • 49:32psychoeducation and omega-3 etc.
  • 49:35So I mean, it's really important to see
  • 49:37that we still need the fundamental research,
  • 49:40so that's still very much needed,
  • 49:43so it's interesting.
  • 49:46But I think that's something that was
  • 49:50also interesting in this clinical
  • 49:52assessment was to build strong cohorts an,
  • 49:55so that's something that they
  • 49:57they really managed to do,
  • 49:59and I'm wondering because.
  • 50:00Cohorts that you cited include
  • 50:03very few children.
  • 50:04So do you know of ongoing
  • 50:08cohort that would maybe?
  • 50:10Gather better data on neuromuscular skills,
  • 50:12etc.
  • 50:13Yeah yeah,
  • 50:13yeah yeah,
  • 50:14I'm new to
  • 50:15them or the clinical world as I
  • 50:18actually recently talked to one
  • 50:20of our graduates from our program,
  • 50:22Jerome Doctor Taylor,
  • 50:23who's now faculty at Penn,
  • 50:25and he's working as part of the
  • 50:28lifespan lifespan group with the girls,
  • 50:30and so they have this larger study.
  • 50:33I don't know the exact things
  • 50:35like Philadelphia, children,
  • 50:36something where they're really
  • 50:38deeply profiling children,
  • 50:39just agnostically kids that.
  • 50:41Initially, like a lot of the
  • 50:42work was focused on stress and
  • 50:44like kind of exposure to trauma.
  • 50:46We heard like a presentation from we
  • 50:48went there for a treat a couple years ago,
  • 50:51but they are actually interested
  • 50:52in psychosis and so they are
  • 50:54looking more closely at that group.
  • 50:56That same group actually does
  • 50:57looks at patients of 22 Q 11,
  • 50:59which is a genetic deletion that
  • 51:01has a 30% increase chance of
  • 51:03developing schizophrenia is way
  • 51:04higher than the normal population.
  • 51:05So there are also carefully built a
  • 51:07large cohort of these patients because
  • 51:09the issue is schizophrenia as it's rare.
  • 51:11Right?
  • 51:12Like even in highly focused
  • 51:13clinical populations of high risk,
  • 51:1530% convert.
  • 51:15And so if you're taking all takers,
  • 51:18you're going to get the population
  • 51:19rate and so something like 22 Q 11
  • 51:22are some of these genetic disorders
  • 51:24that you can pick out immediately.
  • 51:25Who has these, and you can really trace them.
  • 51:28Could be, I think,
  • 51:29a fruitful way of making sense of this.
  • 51:32But yeah, numbers,
  • 51:33you just gotta get higher and higher numbers.
  • 51:35But I think the Philadelphia
  • 51:37size actually is really cool work
  • 51:39in the garage slab there.
  • 51:41Can I just say before LL and will
  • 51:43have one last question and then
  • 51:45we can hang out with with Kartik?
  • 51:47We won't close a room but if people
  • 51:49need to leave but I just want to
  • 51:52call your attention to next week's
  • 51:54grand rounds which is going to be
  • 51:56a little bit continuing and some
  • 51:57related way so Kieran O'Donnell,
  • 51:59one of our newest faculty members is going
  • 52:01to be talking to us about early early,
  • 52:03early, early early years and the
  • 52:05impact on the developing brain.
  • 52:07So I think it's going to be very
  • 52:09in keeping with today's talk.
  • 52:10So Ellen last question for Karthik.
  • 52:13Carter
  • 52:13great great talk, I really enjoyed it.
  • 52:16You know, I guess as we think about a lot
  • 52:19of different questions that one question
  • 52:22which I wrote in the chat with you.
  • 52:25Sorry you arrived at the
  • 52:27retinoic acid pathway.
  • 52:28Sort of thinking about risk genes
  • 52:30or following the genes there.
  • 52:32So to what extent do you think that
  • 52:35this pathway and the thalamus to
  • 52:37prefrontal cortex connection might
  • 52:39represent a common pathway across?
  • 52:42Schizophrenia risk genes versus one
  • 52:44of several routes there and the sort
  • 52:46of related to the other question.
  • 52:48Previous question,
  • 52:49to what extent might this represent
  • 52:51a common pathway across other
  • 52:53neurodevelopmental disorders?
  • 52:54And that's such an important
  • 52:56question for us to
  • 52:57really be able to transition
  • 52:59this intervention right?
  • 53:00Because as this was a mouse model, right?
  • 53:03And we actually weren't
  • 53:05looking at schizophrenia,
  • 53:06we were really interesting.
  • 53:08Just prefrontal cortex evolution
  • 53:09and development when we initially
  • 53:11started this study and so retinoic
  • 53:13acid actually agnostically popped up.
  • 53:15There was some data before from
  • 53:17an earlier study in this testing
  • 53:18lab that pointed at retinoic acid,
  • 53:20and then we kind of reconfirmed
  • 53:23it and then analyzed it.
  • 53:25Retinoic acid specific.
  • 53:26There's a couple of papers that
  • 53:28have really kind of focused on
  • 53:30retinoic acid as as possible as
  • 53:31a possible signaling pathway.
  • 53:33I still I'm kind of wary about
  • 53:35specifically retinoic acid.
  • 53:36I think as Doctor Vaccarino mentioned
  • 53:38is I think something downstream
  • 53:40and also kind of putting together
  • 53:42with our current knowledge that
  • 53:44seems to be like a lot of the
  • 53:46schizophrenia associations an
  • 53:47autism will soon be at the synapse.
  • 53:49Maybe it's something specifically
  • 53:51about Synapse connectivity and we
  • 53:53just happened to stumble upon it by.
  • 53:55Going the retinoic acid path,
  • 53:56I do though to you later point.
  • 53:59I do think plama cortical dysfunction may
  • 54:01be some core pathology in the disorder,
  • 54:03and that I like that circuit based
  • 54:06focus versus like a signaling pathway,
  • 54:08because I feel like,
  • 54:09as I mentioned before,
  • 54:11like retinoic acid involved in everything.
  • 54:13So it would be really hard to target it,
  • 54:16and so I think there is some data that's
  • 54:18coming together that Flamel cortical
  • 54:20dysfunction is specifically an issue,
  • 54:22whether it's specifically
  • 54:23thalamic to prefrontal cortex.
  • 54:24I think that might be at
  • 54:27least a subpopulation.
  • 54:28Of it will be interesting,
  • 54:29kind of the gating question.
  • 54:31Maybe other Islamic deficits,
  • 54:32Lima cortical connectivity deficits
  • 54:33could explain other things.
  • 54:34And if you kind of read I I spent
  • 54:36a lot of time before this talk,
  • 54:39like kind of reading through all
  • 54:41this schizophrenia literature
  • 54:42because I spend more time reading
  • 54:43like gene regulation stuff and one
  • 54:45of the theories has put forward.
  • 54:47There's almost two types of schizophrenia.
  • 54:48There's like a classic honor
  • 54:50student type of schizophrenia,
  • 54:51word, psychotic symptoms,
  • 54:521st and then you kind of progressives
  • 54:54are very responsive to antipsychotic's.
  • 54:55But there's also the second
  • 54:57type which is very driven.
  • 54:58This cognitive negative symptoms,
  • 55:00so I wonder if, like you know,
  • 55:02one population is specifically shows
  • 55:04this Pfc Flamel cortical deficit,
  • 55:06and while his other population doesn't.
  • 55:08You know,
  • 55:08kind of phenotyping versus maybe
  • 55:10thalamic cortical connectivity.
  • 55:11Maybe something that spans across
  • 55:13these different endo phenotypes,
  • 55:14but I think we just need more close,
  • 55:17thoughtful thinking.
  • 55:18Like I guess,
  • 55:19characterization of these patients
  • 55:20of patients schizophrenia to
  • 55:22really kind of link a disorder.
  • 55:23But yeah,
  • 55:24I do think there's something with
  • 55:26the llama cortical connectivity.
  • 55:28It's always in some.
  • 55:29I've been interested in that may
  • 55:30underlie schizophrenia across the board.
  • 55:34Well Kartik, thank you for a
  • 55:36really magnificent grand rounds.
  • 55:37We're going to leave the room
  • 55:39open if anyone wants to join,
  • 55:41but I want to thank you.
  • 55:42And you've taught us a lot of
  • 55:44science science with a heart,
  • 55:46so that's that's really good.