Skip to Main Content

Alan Anticevic, PhD. February 2022

January 18, 2023

Title: Mapping neuro-behavioral heterogeneity of psychedelic neurobiology in humans

ID
9385

Transcript

  • 00:17Welcome, everyone.
  • 00:18It's a pleasure today to
  • 00:21have Alan antiseptic here.
  • 00:23Alan, I think many of you know,
  • 00:24is an associate professor in
  • 00:26the Department of Psychiatry.
  • 00:29As well as many other hats.
  • 00:31And his was a really a pioneer in our
  • 00:34in our community and bringing high
  • 00:37resolution human Connectome Project
  • 00:39style imaging to the community and
  • 00:42then applying innovative computational
  • 00:45strategies to analyzing these data.
  • 00:48And then in more recent years,
  • 00:49it started to do some some really
  • 00:52exciting work in looking at the
  • 00:55brain effects first of ketamine
  • 00:56in his earlier work and then
  • 00:59more recently of psilocybin,
  • 01:00LSD and other psychedelics,
  • 01:02which is the focus today.
  • 01:04So Alan,
  • 01:04thank you so much for being
  • 01:05with us and we look forward
  • 01:07to to learning from you.
  • 01:10Thanks, Chris. Mark Gustavo.
  • 01:13Anytime at all.
  • 01:15It's really with this pandemic,
  • 01:17I feel like I haven't seen you
  • 01:20guys in forever and it's feeling
  • 01:22more disconnected than ever,
  • 01:24but it's it's it's really.
  • 01:26I'm excited to tell you
  • 01:28what we've been up to so.
  • 01:30The the title is so let's see mapping
  • 01:35or behavioral heterogeneity of
  • 01:37psychedelic neurology and humans.
  • 01:38So that's. An overly ambitious,
  • 01:41probably not true title.
  • 01:42So we're not quite there
  • 01:44and this is aspirational.
  • 01:45And So what I'd like to kind of
  • 01:48talk about is as as Chris noted,
  • 01:50what are the techniques and approaches
  • 01:52we're bringing to bear towards
  • 01:53this goal and some of the work
  • 01:55that's been coming out of the lab.
  • 01:57So it will be a hybrid of sort of the, the,
  • 02:00the theme and the ethos of what we're doing,
  • 02:03how some of the efforts in in mapping
  • 02:08variation in clinical populations.
  • 02:11Can be related to neuroimaging
  • 02:14effects of pharmacological compounds
  • 02:16with the focus on psychedelics.
  • 02:18So that's kind of the idea,
  • 02:19right.
  • 02:19And so I I want to just disclose
  • 02:23that I'm a member of the TAB
  • 02:26Technology Advisory Board for Nemora
  • 02:30Therapeutic Therapeutics I consult.
  • 02:32For Gilgamesh and I just Co founded
  • 02:35a biotech with my colleague John
  • 02:38Murray that's called Manifest
  • 02:41Technologies and I I say this
  • 02:43really proudly because without the
  • 02:46support of Yale the spin out what it
  • 02:48would not be possible and so we're
  • 02:50we're really excited about this
  • 02:52work as it has this potential so.
  • 02:54Basically the way I kind of
  • 02:56think about the entire space.
  • 03:01Neuropsychiatric mapping is
  • 03:02really about the challenge and the
  • 03:04opportunity in front of us, right?
  • 03:06So there's two ways to think about it.
  • 03:08And why has this opportunity not
  • 03:11been realized in the field of brain
  • 03:13behavioral health and I'll use this term
  • 03:16brain behavioral health because I I I
  • 03:19actually think it's important that we
  • 03:22destigmatize this terminology, right.
  • 03:25So it's a difficulties in regulating
  • 03:29brain behavioral relationships that that
  • 03:32we're after and then I'll talk about a
  • 03:35framework for this quantitative and and.
  • 03:38The neurobiological framework for mapping
  • 03:40brain behavioral relationships with the
  • 03:42assistance obviously of pharmacological or
  • 03:44imaging as a key tool in order to do this,
  • 03:47right. So what's the challenge?
  • 03:50And so this is one of many challenges,
  • 03:52but in my mind a very important one,
  • 03:54right, which is heterogeneity.
  • 03:56And by heterogeneity,
  • 03:57I mean both within a human being over
  • 04:00time and across people in relation
  • 04:03to brain behavioral variation, right.
  • 04:06And so this is an old problem.
  • 04:09We know this, right?
  • 04:10And so, but there are.
  • 04:12Questions that arise because of this problem
  • 04:16and the opportunity in front of us is if we.
  • 04:19Have a drug like any compound.
  • 04:23And psychedelics are a great example,
  • 04:26right?
  • 04:26How do we select the optimal person
  • 04:29who will benefit from that compound?
  • 04:31We don't have a principled quantitative,
  • 04:33rationally guided framework for this,
  • 04:36for anything in our field.
  • 04:38And then in the future,
  • 04:40right,
  • 04:40if we do have a molecule right
  • 04:42before we do not have a molecule,
  • 04:43if we don't have a compound that
  • 04:45crosses the blood brain barrier
  • 04:47safely or any therapeutic,
  • 04:48how do we develop one with
  • 04:50individual precision as the goal,
  • 04:52not patients versus controls,
  • 04:55but individual people, right.
  • 04:57So that's the opportunity as I see it,
  • 05:00right and so.
  • 05:01It's that how can we target
  • 05:03specific people with?
  • 05:05Quantitative precision.
  • 05:06So this problem I see is mapping one
  • 05:10to many levels of analysis, right?
  • 05:12This is really what stands in front of us,
  • 05:14so I don't have to tell you guys that.
  • 05:17Polygenic disturbances and variants.
  • 05:22Variations with rare mutations, right?
  • 05:27Basically those are the two lowest
  • 05:29level possibilities that our field
  • 05:31is studying and how they can affect.
  • 05:34Molecules, synapses and cells and the
  • 05:36balance between those cells, right?
  • 05:38That's that's at the very
  • 05:40baseline of the problem, right?
  • 05:42In turn,
  • 05:43how do we take that information and
  • 05:45map it onto system level observation?
  • 05:48Some people would say this is an ill
  • 05:49posed problem because it's impossible.
  • 05:51There's just too many mappings.
  • 05:52But you know,
  • 05:53I'll leave that for debate later.
  • 05:55And then finally,
  • 05:56how do we link this to Spectra of
  • 05:59behavioral disturbances, right.
  • 06:00And so I'd like to argue that this
  • 06:02mapping is fundamentally unknown.
  • 06:04We don't know it,
  • 06:05and if somebody argues that we do,
  • 06:06I think that they're flying.
  • 06:09Even in circuits where we understand
  • 06:11our biology very well, like fear,
  • 06:13we still can't treat PTSD, right?
  • 06:15So just this mapping is not accomplished,
  • 06:18right?
  • 06:19And I actually think that system
  • 06:21level observations at the
  • 06:22level of neural systems is where the
  • 06:24right link to behavior should be,
  • 06:26not at the level of a synapse,
  • 06:28because the heterogeneity just explodes,
  • 06:30the combinatorics become impossible to
  • 06:32intractable to quantify or deal with.
  • 06:35So why have we not solved this problem right?
  • 06:39Why is this opportunity not been realized?
  • 06:41And so I think that.
  • 06:44I might argue my many reasons,
  • 06:46but there's some legacy barriers,
  • 06:48right, that we're still
  • 06:49trying to overcome as a field.
  • 06:50And I and the legacy approach are called
  • 06:54legacy because it's historically important
  • 06:57to acknowledge that this this is.
  • 07:00The framework that we have been
  • 07:02operating under is has tremendous
  • 07:03utility for what it was designed
  • 07:05for and by that I mean DSM, right?
  • 07:07It does what it was built for to do,
  • 07:11which is it reliably gets me and
  • 07:14everybody else to agree that some person
  • 07:18has X out of P symptoms over T time.
  • 07:22That it does quantitatively accomplishes
  • 07:24that, so we can reliably agree.
  • 07:27To categorize a human being as
  • 07:29you showed X symptoms out of,
  • 07:32you know, some rubric overtime.
  • 07:35And then we give you a label.
  • 07:36I'd like to argue that's ill
  • 07:38fitting for what we need,
  • 07:39actually not wrong for what it was
  • 07:42built to do, just not what we need.
  • 07:45And then further,
  • 07:46we lack data and methods to
  • 07:47quantitatively molecular benchmark
  • 07:49brain behavior relationships, right?
  • 07:51So the legacy approach doesn't have it.
  • 07:52And then we don't have technological
  • 07:54solutions to actually scale this with
  • 07:56the kinds of observations that are so
  • 07:58important in the area of psychedelic
  • 08:00medicine for precision therapeutics,
  • 08:02right.
  • 08:02So this is a dystopian vision of the future,
  • 08:07right,
  • 08:07that I'd like to show sometimes like
  • 08:10basically where we use computer assisted
  • 08:12decision making or machine learning informed.
  • 08:15Decisions with multimodal data where
  • 08:16the brain is in the middle, right?
  • 08:18I'm not taking the person out of this.
  • 08:20I'm not, you know,
  • 08:21reductionist to that point,
  • 08:23but the organ has to be in
  • 08:25the middle in my mind.
  • 08:27And then we're hopefully optimizing these
  • 08:29two decisions through an iterative cycle,
  • 08:32right?
  • 08:32But we're not there and and so,
  • 08:34So what do we do about that?
  • 08:36So my group here and our other collaborators
  • 08:40here at Yale are really approaching this in,
  • 08:43in the following way.
  • 08:45So let me walk you through the framework.
  • 08:48So we have to 1st agree that the
  • 08:52neurobehavioral mapping problem is
  • 08:54quantitatively A must, we have to do that.
  • 08:57Correctly.
  • 08:57And I'll explain what I mean by that, right?
  • 08:59Because if if we are using an
  • 09:02ill fitting framework to map
  • 09:04onto neurobehavioral variation,
  • 09:05then just won't work.
  • 09:07We won't even translate what we want.
  • 09:10Gene expression alterations right
  • 09:12and disturbances in the way
  • 09:14that the circuits are formed are
  • 09:17it's useful information.
  • 09:19We harness that from say the island
  • 09:22human brain Atlas to inform our our
  • 09:24our models that then can simulate
  • 09:27pharmacological fMRI effects which in
  • 09:29turn can be fixed individual people.
  • 09:31So this is just one way
  • 09:33that we're approaching this
  • 09:35problem in the area of just mental
  • 09:37health and and psychedelic pharmacology.
  • 09:39Specifically, but there's there's many
  • 09:41other tools that we're leaving on the table,
  • 09:44but this is what I'm going to talk about,
  • 09:46right. So again I'd like to argue
  • 09:47that you know Chris asked me to talk
  • 09:50about neuroimaging today specifically
  • 09:51and to really have that focus.
  • 09:53So I'd like to argue that new imaging
  • 09:56is not an option anymore in our work.
  • 09:59And by that I mean normalizing broadly,
  • 10:01right? But a necessity, because here,
  • 10:04here's a choice, right?
  • 10:05This is literally what we had on the table,
  • 10:08right? But we need this.
  • 10:12And so I'd like to argue that if if you
  • 10:15present the problem this way, right,
  • 10:16like how can we not leverage brain data
  • 10:19in the service of decision making as a must,
  • 10:22specifically for things as complicated
  • 10:24as psychedelic neurobiology at
  • 10:26the individual level, right.
  • 10:28So.
  • 10:28So, OK, so how can we exploit imaging
  • 10:31in the service of this goal?
  • 10:33So that's the rest of the talk.
  • 10:35So for those of you who haven't
  • 10:38really thought about.
  • 10:40Structural functional
  • 10:41multimodal imaging recently,
  • 10:43this is a very useful reminder of the
  • 10:45resolution of what imaging straddles,
  • 10:47right.
  • 10:47So on the Y axis is the size of
  • 10:50the observation and time is on the
  • 10:54the X axis and and you'll notice
  • 10:56that human imaging across modality
  • 10:58straddles a good bit of the space.
  • 11:00We're not helpless right.
  • 11:01Like we can actually measure signals
  • 11:04in various ways in relation to various
  • 11:06phenomena, but we still are, you know,
  • 11:08out of reach of certain levels of analysis.
  • 11:10Like with human imaging with symptoms
  • 11:12improving but still not quite there.
  • 11:14And we also don't have just one
  • 11:16way to measure this,
  • 11:17right.
  • 11:17We have multiple modalities that now can
  • 11:20combine that give you different slices
  • 11:23of the signal that this incredibly
  • 11:26complex piece of tissue produces,
  • 11:28right.
  • 11:29And so,
  • 11:29so I'm going to talk about specifically
  • 11:32throughout the rest of talk about bold F MRI,
  • 11:34right, which is a measure that is is.
  • 11:39One of the expertise areas in my group,
  • 11:41right so.
  • 11:44Specifically,
  • 11:44the phenomena that for the rest
  • 11:46of the the talk will focus on is
  • 11:49this idea of resting state, right?
  • 11:51And so this is now a household name.
  • 11:53I, you know,
  • 11:54I don't have to go over this
  • 11:55in a lot of detail,
  • 11:56but I'd like to kind of just
  • 11:58review the history of it.
  • 11:59I always like to do this because I
  • 12:01like to remind people that in 1995,
  • 12:03right, broad Biswal.
  • 12:05Actually observe this simply
  • 12:08by taking signal.
  • 12:10Out of 1 hemisphere of the motor
  • 12:13cortex and asking where in the
  • 12:15brain is there time dependent
  • 12:17covariation of signal within a person
  • 12:20and then average across people.
  • 12:21And he saw this map right which is
  • 12:24bilateral motor cortex comes out.
  • 12:26And people thought this was junk.
  • 12:28This was a compound, right?
  • 12:30And they ignored it.
  • 12:31In fact, he was attacked for it quite a lot,
  • 12:33right? And then only a
  • 12:36decade later with some.
  • 12:40Advances from Michael Greicius
  • 12:41and then Marcus Raichle.
  • 12:43Did this phenomena really
  • 12:45become a mainstream?
  • 12:46And so now if you Fast forward
  • 12:48what is now a decade ago,
  • 12:49which is hard to believe right,
  • 12:50that Thomas Yao and Randy Buckner
  • 12:54actually mapped comprehensively right.
  • 12:56Large scale networks
  • 12:58across individuals right.
  • 12:59And this is this is now a joke
  • 13:03like we we know we can do this now
  • 13:04and every single person right.
  • 13:05But it was controversial
  • 13:07then and so then 2016 onward.
  • 13:09Human connectome produced sparse
  • 13:11relation which gives you an index of
  • 13:14the boundaries between the areas that
  • 13:16is comprehensive but not definitive.
  • 13:18This will probably improve it or actively,
  • 13:20but I'd like to tell you like we've
  • 13:22made some serious progress, right,
  • 13:23with human or imaging and we can exploit it,
  • 13:26right?
  • 13:27And furthermore,
  • 13:28this is something I will repeat
  • 13:31in every talk I give.
  • 13:34So the reason why the Human
  • 13:36Connectome project pipelines and the
  • 13:38entire effort towards so impactful,
  • 13:40which is why I obviously drink the
  • 13:42kool-aid because I trained there, but.
  • 13:46The human brain and the cortical
  • 13:49mantle is A2 dimensional surface
  • 13:51wrapped around white matter that is
  • 13:54about 4 millimeters thick and it's the
  • 13:57size of a pizza and that geometry matters.
  • 13:59It matters fundamentally when you're
  • 14:01going to do precision medicine
  • 14:03analytics with single subject
  • 14:05human cortical surfaces, right?
  • 14:07In fact,
  • 14:07just before this talk I got off
  • 14:09the call with one of my students,
  • 14:10Amber Howell,
  • 14:11deeply debating the importance of
  • 14:13importance of social curvature
  • 14:14and depth on a single subject
  • 14:16level in relation to.
  • 14:17Diffusivity of white matter tracts,
  • 14:19and turns out it matters.
  • 14:20It matters a lot anyway.
  • 14:23Analyzing your data on the surface,
  • 14:25I think is is. I must like you.
  • 14:29You are blind without that to
  • 14:31individual variation, right?
  • 14:33So.
  • 14:34When you do this right,
  • 14:36then you can produce metrics in the
  • 14:39geometry of the cortical surface
  • 14:41that can quantify some signal in
  • 14:44every cortical parcel or area.
  • 14:46I'll use the term parcel because
  • 14:48that's the formal definition.
  • 14:49And then you can operate with those
  • 14:51metrics to analyze it within a
  • 14:53subject across people in various ways,
  • 14:55right.
  • 14:55So furthermore,
  • 14:56what we've done in my group out of necessity,
  • 14:59right,
  • 15:00we started to use the cortical parcellation
  • 15:02from the Human Connectome Project,
  • 15:04which is shown here.
  • 15:06These little borders,
  • 15:07but then we use the network partitions from
  • 15:09from other groups and they were great,
  • 15:12they they worked really well.
  • 15:13But then we realized subcortical
  • 15:15coverage is not there.
  • 15:17Like thalamus wasn't covered appropriately,
  • 15:19brainstem wasn't covered.
  • 15:20And you guys know that psychiatric
  • 15:23medication works on subcortical
  • 15:25circuits and precision of isolating
  • 15:28specific voxels in relation to
  • 15:30networks and parcels is really important.
  • 15:32So what I'm showing you here is
  • 15:34work produced by my student Lisa.
  • 15:36Who's extended the cortical partition
  • 15:39into networks Cortically first,
  • 15:41and then studied how subcortical
  • 15:44voxels covary with those networks,
  • 15:47assigning every single voxel in
  • 15:49subcortical brain matter to a
  • 15:52network until it reached split half?
  • 15:55Stability across the entire HCP sample.
  • 15:58And so the reason why we did
  • 15:59this out of necessity is we need
  • 16:01this for clinical application.
  • 16:02We actually needed a whole brain partition
  • 16:04to covers every piece of Gray matter.
  • 16:06We can't leave things on the table.
  • 16:08So now this is published and this is what
  • 16:10we're going to be using for us to talk.
  • 16:11Now just to convince you that
  • 16:13this actually is better, right.
  • 16:15So you can take a dense signal, right?
  • 16:18In other words, at the level of vertex
  • 16:19on a cortical surface, you can parse,
  • 16:22relate it prior to computing some
  • 16:23metric or you can parse relate it.
  • 16:25Post right?
  • 16:26So if the person relation is.
  • 16:29Valid and consistent across subjects,
  • 16:32then this should be better than that.
  • 16:34And that's in fact true, right?
  • 16:35And so this is not news,
  • 16:37not Glasser has shown this
  • 16:38in his work and so on,
  • 16:39but it's just to convince you guys that
  • 16:42these parcellation are actually very,
  • 16:44very useful, not just quantitatively,
  • 16:46but as a feature space reduction,
  • 16:49because now you are no longer
  • 16:50working with 95,000 voxels,
  • 16:52you're working with 700 areas
  • 16:53and now you can do some clever
  • 16:55feature engineering on top of that.
  • 16:57So that matters, right?
  • 16:59So how do we now link this to
  • 17:02molecular mechanism, right,
  • 17:04very broadly speaking and so.
  • 17:07So, so,
  • 17:07so the way we do this and the
  • 17:09way we've approached this across
  • 17:11all compounds ketamine,
  • 17:13single sideband as well as application
  • 17:17to clinical questions is what
  • 17:21are the principal organizational.
  • 17:26Features, if you will, of the human
  • 17:28brain or the mammalian brain in general,
  • 17:30and I'd like to argue one that we know about,
  • 17:33is functional specialization
  • 17:34across the cortical axis.
  • 17:37So we know that lower order and higher
  • 17:39order areas have very distinct patterns of
  • 17:41feed forward and back connections, right?
  • 17:44So that's an organizational principle.
  • 17:45This is a classic picture from the
  • 17:47Fellman and Vanessa and publication,
  • 17:49which also highlights that there is a
  • 17:52hierarchy and information processing from.
  • 17:55Answer to association agents, right?
  • 17:57We also know from work such as this
  • 18:01paper from John Murray while he was
  • 18:04starting here at Yale that there
  • 18:06is a difference in the spontaneous
  • 18:09autocorrelated activity across areas
  • 18:11from non human primate data and this
  • 18:15intrinsic activity scales suggesting
  • 18:17a hierarchy of functional hierarchy.
  • 18:19Furthermore,
  • 18:20we know that during cognitive operations
  • 18:23such as working memory primary.
  • 18:25Visual areas such as Mt do not sustain
  • 18:29signal during the mnemonic phase,
  • 18:32whereas areas such as LP FC sustain
  • 18:35a recurrent reverberatory activity,
  • 18:38right again highlighting distinct
  • 18:40functional specialization in this
  • 18:42very coarse way.
  • 18:43Furthermore, we know that we can leverage.
  • 18:47Microstructure values from the T1 and T2
  • 18:51maps to derive a proxy of myelin cortically,
  • 18:56which shows a hierarchy.
  • 18:58It smoothly varies from
  • 19:00association to sensory areas.
  • 19:02And this is also true in the macaque,
  • 19:05right?
  • 19:05So now we have some clues that the
  • 19:07brain varies hierarchically and this
  • 19:09shouldn't be controversial, right?
  • 19:11Can we leverage this information to
  • 19:15understand how the effects of pharmacology?
  • 19:18Which we'll get to and so.
  • 19:20The the motivation for this work was
  • 19:23driven by a grad student in John's lab,
  • 19:26Josh Burt, who did some really
  • 19:28elegant gene expression mapping.
  • 19:30And I'll walk you through why this matters,
  • 19:31right.
  • 19:31So we have this human myelin map, right?
  • 19:34We've established that it has a
  • 19:36hierarchical organization, right.
  • 19:37And what he's just shown that when
  • 19:39you use Lisas network partition,
  • 19:41but in fact there is a difference
  • 19:43across networks,
  • 19:44right,
  • 19:44that the association networks have less
  • 19:47myelin than sensory somatomotor networks.
  • 19:49OK, just the validity check.
  • 19:51Furthermore,
  • 19:51he's taken mcac myelin values,
  • 19:54and he's taken tracer data,
  • 19:56defining the hierarchy as feed
  • 19:58forward and feedback connections in
  • 20:00collaboration with David Vanessa's group.
  • 20:02And he's correlated.
  • 20:03This again establishing that
  • 20:05there is hierarchy right.
  • 20:07So far,
  • 20:07so good.
  • 20:08Then he went into the alien human
  • 20:11Brain Atlas gene expression database.
  • 20:14And he's taken from every cortical
  • 20:18microarray a probe across something
  • 20:20like 20,000 different genes.
  • 20:23And because the amount of human
  • 20:25brain Atlas has serendipitously,
  • 20:27thank goodness,
  • 20:28scanned every single person of
  • 20:30the six people that they studied.
  • 20:33Postmortem.
  • 20:33We could then reconstruct the cortical
  • 20:37surface right anatomy postmortem and
  • 20:39map it onto the human Connectome Atlas.
  • 20:43Particulate it right using the
  • 20:45parcellation that I just introduced.
  • 20:47Do this for every person from the
  • 20:50island human brain Atlas and then
  • 20:52average it to get the aggregate
  • 20:54group map across every gene
  • 20:57for every cortical parcel.
  • 20:58Now you can imagine why this is powerful.
  • 21:00Now we have an actual gene expression
  • 21:02topography for every gene across
  • 21:04all the areas that we have our
  • 21:05new imaging maps for, right?
  • 21:07So what can we do with this?
  • 21:09So the first question is,
  • 21:10what is the principal gradient,
  • 21:12the first principal component of
  • 21:15gene expression topography, right.
  • 21:16And this is the picture.
  • 21:18This is how it varies, right?
  • 21:19And it varies this way,
  • 21:22so much so that it
  • 21:23did you say that this this expression
  • 21:26is from 6 brains, six people. OK.
  • 21:29So I'm wondering how that limits the power
  • 21:31of this kind of correlation analysis.
  • 21:33If you only have you have a huge number
  • 21:35of genes but you only have 6 replicates
  • 21:38at each for each gene in each parcel,
  • 21:41that's a great question.
  • 21:41So let me go back.
  • 21:43So, So what Chris is really
  • 21:45highlighting is something that is.
  • 21:48Rental, which is you when you're running any
  • 21:52analysis on the matrix of genes by parcels,
  • 21:55notice that their group averaged, right?
  • 21:58So what we do is we first ask, what is the?
  • 22:02Coverage of that microarray probe
  • 22:04in that cortical location, right.
  • 22:07Is there a good signal?
  • 22:08And then we evaluate the differential
  • 22:10stability across individuals, right.
  • 22:12So we want to be confident that it's
  • 22:14consistent across these six people,
  • 22:15right, and that there's good coverage.
  • 22:17Then we produce a single value single number,
  • 22:20which is the average right
  • 22:21across these six people.
  • 22:23All the analysis are done on the data
  • 22:25object that's you're seeing here,
  • 22:27which is at the group parcel level.
  • 22:29In other words,
  • 22:30the principal component does not consider.
  • 22:32Variation across people,
  • 22:34it considers variation across areas,
  • 22:36right,
  • 22:37with the assumption obviously that
  • 22:39people are consistently expressing
  • 22:40these genes in these areas and
  • 22:42that's an empirical question,
  • 22:43right?
  • 22:44One that we keep talking to NIH
  • 22:45and the on human brain that was
  • 22:47folks that they need more brains.
  • 22:49They need to produce this kind of
  • 22:52mapping a sample of sufficient size
  • 22:54that we can interrogate whether
  • 22:56there is true human variability.
  • 22:59And furthermore,
  • 23:00you could imagine this matters for
  • 23:02psychedelic cardiology which is if your 5 HD.
  • 23:04Receptors,
  • 23:04which I'll talk about in a second,
  • 23:06very differentiated across people.
  • 23:07One could argue that some of the
  • 23:09conclusions that will draw are incorrect,
  • 23:11but I think you're,
  • 23:12you're you're as always always go
  • 23:15to the key intuition right away,
  • 23:17which is that these maps are
  • 23:19average across people and therefore
  • 23:22limits generalizability across
  • 23:23the the entire population, right.
  • 23:26We don't know
  • 23:27and also implies that your principal
  • 23:29components are dependent on multiple genes.
  • 23:31They're looking for patterns
  • 23:32of multiple genes across areas
  • 23:33they're not going to give. Exactly.
  • 23:35Whereas if you had a much larger data set,
  • 23:37you could look for components that
  • 23:39are for grades within single genes,
  • 23:41but you can't do that in this data set, so.
  • 23:44Exactly, exactly.
  • 23:45So you could ask the question of is
  • 23:48there a gradient of a single gene across
  • 23:51people in an area or across areas, right.
  • 23:54That's another level of variance
  • 23:55that is left on the table.
  • 23:57But the first question,
  • 23:59the only question really that we could ask
  • 24:01was what's the spatial gradient, right?
  • 24:03What is the spatial topography of the way
  • 24:06these genes vary across cortical areas,
  • 24:08right. And this is how they vary.
  • 24:11And so it turns out that that explains
  • 24:14almost 30% of all the variants, right?
  • 24:16Not, not everything, but a lot, right?
  • 24:18And so now you could say,
  • 24:20oh, what does this look like?
  • 24:21What kind of looks like Milan, right?
  • 24:23So you could quantify that, right?
  • 24:24And it turns out it looks a lot like my own.
  • 24:27In other words,
  • 24:29it varies along a hierarchy, right?
  • 24:31Almost 1/3 of all the variants in
  • 24:34human gene expression in the adult
  • 24:36brains with this these six people
  • 24:39varies along the principal axis,
  • 24:41where it's high in sensory
  • 24:44somatomotor or low.
  • 24:46And low or high in association cortices,
  • 24:50right?
  • 24:51This is this gradient can go both ways,
  • 24:53right?
  • 24:54And this is cool,
  • 24:55because now you can imagine this is
  • 24:57the key way that the brain breaks,
  • 24:59which is across this cortical hierarchy
  • 25:00and its pattern of gene expression.
  • 25:04Can you then look at what genes
  • 25:07are contributing substantially to
  • 25:09that first principal component?
  • 25:11Because you would predict that,
  • 25:13for example, oligodendrocyte genes
  • 25:16might contribute significantly.
  • 25:17So if oligodendrocyte genes covary
  • 25:19with the myelin density map, that's.
  • 25:21A different way of measuring
  • 25:23exactly the same thing.
  • 25:25It's not telling you something new as
  • 25:26opposed to if there are other neural
  • 25:28genes that might be less intuitive
  • 25:29that they should vary and that
  • 25:30might be telling you something new.
  • 25:31So can you dig into the contributors
  • 25:33to this component and start to
  • 25:35make that kind of inference.
  • 25:36Yeah of course you can check what's
  • 25:38the loading of each gene onto this
  • 25:40component and so on so definitely so.
  • 25:42So I don't know if Josh has that
  • 25:44in one of the tables are not
  • 25:45in the paper but we can check.
  • 25:47It's a really good question right and and
  • 25:49allows for another access to exploration
  • 25:51but the point that you're already.
  • 25:53You know, your your questions and and
  • 25:55suggestions are already highlighting
  • 25:57this which is the cortical gene
  • 25:59expression variation is dominated,
  • 26:00right, by a single principle axis
  • 26:02which is highly correlated with these
  • 26:05expressions of hierarchy and that's great.
  • 26:07OK, that's an observation, right.
  • 26:09And these gradients of micro scale
  • 26:11properties then can contribute perhaps
  • 26:13to sensory association specialization,
  • 26:15but furthermore may contribute
  • 26:17to the effects of pharmacology
  • 26:18across these cortical areas, right.
  • 26:21And so this is this is work.
  • 26:23That we've published on and actually
  • 26:25I'm I'm also really proud of this
  • 26:27John and I Co wrote a patient with
  • 26:29our colleague Bill Martin who is
  • 26:30now head of Global Neuro at J&J
  • 26:33and this was just recently awarded.
  • 26:35So now we have a patent on this,
  • 26:36not with that useful for anything
  • 26:38but was it was something that we
  • 26:41wanted to develop and really kind
  • 26:43of drive forward anyway so.
  • 26:46Sharp left turn.
  • 26:47How does this relate to pharmacological
  • 26:50effects of any kind and psychedelic
  • 26:52psychedelics in particular?
  • 26:54You guys are probably thinking when is he
  • 26:55going to get to anything psychedelic related?
  • 26:57Like why am I listening to this?
  • 26:58So, so we're we're getting there, so.
  • 27:03OK, this map and and I had some
  • 27:07set up slides, but,
  • 27:08but I actually want to go a little faster,
  • 27:09right.
  • 27:10So this is a paper published by
  • 27:12Katrine in elife a couple years ago,
  • 27:15right.
  • 27:15And So what she's done is
  • 27:17basically giving people a.
  • 27:22A pill of LSD which targets the
  • 27:25serotonin receptor versus placebo.
  • 27:28And what we've done is we've computed
  • 27:30a map of unresting state of the effect
  • 27:33of LSD on every single person and this
  • 27:36is the average across all the people,
  • 27:39right?
  • 27:41In the effects of LSD on a metric that
  • 27:43we call global brain connectivity.
  • 27:46So let me unpack this a little bit
  • 27:48so you so you develop an intuition.
  • 27:50Every warm area that you see here
  • 27:52is an area that has an elevation
  • 27:56in its brain wide covariation.
  • 27:58When people are given LSD relative
  • 28:00to cell cycle, so in other words,
  • 28:03you could you could say it's
  • 28:04a hyperconnectivity, right?
  • 28:06But I don't like to use that term
  • 28:08before impacting it first, right?
  • 28:10So for instance,
  • 28:11the visual cortex here would.
  • 28:14This the interpretation is that
  • 28:17LSD elevates the connectivity with
  • 28:19the rest of the brain for visual
  • 28:22cortex bilaterally and for sensory
  • 28:25somatomotor cortex and MTV.
  • 28:28But it reduces connectivity in
  • 28:31these association areas, right?
  • 28:34So this is important, right?
  • 28:35In other words,
  • 28:36there is a bidirectional effect
  • 28:38of LSD versus placebo on sensory
  • 28:41versus association regions, right?
  • 28:43At least it appears that way,
  • 28:45right? And so we can now isolate the Type
  • 28:491 error protected effect right by doing
  • 28:52TFCC protection at the whole brain level.
  • 28:55And now you can see this values,
  • 28:57this is what's pulling out the values
  • 28:59out of this area now what Katrina has
  • 29:02also done which is. Pretty clever.
  • 29:05She's given people ketanserin prior to the
  • 29:08administration of LSD right and contains.
  • 29:11Azrin is thought to be a very selective
  • 29:14antagonist of the five HT 2A receptor.
  • 29:17And you'll notice that when people
  • 29:19are pretreated with ketanserin,
  • 29:20they look just like.
  • 29:23Placebo right.
  • 29:24In other words,
  • 29:25there is no effect of LSD in those areas.
  • 29:27And this is true for the areas that
  • 29:29show a reduction in connectivity
  • 29:31by LSD and the regions that show
  • 29:33an increase in connectivity by LSD.
  • 29:36So there is a full blockade of the effect
  • 29:38of LSD by Captain Strand on average, right?
  • 29:41So that's pretty cool, right.
  • 29:43And and I remember showing this to to John
  • 29:47Crystal years ago when we first saw this
  • 29:50effect and saying like look the two maps,
  • 29:53the LSD versus placebo and the
  • 29:55LSD plus captain string versus
  • 29:57LSD are super correlated.
  • 29:58Look, they're almost the same.
  • 30:00And and I'll show you what they
  • 30:02look like side by side, right.
  • 30:04This is same people, two different days.
  • 30:07One day they're given LSD
  • 30:08alone versus placebo,
  • 30:10the other day they're given
  • 30:11contains trend prior to.
  • 30:12Was the and then contrasted to placebo.
  • 30:16And I'm like, this is incredible.
  • 30:17They look the same.
  • 30:18And he just laughed at me.
  • 30:19He was like, ohh,
  • 30:20of course it's pharmacology.
  • 30:21It has to be.
  • 30:22Was like,
  • 30:22well,
  • 30:23I'm so glad it's so obvious to you
  • 30:24that that you're going to get such a
  • 30:27correspondence in the within subject
  • 30:28effect of pharmacological effects on
  • 30:30brain imaging at the surface level.
  • 30:32Like I wouldn't have guessed that,
  • 30:34right, but this is important,
  • 30:36right,
  • 30:36because it highlights that we can
  • 30:39leverage surface based topography
  • 30:40as an index of the effect of
  • 30:43pharmacology on the human brain, right.
  • 30:45And so now, now we get this map,
  • 30:48this delta map, delta GBC,
  • 30:50right and now we ask.
  • 30:52OK, what about gene expression patterns,
  • 30:54right?
  • 30:54If this is truly related to the
  • 30:56serotonin 5H2 receptor which we
  • 30:58believe contains from this blocking?
  • 31:00Then presumably the 5H2 receptor map
  • 31:03ought to look like this map, right?
  • 31:05So that's what we tried.
  • 31:07We took the gene expression map,
  • 31:08right,
  • 31:09and we computed the correlation
  • 31:11right across these two, right.
  • 31:13And we also took some other target genes,
  • 31:16right,
  • 31:16that are thought in the literature
  • 31:19to be targeted by LSD.
  • 31:22And then we computed a similarity of
  • 31:24correlation between this and this is
  • 31:26serotonin is right up here, right.
  • 31:28So it's the of these, it's the most.
  • 31:31Positively correlated.
  • 31:32And then we repeated this for the
  • 31:34entire distribution of all genes
  • 31:36from the alien human brain Atlas.
  • 31:38And again,
  • 31:39serotonin comes out here in the
  • 31:4195th at almost 96 percentile, right?
  • 31:44So there are some things that
  • 31:45by chance could
  • 31:45be higher, but this is pretty.
  • 31:47Encouraging as a initial proof of
  • 31:49principle that we can actually map
  • 31:51human gene expression in relation
  • 31:52to the pharmacological effects on
  • 31:54the human brain in vivo of a given
  • 31:57pharmacological agent that was blocked by
  • 32:00the hypothesized receptor antagonist, right?
  • 32:03That's in my mind pretty cool.
  • 32:06And so, so you also see the opposite,
  • 32:09which is HR7, which in my mind has
  • 32:11some very interesting pharmacology,
  • 32:13but we won't get into that today, right?
  • 32:15It's just you also see the opposite
  • 32:17effects in this, this kind of analysis.
  • 32:19OK. So. How do we take
  • 32:23this quick question?
  • 32:24If you go back, I'm thinking about
  • 32:27the 1A receptor which is usually
  • 32:29pre synaptic on Axon terminals,
  • 32:31which means that the receptor is
  • 32:34not in the same place as the M RNA.
  • 32:37That's what you're doing here.
  • 32:39When you're when you're looking at the
  • 32:41distribution of gene expression across
  • 32:43the brain, you're looking at M RNA,
  • 32:45which is basically where the cell bodies.
  • 32:49Right and. That should be pretty good
  • 32:52because it's amended and Reddick.
  • 32:53But for the 1A receptor there,
  • 32:55there's going to be a substantial
  • 32:57dissociation between where the M
  • 32:58RNA is and where the receptor is,
  • 33:00which I don't think you can get
  • 33:01it at this technique.
  • 33:02So just for some receptors that may be
  • 33:04applicant, you're absolutely right.
  • 33:06So you're absolutely right.
  • 33:08So what you're highlighting is the nuance,
  • 33:11the Super important nuance between
  • 33:13the ligand and the receptor, right.
  • 33:15And basically the fact that the M RNA
  • 33:16may be coding for the ligand or the
  • 33:18receptor and in the cases where it's
  • 33:20coding for the receptor, this analysis.
  • 33:22Approach may be very informative,
  • 33:24but in the cases where it's
  • 33:25coding for the ligand,
  • 33:27it may or may not be informative.
  • 33:28It's not about whether it's the ligand,
  • 33:29it's where the receptor is on the neuron.
  • 33:33So for pre, for pre synaptic receptors,
  • 33:36for receptors that are
  • 33:37targeted to Axon terminals,
  • 33:39we see this in the animal literature
  • 33:41all the time where the receptor
  • 33:42in the M RNA are in different
  • 33:43places because the receptor is
  • 33:44way out on the Axon terminals,
  • 33:46which is in a different part of
  • 33:48the brain from the cell body.
  • 33:49It's not about the ligand,
  • 33:50it's about where the receptor
  • 33:52is localized in the neuron.
  • 33:53So you're highlighting something
  • 33:55even different, right, which is which
  • 33:56is now beginning to appreciate it.
  • 33:58So you're saying that the postsynaptic
  • 34:01receptor expression of the five HD.
  • 34:03Way. Is potentially captured very well
  • 34:07by the M RNA and the the the probes
  • 34:12whereas the presynaptic 1A may may be a
  • 34:15very different phenomenon because it's
  • 34:17on the presynaptic terminals right.
  • 34:19And therefore you're not binding to it.
  • 34:21And then furthermore furthermore it's
  • 34:23this is where my thinking was going
  • 34:26which is the ligand versus the receptor,
  • 34:29right because now you have that third
  • 34:31axis of variation and so and and to your
  • 34:33point this gets complicated because.
  • 34:35When you give a substance like a psychedelic,
  • 34:38you have post,
  • 34:39you have polysynaptic distal effects, right?
  • 34:43Which is it's going to travel right through
  • 34:46the Axon and potentially shuttle onto the.
  • 34:50Receptors and activate those terminals
  • 34:52on distal neurons that are not in the
  • 34:55local tissue bed of the high expression,
  • 34:57which is why and this is actually
  • 34:59gets really nuanced why looking at
  • 35:01the dense GBC at the voxel level and
  • 35:04partially the GBC doesn't necessarily
  • 35:06fully map onto one another because that
  • 35:09level of granularity begins to matter,
  • 35:11right.
  • 35:11And you can now appreciate right
  • 35:13that if you are averaging signal
  • 35:15within an area or if you're looking
  • 35:17at boxing level pharmacology,
  • 35:18right and furthermore if you can.
  • 35:20Some even deeper into
  • 35:22columnar level pharmacology,
  • 35:23but they're an important ones here, right?
  • 35:25But at the very coarse level,
  • 35:28you can at least begin to
  • 35:30identify these first principles,
  • 35:31which is pharmacological
  • 35:33neuroimaging topographies with GBC,
  • 35:35which is this random freaking measure, right?
  • 35:37That that seemingly has these
  • 35:39properties that we really like maps
  • 35:42onto gene expression gradients, right?
  • 35:44Like who would have thunk it?
  • 35:46And it's not.
  • 35:46And and the important thing about this
  • 35:48is that if you use some graph, theoretical.
  • 35:51Distraction.
  • 35:51Without a surface map,
  • 35:53there's no freaking way you can
  • 35:55get this right.
  • 35:56And that's actually kind of the take
  • 35:58away that I was trying to get at right
  • 36:00is that that this is obscured if you
  • 36:02do not have a surface map, right?
  • 36:04You just can never see it, Umm,
  • 36:07because that's what drives the
  • 36:10correspondence, right, and the location.
  • 36:12So anyway, so, so,
  • 36:13so now I I want to be sensitive to time,
  • 36:16so I may have to kind of speed
  • 36:17up to some of this stuff.
  • 36:18So, so basically the point of this is
  • 36:20how do we map this onto neurobehavioral,
  • 36:23geometric models in in clinical
  • 36:26population level analysis,
  • 36:27right.
  • 36:28And so this cartoon is just
  • 36:29highlighting that there's some
  • 36:31brain to behavioral relationship and
  • 36:32that it's probably or some oblique,
  • 36:34maybe even not linear.
  • 36:35And we don't know what it is.
  • 36:37And so this is work really purely done
  • 36:41by my former student now research
  • 36:43scientist here in our department,
  • 36:45Lisa and and she's published
  • 36:48this in life earlier last year.
  • 36:51After a whole saga.
  • 36:53So,
  • 36:54so I I I want to this gets a little
  • 36:56technical, so I'll try to kind of keep
  • 36:58it clear and then get to the key points.
  • 37:01So if we're going to leverage
  • 37:03pharmacological new imaging that's
  • 37:05even benchmark with gene expression or
  • 37:07what have you right to achieve brain
  • 37:10behavioral models that can actually be
  • 37:12deployed for any therapeutic purpose.
  • 37:14There are some criteria that I'd like to
  • 37:16argue we need to really hold in mind, right.
  • 37:19And these criteria are are not exhaustive and
  • 37:21things that I've learned the hard way that,
  • 37:24you know, if you don't do this,
  • 37:26things are just brittle.
  • 37:27So the first one is more
  • 37:29kind of for the whole field,
  • 37:30which is that anything that we
  • 37:32produce as a field has to scale and
  • 37:35interoperate in an informatics way.
  • 37:38So for instance,
  • 37:39what Pronet is doing and what Professor
  • 37:41Woods is doing in our department with
  • 37:43this massive worldwide consortium.
  • 37:45Right.
  • 37:45So, so the days of me working on my PC
  • 37:49and producing imaging are gone, right?
  • 37:51Like it's no longer that.
  • 37:52So then the measure selection,
  • 37:55and I mean the behavioral measure selection
  • 37:58matters here and it matters a lot.
  • 37:59And I'll show you why. Then.
  • 38:02How do we cross validate those
  • 38:04behavioral models,
  • 38:05right.
  • 38:06And this is stuff that has to do with
  • 38:08trustworthiness of the reproducibility
  • 38:10of those models at the behavioral level.
  • 38:14No imaging yet.
  • 38:15This is this is something that that
  • 38:17we also found out is very important.
  • 38:20Then Criterion 4 is how do we then
  • 38:22produce a robust and interpretable
  • 38:25neuroimaging maps that are linkable
  • 38:28to that behavioral variation?
  • 38:29And then finally,
  • 38:30how do we cross validate that
  • 38:32brain behavioral?
  • 38:33Right.
  • 38:33So this is a lot of stuff to cover
  • 38:35in like 15 minutes.
  • 38:36So some of the hit on some of the highlights
  • 38:39and then we'll pause your questions.
  • 38:41So this is.
  • 38:42You know,
  • 38:43a trivial point, right,
  • 38:44it's just hard to do,
  • 38:46which is we need informatics
  • 38:47solutions that scale.
  • 38:48And Yale is at the forefront of this.
  • 38:50I think that what we're doing
  • 38:52in our department,
  • 38:53I'm tremendously proud of and I
  • 38:54think some of the work of faculty
  • 38:56on this call and others like we're
  • 38:58really pushing the boundary of this.
  • 39:00This is the point is really things
  • 39:01have to scale and drop rate if
  • 39:03we're going to develop precision
  • 39:05medicine solutions, right.
  • 39:06So I'll just forward and just
  • 39:08say a key thing inside this
  • 39:10architecture is analytic discovering.
  • 39:12Years.
  • 39:13And so this is the workflow from
  • 39:15Lisa's paper,
  • 39:15just as a shameless plug.
  • 39:17But the point is that analytics have to be
  • 39:20organically built into this for the Discovery
  • 39:22science engine to work where it cannot be.
  • 39:25Data collection devoid of analytics.
  • 39:27It's it's it's all,
  • 39:29it's all combined, right?
  • 39:30So talk through how we do this with
  • 39:32a particular analytic framework using
  • 39:34a data set that's called Beast Snip.
  • 39:37Our colleague,
  • 39:38Godfrey Pearlson was one of the
  • 39:41principal investigators on the original.
  • 39:43Snip that made it into the public domain,
  • 39:44and now they're on to be snipped too.
  • 39:46I don't even know three,
  • 39:47but this is a public domain
  • 39:49datasets that made it into NH.
  • 39:51We downloaded it out of the National
  • 39:53Debt archive and processed it using the
  • 39:56human connectome processing pipelines.
  • 39:58Like that's as much as I'll
  • 40:00say to speed ahead, OK?
  • 40:01So I've shown this several times.
  • 40:04Maybe some of you have seen these data,
  • 40:06but now it's published and I can kind
  • 40:07of show you the full gamut of this.
  • 40:09So the first question that we asked
  • 40:13ourselves was what is the covariance
  • 40:16structure across people in the symptom
  • 40:20geometry of the psychosis spectrum
  • 40:22population of these 436 people?
  • 40:25And you're looking at pans,
  • 40:27these are pans items, right?
  • 40:28The backs is on top backs,
  • 40:31which is the brief.
  • 40:32Assessment of cognition and
  • 40:34then pans items here.
  • 40:35So this is the covariance matrix
  • 40:38and there's some correlation between
  • 40:40these right across 436 people.
  • 40:42So in other words,
  • 40:43they're structure between these symptoms,
  • 40:45which is expected.
  • 40:46This is not new, this is, this makes sense.
  • 40:49But when you plotted across
  • 40:51the DSM categories, right,
  • 40:52where bipolar is shown in yellow,
  • 40:54schizoaffective in this
  • 40:56kind of orangish color,
  • 40:58dark red is schizophrenia and all pro bands,
  • 41:00all patients are shown in black,
  • 41:02you know, I like to argue that.
  • 41:06There's a lot of variation in each one of
  • 41:09these sub scores on the pans and cognition,
  • 41:12but not really clear,
  • 41:13very clear distinctions between
  • 41:15diagnostic categories, right?
  • 41:16And so this is not news.
  • 41:19Psychosis spectrum disorder is heterogeneous,
  • 41:20exhibits covariation symptoms
  • 41:22across clinical scales. OK, great.
  • 41:24You know, cool story Allen.
  • 41:26So now what?
  • 41:26So the question is what is the
  • 41:28dimensionality of this solution,
  • 41:30right.
  • 41:30Is there a low dimensional solution that
  • 41:32we can reduce the map to the brain right.
  • 41:34Can we do that and simply right.
  • 41:37You could ask is there say principal
  • 41:40component analytic solution that
  • 41:42explains these data or a factor analytic
  • 41:44solution or K means clustering solution,
  • 41:47just something that is looking at
  • 41:49the covariance structure of the
  • 41:51data in a lower dimensional space.
  • 41:53And so it turns out yes.
  • 41:55Right.
  • 41:55So these are the components that come out,
  • 41:57which brings me to criterion too, right?
  • 41:59Can we select the right measures
  • 42:01to map onto the brain? Right.
  • 42:03And can we obtain an interpretable
  • 42:05solution here?
  • 42:06Right.
  • 42:06So, I mean, I'm not going to walk
  • 42:08through these principal components.
  • 42:09More importantly,
  • 42:10when I want to highlight is what the geometry
  • 42:12looks like just so you can get an intuition.
  • 42:15Every dot is a patient.
  • 42:16They're color-coded as noted here,
  • 42:18right?
  • 42:19These arrows in this space are
  • 42:22vectors of the pans and backs.
  • 42:28Average scores. The green arrow is backs,
  • 42:32which is almost perfectly
  • 42:33collinear with the cognition axis.
  • 42:35That's one of the principal components,
  • 42:37the black dots that you may see
  • 42:40here healthy controls right?
  • 42:41And then the two the the the
  • 42:45arrows that are coming up the
  • 42:48the blue and the purple are the.
  • 42:52Negative and positive symptoms,
  • 42:54respectively, right?
  • 42:55Notice that they project onto
  • 42:58this access under an angle.
  • 43:01It's not. They're not collinear,
  • 43:03and they're obliquely rotated, right?
  • 43:06And then there's a global dysfunction
  • 43:09which is all the patients are not
  • 43:11functioning as well as controls.
  • 43:13So it's this PC three that we're
  • 43:15going to talk about, right.
  • 43:17So this is a static picture of that solution,
  • 43:20right.
  • 43:21And if if you can see after correct
  • 43:24schizoaffective is misspelled,
  • 43:26but if you can see the positive
  • 43:28and the negative vectors,
  • 43:29they form a 45 degree angle onto PC-3.
  • 43:32OK.
  • 43:33So now what is this action you
  • 43:35look like numerically, right?
  • 43:36Zoom in and these are the linear combinations
  • 43:39and this solution right in this sample.
  • 43:42So if I plot the DSM categories,
  • 43:44you'd say there's an effect.
  • 43:47Right. They don't differ,
  • 43:49but that's actually the point.
  • 43:51The point is that when you cut
  • 43:54through this geometry with the.
  • 43:56Data-driven solution.
  • 43:57You ought not to see differences in DSM
  • 43:59categories because they don't seem to
  • 44:02actually follow natural variation, right?
  • 44:03And so how can I convince you of that, right?
  • 44:06So.
  • 44:06So let's take a look at the
  • 44:09configuration of the PC-3 items, right.
  • 44:12So typical person would be somewhat
  • 44:14delusional, conceptual, disorganized.
  • 44:16They are hallucinating,
  • 44:17they have some excitement,
  • 44:19grandiosity, right?
  • 44:20But they're not, you know,
  • 44:22purely collinear with the negative symptoms.
  • 44:24They have something, some they don't.
  • 44:26They're a little bit cognitively impaired,
  • 44:27right?
  • 44:28But again not a clean, you know,
  • 44:31one to one mapping between these these axes,
  • 44:33right, between the the subscores of the pans.
  • 44:36So again, you know,
  • 44:37let's put this to the test
  • 44:38I I'm a competitive person,
  • 44:39I like competition, right.
  • 44:40And and I like to, you know,
  • 44:43create competitions,
  • 44:44incentive questions, right.
  • 44:45So let's see is the SM going to
  • 44:48outperform a data-driven solution, right.
  • 44:50Because we need it to map it
  • 44:52onto pharmacology, right?
  • 44:53Like we need something that is robust, so.
  • 44:58Criterion 3.
  • 44:59Before we even get there right,
  • 45:01we have to check that the
  • 45:03solution of this model is stable.
  • 45:05So this is a summary of the leave
  • 45:07each site out cross validation,
  • 45:10a summary of the predicted
  • 45:11versus observed scores,
  • 45:12a summary of the predicted versus
  • 45:15observed single subject scores
  • 45:16from K fold bootstrapping,
  • 45:18and similarity of the actual loadings
  • 45:20on the PCA for leave site out five
  • 45:23fold bootstrapping and split half.
  • 45:25And hopefully this shows you that
  • 45:27the solution is really stable.
  • 45:28Which means that the basement consortium
  • 45:30did a really good job actually,
  • 45:32right.
  • 45:32They collected and and asset clinically
  • 45:34the data in a very consistent way
  • 45:37and we're able to get a pretty
  • 45:39good stable behavioral model,
  • 45:41right. So the PCA variance is generalizes,
  • 45:45the score is generalized and the
  • 45:46PC weights generalized, right.
  • 45:48Otherwise why are we mapping it onto
  • 45:50the brain if it doesn't, right? Cool.
  • 45:52So now let's actually go even further
  • 45:54from DSM and take pans positive symptoms.
  • 45:57Let's give pans a fair shot because
  • 45:59this is what the industry uses, right?
  • 46:01If you're going to test if
  • 46:03a antipsychotic works,
  • 46:04you're going to use pans positive symptoms.
  • 46:06That's your benchmark.
  • 46:07That's the gold standard for the industry,
  • 46:09right. And and this is the
  • 46:13psychosis configuration PCA effect,
  • 46:15which required only only one level
  • 46:18of supervision, which is to pick PCA.
  • 46:21That's it. We just said.
  • 46:22Let's run a PCA.
  • 46:23So now, which one will give you
  • 46:25a better brain map, right?
  • 46:26That's what we care about.
  • 46:28We care which Brain Mac is better.
  • 46:31And so again,
  • 46:31we're going to use GBC as the
  • 46:33brain measure and we're going to
  • 46:36calculate the variation from each.
  • 46:37Parcel to every other parcel using this
  • 46:40quantitative technique that I explained,
  • 46:41right.
  • 46:42And so the intuition again is that we're
  • 46:44going to get this value for every parcel.
  • 46:47We're going to then correlate
  • 46:49the area level signal with the
  • 46:52symptom for every patient.
  • 46:54And then we're going to get in a
  • 46:56cross subject map that tells us
  • 46:58how people vary across the sample
  • 47:00with respect to their GBC, right.
  • 47:02So it's an individual difference analysis.
  • 47:04And so this is the map you get with pans
  • 47:07with 436 people and this is the map.
  • 47:09You get when you use the PC-3.
  • 47:12Now.
  • 47:12I want to just pause here because
  • 47:15hopefully it's self-evident to
  • 47:17everybody which one is better
  • 47:19and if somebody says a.
  • 47:21I I hope they're joking.
  • 47:24So.
  • 47:24This is not nothing done except simply
  • 47:27taking a data-driven behavioral
  • 47:30analysis of pens and backs,
  • 47:33a data-driven neural measure with
  • 47:35the only piece of supervision being.
  • 47:38Reduction of the FC matrix using GBC.
  • 47:41And then you get this slice through
  • 47:43the geometry that hopefully one
  • 47:45could argue is is better and
  • 47:47quantitatively it is better, right?
  • 47:49You can actually check that statistically
  • 47:51that the variance covered is higher
  • 47:53and that the range of the Z values is better.
  • 47:56You can do all sorts of other things,
  • 47:57but it's just better.
  • 47:59So, OK, now we have something right
  • 48:03now criterion 5 is,
  • 48:05is this thing generalizable?
  • 48:07And what I mean by that is if I were to.
  • 48:11Say Mark or Chris,
  • 48:13can you guys use the weights,
  • 48:15the actual thing that I found
  • 48:18here and reproduce the exact map?
  • 48:21Using a split half cross validation
  • 48:24of the model,
  • 48:25can you get the same picture again?
  • 48:28That's that's what we care about.
  • 48:29Not just that you can point to five,
  • 48:31reject the null and publish,
  • 48:33but that the picture of the neural
  • 48:36topography is reproducible.
  • 48:37And this is what we get in this case,
  • 48:39right?
  • 48:40So and we did this 10,000 times,
  • 48:43but half and various ways and tried to
  • 48:46break it. You know, pretty robust.
  • 48:48Both dense level and the parcel level, right?
  • 48:52And this is only 219 people, right?
  • 48:54We're not talking gargantuan samples here,
  • 48:56right? It's just that you have the
  • 48:59right slice through the geometry and
  • 49:01then all of a sudden you're getting
  • 49:03maps that reproduce even in patients.
  • 49:05So now this is the engine that we
  • 49:08submit this to in order to select
  • 49:11the most stable features, right.
  • 49:13And I'm not going to walk through
  • 49:15this because it's certainly dense,
  • 49:16but it's, it's simply, you know,
  • 49:19under the hood it's some basic
  • 49:21linear algebra of optimizing each
  • 49:23feature in relation to stability
  • 49:25criteria from the out of sample.
  • 49:28Generalization.
  • 49:28And so then you can ask the question of which
  • 49:32parcels of the map should we trust the most?
  • 49:35And those are the parcels that
  • 49:36then we can use as a, you know,
  • 49:38for further feature engineering.
  • 49:40Interestingly though,
  • 49:41what you can also do is then once
  • 49:43you've done this and you find
  • 49:45your trustworthy parcels right,
  • 49:46the ones that truly generalize,
  • 49:48you can then ask how do they covary in
  • 49:51relation to the behavioral feature selection?
  • 49:54Turns out there's a nonlinear relationship,
  • 49:56right?
  • 49:56Which means that the more extreme the
  • 49:58person is on their behavioral loading,
  • 50:01the more you trust their neural net.
  • 50:03That makes sense, right?
  • 50:05That's intuitive.
  • 50:06And so when you then do this and purely
  • 50:09filter people based on behavior,
  • 50:11just take the 10th.
  • 50:13And the 90th percentiles of the
  • 50:16behavioral scores and segment that way.
  • 50:19Then you can begin to segment
  • 50:22based on neurobehavioral similarity
  • 50:25of the map until you get.
  • 50:27To the very peak of this patient
  • 50:29selection and then you ask how accurate
  • 50:31is this model and then you can see that
  • 50:34it's pretty damn accurate at a sample,
  • 50:36right.
  • 50:36So in other words it's classifying people as
  • 50:38plus or minus and in terms of their range.
  • 50:41And then you can repeat this on
  • 50:43a completely independent sample
  • 50:44and again show that it works.
  • 50:46In terms of segmentation by the way,
  • 50:48Chris,
  • 50:49this is the OCD and and skids data
  • 50:53set which is cross diagnostic that
  • 50:55we tried this with, right so.
  • 50:57This isn't even patients schizophrenia
  • 50:59anymore right.
  • 50:59It's just saying does your brain
  • 51:01map look like some norm that we
  • 51:04can behaviorally incur right.
  • 51:06So it's really about symptom
  • 51:08configurations right.
  • 51:09No longer about our you know,
  • 51:11do you have a diagnostic category
  • 51:13in it anyway,
  • 51:14how can we leverage gene expression
  • 51:16out to molecularly benchmark this
  • 51:18and link it back to pharmacology.
  • 51:20So again I'm going to just remind you
  • 51:21of this framework Gemini dot, right.
  • 51:23So now we can take this PC three
  • 51:26map that is trustworthy.
  • 51:28And again,
  • 51:29correlated with gene expression patterns
  • 51:31in the same way that we've done with MSD.
  • 51:33And this is just proof of principle,
  • 51:35right?
  • 51:35Again, we can show some relationships,
  • 51:37I'm not going to get into this too much,
  • 51:39but for instance,
  • 51:40you can show that the interneuron markers
  • 51:43or GABA subunits or serotonin
  • 51:45receptor subunits have
  • 51:47correspondence with this map.
  • 51:48It's I'm not claiming mechanism or anything,
  • 51:52I'm just saying you could do this,
  • 51:53right. This is doable.
  • 51:55But finally to conclude, you can then.
  • 51:59Benchmark this against our
  • 52:01pharmacological targets.
  • 52:02So this is actually an in vivo ketamine map,
  • 52:06same GBC measure,
  • 52:07healthies versus healthy people,
  • 52:09placebo versus.
  • 52:13Infusion, right?
  • 52:14And then we can select people along the
  • 52:17axis that presumably varies in relation
  • 52:20to that work without ever optimizing it.
  • 52:23We're not optimizing it yet.
  • 52:24And then you take two people
  • 52:25on the extreme ends, right,
  • 52:27and these are their actual brain maps.
  • 52:29These are two people diagnosed
  • 52:30with schizophrenia, right?
  • 52:31They both have the same diagnosis.
  • 52:32Yet I'd like to argue that their symptom
  • 52:35configurations are completely different
  • 52:36and their brains don't look the same.
  • 52:38Right. Yet we're treating them the same.
  • 52:40We're giving D2 blockers to both of these
  • 52:42people as the initial line of defense,
  • 52:44when in fact, who knows,
  • 52:46maybe one person would respond
  • 52:47way better to close the people.
  • 52:48And we have no idea that that
  • 52:50is true or not true, right?
  • 52:52But you can then quantify that using.
  • 52:56This.
  • 52:56Framework that Lisa has advanced in
  • 52:59relation to a given target and you can
  • 53:01say which person is more similar, right?
  • 53:04So this person looks like PC-3,
  • 53:06which looks like ketamine.
  • 53:08So presumably this person will get
  • 53:09worse if you give them ketamine and this
  • 53:11person would maybe even get better, right?
  • 53:13I don't know.
  • 53:14But you could do the same thing with
  • 53:17LSD now and repeat this for another
  • 53:19access and recapitulate this principle.
  • 53:22Now these maps can be iteratively optimized.
  • 53:27This is a.
  • 53:28Feature selection problem.
  • 53:29Now we can use the LSD,
  • 53:31psychedelic or ketamine target
  • 53:33maps to find people who may
  • 53:35benefit the most quantitatively,
  • 53:37rationally,
  • 53:38iteratively in a fast fail algorithm that
  • 53:41says these two patient populations ought
  • 53:44to show the opposite effects of this drug.
  • 53:47And that's a strong inference
  • 53:48rational framework, right?
  • 53:49And so just to summarize,
  • 53:52like I do think we need
  • 53:54informatics and scalability.
  • 53:55I do think we need to first and foremost.
  • 53:58Their behavior,
  • 53:58right?
  • 53:59Select the right combination we need to
  • 54:02achieve criteria for trustworthiness
  • 54:04of those behavioral models, right?
  • 54:07That's that's a must.
  • 54:09Then and only then do we go to brain imaging,
  • 54:12and then we need an interpretable.
  • 54:14Robust and generalizable solution of
  • 54:17the brain back which in turn then we
  • 54:20can cross validate with pharmacological
  • 54:23and gene expression and other metrics
  • 54:26that that the field is bringing to bear.
  • 54:28So, so in summary,
  • 54:29I I do think that we have an
  • 54:31opportunity here, right.
  • 54:32I think that what we're doing in our
  • 54:34department is truly transformative.
  • 54:35I'm just one out of many people who
  • 54:37are doing this work and we have I
  • 54:40think an iterative framework right for
  • 54:42really dissecting heterogeneity with.
  • 54:44Imaging and behavior.
  • 54:45And this can be optimized actually
  • 54:47again for patient selection and
  • 54:49precise delivery of of psychedelic
  • 54:51compounds to the right patient.
  • 54:53So I'll stop there and think
  • 54:55I'm actually in time.
  • 54:56Remarkable.
  • 54:58Impressive. Thank you, Alan.
  • 55:02That was a remarkably lucid presentation
  • 55:04of some very complicated material.
  • 55:09Questions. Comments. We have just a
  • 55:12couple minutes before official end time.
  • 55:25Ellen, I wonder if you could
  • 55:27swing back to speculate.
  • 55:29Since sort of the motivating you,
  • 55:32you've spent a lot of time talking
  • 55:33about the framework, the technology,
  • 55:34the analytics and the potential and and
  • 55:37just a couple slides on the psychedelics.
  • 55:39In the middle,
  • 55:40which I think is fine because, you know,
  • 55:43it's important for us to recognize this.
  • 55:46What what what you're working on,
  • 55:47but I wonder if you could project OK.
  • 55:50This group is motivated primarily by an
  • 55:52interest in how do the psychedelics work?
  • 55:54Who can they help? You know,
  • 55:56how would you imagine over the coming years?
  • 55:58And I know you've thought about
  • 56:00this a lot because you're doing
  • 56:01it and planning on doing it.
  • 56:03So how, how would you envision
  • 56:05applying this framework to a deep,
  • 56:07to developing a deeper
  • 56:09understanding of how psychedelics?
  • 56:11Affect the brain both in terms
  • 56:13of their acute, you know,
  • 56:15psychotomimetic, dissociative,
  • 56:16whatever effects and in terms of
  • 56:19their longer term therapeutic effects.
  • 56:21Yeah, so.
  • 56:24So there
  • 56:25there's two pieces of work
  • 56:26that I didn't have the time to
  • 56:27highlight and I was wrestling with.
  • 56:29Do I want to go into them or not?
  • 56:30And so, so one paper that Katrin
  • 56:33published is looking at time
  • 56:35dependent effects on the brain of
  • 56:37silybin and the same imaging session.
  • 56:39So one thing that she's done that I
  • 56:42think is really impressive as shown
  • 56:44the evolving neural neural effect
  • 56:46of these compounds in the same
  • 56:49person and showing how these maps,
  • 56:51these topographies evolve as we.
  • 56:54Selecting data overtime and that
  • 56:56gives us confidence of the, the, the.
  • 57:01Basically neural targeting engagement.
  • 57:02So that's one thing that I think
  • 57:04we really need more of these neural
  • 57:07targeting management and so then then
  • 57:08what Josh has actually done cleverly
  • 57:10in in a in a sister paper to Lisa's paper,
  • 57:12which is a whole nother beast
  • 57:14that I didn't want to get into.
  • 57:15John senior author on that,
  • 57:17he's actually taken the the observation
  • 57:19from Katrina was the observation
  • 57:21and then fit gene expression to the
  • 57:24computational models out of John's labs.
  • 57:26And I,
  • 57:27I I really didn't want to get into that
  • 57:29because the the technical detail behind it.
  • 57:31This is maybe beyond our time scope today,
  • 57:34but you guys should invite
  • 57:35him to talk about that.
  • 57:37And So what he's done is put in
  • 57:40gradients of 5H2A pharmacology
  • 57:42into the biophysical models,
  • 57:44right,
  • 57:45simulated surrogate models and then
  • 57:47fit them to individual people given
  • 57:49LSD and found that actually explains
  • 57:52the data way better in relation to
  • 57:54their symptoms that they get acutely.
  • 57:56So that's that's another paper.
  • 57:58So these two pieces of work are all
  • 58:00about neural target engagement.
  • 58:01Confidence.
  • 58:01And then Chris,
  • 58:02your question is how do we apply this?
  • 58:04What do we do with this in relation
  • 58:07to helping people who may benefit
  • 58:09from the the administration of
  • 58:11these psychedelics and that has
  • 58:13to do with finding individuals in
  • 58:15the general population whose?
  • 58:18Purported or potential neural
  • 58:21system disturbance alteration,
  • 58:24whatever term you want to use
  • 58:25in relation to their behavior.
  • 58:27In this case mood maps onto that
  • 58:31neural target engagement profile right.
  • 58:34And so the question becomes there
  • 58:36are two questions right that we're
  • 58:39after is the effect of LSD and or
  • 58:42silicide been uniform across people.
  • 58:44In other words if you give it
  • 58:46to me and you and mark and.
  • 58:48Anita across.
  • 58:49Doses.
  • 58:50Is our brain topography gonna look the same?
  • 58:54Turns out not,
  • 58:56that's not true and that matters.
  • 58:59So for patient precision
  • 59:01delivery that matters, right,
  • 59:02because if say you are particularly
  • 59:05amenable to respond to that compound,
  • 59:07but I'm not right,
  • 59:08then you wouldn't give it to me.
  • 59:10And that has nothing to do with
  • 59:12my behavioral alteration per se,
  • 59:14but it may have a lot to do with the
  • 59:17receptor occupancy and the the nature of the,
  • 59:20you know, individual.
  • 59:21Creation.
  • 59:21Turns out this is unpublished work,
  • 59:23but ketamine is even more high than.
  • 59:25Functional,
  • 59:26right?
  • 59:26Turns out that there is no one axis
  • 59:28of the average effect
  • 59:29Academy on the human brain.
  • 59:31It's actually highly dimensional,
  • 59:33which obscures paradoxically
  • 59:35the average effect, right,
  • 59:37if you have multiple dimensions.
  • 59:38And so this is what we're after.
  • 59:39We're after mapping variation of
  • 59:42psychopharmacology within and
  • 59:43across people in order to then
  • 59:45informed precision of how it
  • 59:47relates to circuit disturbance.
  • 59:53I don't thank you very much for your call.
  • 59:54I have a question about the study
  • 59:57that you show about the functional
  • 01:00:01connectivity after LSD and was it in the
  • 01:00:05acute phase or like post acute or like,
  • 01:00:08I just want to know how long after
  • 01:00:10those of like any psychedelic,
  • 01:00:12we have a question. And two scans,
  • 01:00:16one at 75 minutes, one at 300 minutes.
  • 01:00:18So you could argue because the ketanserin
  • 01:00:21and the LSD half lives have slightly
  • 01:00:24overlapping and distinct curves.
  • 01:00:26So we wanted one early and one late,
  • 01:00:28and it turns out that that matters.
  • 01:00:31So like do you know of any?
  • 01:00:33Like is the Hyperconnectivity continues
  • 01:00:35after for example after one week?
  • 01:00:39I don't know. We don't know.
  • 01:00:41That's a wide open question, right.
  • 01:00:42So. So I don't know that anybody's
  • 01:00:44looked at these sustained effects.
  • 01:00:46What we have some stuff from Arena
  • 01:00:48Australia's data set, right.
  • 01:00:49Which again I it's it's really her
  • 01:00:52story to report but but it turns
  • 01:00:53out also when we give ketamine
  • 01:00:55and look at people a day later,
  • 01:00:57right, with F MRI and behavior,
  • 01:00:59there's this and you guys know this, right.
  • 01:01:01There's this crazy inverted V relationship.
  • 01:01:03Some people have a sustained effect of
  • 01:01:06the antidepressant phenomenon and other
  • 01:01:08people go right back to where they were.
  • 01:01:10And we don't know why this is.
  • 01:01:12This is unexplored neurobehavioral effects.
  • 01:01:14We don't know,
  • 01:01:15but we know that everybody
  • 01:01:17acutely shows some kind of.
  • 01:01:19Clinical efficacy,
  • 01:01:20but then a day later you have this rebound
  • 01:01:23and who is rebounding and who is not why?
  • 01:01:26You know there's ideas about synaptic
  • 01:01:28plasticity and LTP like phenomena
  • 01:01:30and and you know which people have
  • 01:01:32that dendritic proliferation would
  • 01:01:34then stabilizes and who is most
  • 01:01:36likely to benefit from that kind
  • 01:01:39of and you know psychedelics and
  • 01:01:40ketamine very different from ecology,
  • 01:01:42very,
  • 01:01:42very different but maybe converging on
  • 01:01:44some endpoint of exciting and driving
  • 01:01:46the circuits into an LTP like phenomena,
  • 01:01:48we don't know.
  • 01:01:49And
  • 01:01:50is there any relationship,
  • 01:01:52any association between the
  • 01:01:53degree of this hyperconnectivity
  • 01:01:56and response to treatment?
  • 01:01:58We don't know that's nobody has
  • 01:02:00that data set again nobody's
  • 01:02:03done MD either you know major,
  • 01:02:06major depression or you know severe mood
  • 01:02:08disturbance data set or experiment in
  • 01:02:10which people were given either ketamine,
  • 01:02:12indoor silybin or randomized to one of
  • 01:02:16these arms scanned prior at baseline.
  • 01:02:19Scanned acutely scanned post
  • 01:02:21and then scanned later when they
  • 01:02:23either sustain their recovered and
  • 01:02:25understood what predicts it right.
  • 01:02:26Like wide open question.
  • 01:02:27And I think our department is unique
  • 01:02:29position to go after this right.
  • 01:02:31I think there's plenty of
  • 01:02:33people who have the the means,
  • 01:02:35expertise and talent to go after
  • 01:02:36this and it's a fascinating question
  • 01:02:38like this is what we need to know.
  • 01:02:41Thank you. No, my pleasure.
  • 01:02:45Anahita stole my question.
  • 01:02:46I I also had the question about can
  • 01:02:48we predict who was going to respond
  • 01:02:50if they have a certain a certain
  • 01:02:52pattern that that that that shows up.
  • 01:02:55But I also wonder whether there have
  • 01:02:57been other neuroimaging studies,
  • 01:02:58not with psychedelics but with
  • 01:03:00other treatments that showed
  • 01:03:02changes in the connectivity such
  • 01:03:05as T or presence. Totally, totally.
  • 01:03:09So actually I forgot.
  • 01:03:10So charity of Donna did a nice study
  • 01:03:12where he looked at meta analysis of of.
  • 01:03:15Uh, effects of ketamine. I don't wanna,
  • 01:03:17I actually don't wanna forget that I
  • 01:03:19think John's on the paper, John Christow.
  • 01:03:21So there is some evidence of this in
  • 01:03:23the literature that people have done,
  • 01:03:24but just not the experiments that
  • 01:03:26you guys were asking about that.
  • 01:03:28But to your point, yes.
  • 01:03:29In fact, we worked with Anil Malhotra and
  • 01:03:32to look at the effect of clozapine, right.
  • 01:03:35We're actually writing this up
  • 01:03:36for publication as we speak.
  • 01:03:37And so, yes,
  • 01:03:38in fact you can predict and and
  • 01:03:39turns out that these are very strong
  • 01:03:42effects actually when you get
  • 01:03:43people who are responding, right.
  • 01:03:45The neural maps of predicting who responds
  • 01:03:48are actually quite nice and clean.
  • 01:03:50It's just that these are small samples
  • 01:03:53like we're talking 141520 people, right.
  • 01:03:54So it's just the first wave of
  • 01:03:56work that's coming out, right?
  • 01:03:58Like this is the next generation,
  • 01:03:59right.
  • 01:04:00They don't like precision pharmacology
  • 01:04:02to dissect individual variation in
  • 01:04:04relation to neurobehavioral effects.
  • 01:04:06Like, I I'm just super excited,
  • 01:04:08right,
  • 01:04:08because I think this is actually happening.
  • 01:04:10Like we actually see this now like
  • 01:04:11the next 5 to 10 years as possible.
  • 01:04:16Great. Thanks.
  • 01:04:20Very excited. I'm sorry, go ahead.
  • 01:04:24I was just going to say we're
  • 01:04:25overtime and I wonder if
  • 01:04:27we should wrap up that if.
  • 01:04:28Well, I was just going to say,
  • 01:04:30I'll make it quick then.
  • 01:04:31So very exciting
  • 01:04:32work, great presentation and the
  • 01:04:37several questions that were just
  • 01:04:40asked made me think about some of our
  • 01:04:43work in the psychotherapy Development
  • 01:04:45Center and the data that we've been
  • 01:04:47collecting over the past 15 years
  • 01:04:49by integrating F MRI measures into
  • 01:04:51randomized clinical trials and the
  • 01:04:53potential for these approaches we've
  • 01:04:55been using things like connectome based.
  • 01:04:58Predictive modeling, but there
  • 01:04:59are many different approaches to understand
  • 01:05:03better how people may respond to treatment.
  • 01:05:08So I think again very exciting work
  • 01:05:12would be great to to speak further.
  • 01:05:15Because I think that we are at a
  • 01:05:17stage and as you mentioned uniquely
  • 01:05:20positioned within our department to
  • 01:05:22make significant contributions to the
  • 01:05:25understanding of how we might best
  • 01:05:27advance psychiatric care for people.
  • 01:05:29I couldn't agree more mark.
  • 01:05:31And I think that your point there's many
  • 01:05:34different ways that we'll all be in some
  • 01:05:36family of general linear models, right.
  • 01:05:39So and and people can approach
  • 01:05:40this from various angles.
  • 01:05:42I think that you know to your
  • 01:05:44point it's going to be the data.
  • 01:05:45That you guys have and we're going to
  • 01:05:47continue to collect and it's going to
  • 01:05:49be about the right behavioral response
  • 01:05:51mapping which all of you are alluding to.
  • 01:05:53So I I couldn't agree more and I think
  • 01:05:56it's just it's it's just kind of you know.
  • 01:05:59Maybe it's the sunny day,
  • 01:06:00so I feel optimistic,
  • 01:06:02but but I I I genuinely think that this
  • 01:06:06was not possible 15 years ago, right?
  • 01:06:08Like we didn't have the tech to do this
  • 01:06:11and we actually now not only have the tech,
  • 01:06:13but the information to do it.
  • 01:06:14And so I want to just leave
  • 01:06:16people with that idea,
  • 01:06:17right, that that you know.
  • 01:06:21And the fact that Yale is really
  • 01:06:23stepping up into psychedelic
  • 01:06:24medicine and and that you guys are
  • 01:06:26doing this work and I couldn't
  • 01:06:28be more supportive and whatever,
  • 01:06:30whatever you need on on,
  • 01:06:31happy to help.
  • 01:06:36So with that, I think we
  • 01:06:38should close for today. Alan.
  • 01:06:39Thank you again for being here with us
  • 01:06:43today and for covering this material.
  • 01:06:46I believe next month, tentatively,
  • 01:06:48Cyril has agreed to present either he
  • 01:06:50or someone from his group or about some
  • 01:06:52of the work that they've been doing,
  • 01:06:54perhaps about DMT,
  • 01:06:55where they've done some of the first work,
  • 01:06:58both in health and in individuals
  • 01:07:00with depressions. That'd be exciting.
  • 01:07:01That's not confirmed yet,
  • 01:07:02but we'll send out emails once we
  • 01:07:06have a confirmation and a title
  • 01:07:08and hope to see you all then.
  • 01:07:10Take care, everybody.