Skip to Main Content

Childhood adversity and mental health: Identifying opportunities to reduce risk and promote resilience across the life course

November 30, 2022

YCSC Grand Rounds November 29, 2022

Erin Cathleen Dunn ScD, MPH
Associate Professor of Psychiatry
Harvard Medical School

ID
9199

Transcript

  • 00:00Good afternoon, everyone.
  • 00:01I think we'll make a start.
  • 00:05So it's great to
  • 00:06see you all here for grand rounds.
  • 00:08Welcome to grand rounds for those of
  • 00:09you who are celebrating last week.
  • 00:11I hope you enjoyed your Thanksgiving and I
  • 00:13hope that everyone had a restful and relaxing
  • 00:16few days towards the end of last week.
  • 00:18And just a reminder about next week,
  • 00:21we'll have compassionate care rounds
  • 00:23here in the Cohen and live on zoom.
  • 00:25As a reminder,
  • 00:26that session won't be recorded.
  • 00:28So please do join us either in person
  • 00:31or live on zoom, or we'll hear from
  • 00:33an expert panel on the complex.
  • 00:36Care needs of patients dealing with
  • 00:38suicidality and disordered eating.
  • 00:40So please do join us for that.
  • 00:43Now in the spirit of the
  • 00:44holiday that we just marked,
  • 00:46I am very thankful to welcome to
  • 00:48have our speaker join us here today,
  • 00:50Doctor Aaron Dunn.
  • 00:51And so Doctor Dunn is an associate
  • 00:54professor of psychiatry and
  • 00:55Pediatrics and Harvard Medical
  • 00:57School and also an assistant
  • 00:59investigator in Mass General Hospital.
  • 01:01And I think it's fair to say that Doctor
  • 01:03Dunn has pioneered the application of life.
  • 01:06Of course,
  • 01:07epidemiological methods to study the
  • 01:09biological embedding of adversity
  • 01:11and the impact of adversity on
  • 01:14adult mental health outcomes.
  • 01:16And now Doctor Dunn has received
  • 01:18substantial support from the
  • 01:20National Institutes of Health,
  • 01:21including the National Institute
  • 01:22of Mental Health,
  • 01:23and has published prolifically,
  • 01:25as you'll have seen.
  • 01:26And just as recently as two weeks ago,
  • 01:28I think you marked your 100th
  • 01:30publication and a nice systematic
  • 01:31review of the of sensitive periods,
  • 01:34the evidence for sensitive
  • 01:35periods of exposure to child.
  • 01:36Our treatment and the prediction
  • 01:37of adult health outcomes.
  • 01:38So hopefully we'll hear a little
  • 01:40bit about that today and a new
  • 01:42area in the Dunlap looking at
  • 01:44teeth as a potential biomarker of
  • 01:46exposure to early adversity.
  • 01:48Again,
  • 01:48very excited to hear more about that today.
  • 01:51So please give a warm child study
  • 01:53center welcome to Doctor Dunn.
  • 01:59I'm impressed, Karen. You did
  • 02:02that all memorized. It's amazing.
  • 02:06All right, so let me go
  • 02:07ahead and share my screen.
  • 02:18OK. I think we're, I think we're good.
  • 02:22Well, thank you everyone for the
  • 02:23opportunity to be here today.
  • 02:24I'm really excited to share
  • 02:26with you more about my work.
  • 02:27As Kieran said, around childhood
  • 02:30adversity and mental health,
  • 02:31I'm going to tell you a little bit
  • 02:33more about opportunities I think there
  • 02:35are to identify risk and promote
  • 02:37resilience across the lifespan.
  • 02:39Can everyone hear me?
  • 02:40OK, OK, perfect.
  • 02:41So I have no disclosures.
  • 02:45So just to Orient us a little bit,
  • 02:48I want to say a little bit about
  • 02:50childhood adversity.
  • 02:51Childhood adversity,
  • 02:51I think is so critical to study because it's
  • 02:55one of the most impactful social determinants
  • 02:57of mental health as well as physical health.
  • 03:00When I think about childhood adversity,
  • 03:02I think about a range of
  • 03:04different kinds of experiences.
  • 03:05These could be events that happen within
  • 03:07the household or outside of the household.
  • 03:10They could be perpetrated by
  • 03:12loved ones or by strangers.
  • 03:15They could be.
  • 03:15Friends that are acute,
  • 03:16they could be chronic.
  • 03:18Some might meet the definition of a trauma,
  • 03:21others might not.
  • 03:22What we know from large scale
  • 03:24epidemiological studies that
  • 03:26have been done primarily in the
  • 03:28United States is that we know
  • 03:30that adversities are common,
  • 03:31so we know that more than half
  • 03:33of all kids growing up in the US
  • 03:35will experience at least one type
  • 03:37of adversity in their lifespan.
  • 03:39We also know that there are large
  • 03:41racial ethnic minority differences
  • 03:43such that kids who grow up from.
  • 03:46Racial and non white families are
  • 03:49disproportionately affected by adversity,
  • 03:51and similarly,
  • 03:52we also know that girls are
  • 03:54more likely to experience some
  • 03:56adversities compared to boys.
  • 03:58The boys are also more likely
  • 03:59to experience some types of
  • 04:01interpersonal violence in particular.
  • 04:03Now I think this following
  • 04:06statistic is both sobering.
  • 04:08These two sets of statistics are
  • 04:11both sobering but also optimistic.
  • 04:13The first is that we know that
  • 04:15adversity is estimated to at least
  • 04:17double the risk of a mental disorder.
  • 04:19Throughout the lifespan.
  • 04:20Now,
  • 04:21it might not surprise you to hear
  • 04:23that childhood adversity is associated
  • 04:25with child onset or adolescent
  • 04:27onset psychiatric disorders.
  • 04:29But we also know that these
  • 04:31adversities are associated with
  • 04:33increased risk of disorders that
  • 04:34onset for the first time in adulthood.
  • 04:37And we also know,
  • 04:38particularly from recent large
  • 04:40scale meta analysis,
  • 04:42that if these effects of adversity
  • 04:44are causal,
  • 04:44they'd explain about 30 to 40%
  • 04:47of the total variability in risk
  • 04:50for mental health problems.
  • 04:51So to me,
  • 04:52I hear that both optimistically sobering,
  • 04:55right?
  • 04:55It's a it's a scary statistic,
  • 04:57but I think it also suggests
  • 04:59opportunities for where our work
  • 05:01can really have potential impact.
  • 05:03A lot of the work that we do in
  • 05:05my group is focused on depression,
  • 05:07which for those of you who may
  • 05:08not be familiar,
  • 05:09is a major public health problem.
  • 05:11So depression is a disorder that's
  • 05:13common throughout the lifespan.
  • 05:14About one out of every five people
  • 05:16will experience an episode of
  • 05:18depression at some point in their lives.
  • 05:20We also know that depression is a disorder
  • 05:23that disproportionately effects women.
  • 05:25So during childhood, boys and girls
  • 05:27experience similar levels of depression.
  • 05:29But something happens in adolescence
  • 05:31where Girl Scout start to outnumber boys.
  • 05:34By a ratio of two to one, and that
  • 05:36disparity persists throughout the lifespan.
  • 05:39We also know that depression is
  • 05:41associated with a host of negative
  • 05:43consequences in the short and long term.
  • 05:46We know it's recurrent,
  • 05:47we know their side effects from medication.
  • 05:49We know that it affects
  • 05:51people's ability to go to work,
  • 05:53to complete school, and so forth.
  • 05:55And I think in its most severe
  • 05:57form is suicide and self harm.
  • 05:59So because depression is so common,
  • 06:01and because it disproportionately
  • 06:03affects large segments of the population.
  • 06:05And is associated with so many
  • 06:08negative consequences.
  • 06:09Hopefully,
  • 06:09it might not be a surprise to learn
  • 06:12that depression is currently the second
  • 06:14leading cause of disability worldwide.
  • 06:16So my group is really focused on trying to
  • 06:19identify ways that we can prevent depression.
  • 06:21And I think that's really critical
  • 06:23because it's a disorder that
  • 06:25strikes when people are young.
  • 06:26And once it emerges,
  • 06:28it tends to be highly recurrent.
  • 06:30So we know that between 20 to 40% of
  • 06:33people who experience depression will
  • 06:36have had their first onset by age 21.
  • 06:39And we also know that about 3
  • 06:40out of every four people with
  • 06:42depression will experience at least
  • 06:44one relapse in their lifespan.
  • 06:46So when I hear data like this,
  • 06:48to me the the the message is we need to
  • 06:51better understand what causes depression,
  • 06:54what's its etiology,
  • 06:55how does it come about,
  • 06:56so that we can use those insights to
  • 06:59then identify targets to identify kids
  • 07:01who might be at risk and prevent the
  • 07:03onset of depression and do that as early
  • 07:06on as we possibly can in the lifespan.
  • 07:08So.
  • 07:09Committed to that goal.
  • 07:10The current focus of my research group
  • 07:13has been organized in these 44 domains.
  • 07:15So we do work on The Who,
  • 07:17how and the when question
  • 07:19around depression prevention.
  • 07:21So with respect to The Who,
  • 07:23a lot of what we do is focused on trying
  • 07:25to identify people at highest risk using
  • 07:27genetic and other markers of vulnerability.
  • 07:30So that's work that we do in in relationship
  • 07:32to the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium,
  • 07:34for example.
  • 07:35We also do a lot of work,
  • 07:37and I'm going to tell you a
  • 07:38lot about this work today.
  • 07:39Around the biological embedding of
  • 07:41adversity and the mechanisms that
  • 07:44might explain how it is that these
  • 07:46stressors and traumas might get
  • 07:48under our skin to shape our health,
  • 07:50the third area is really focused
  • 07:52on sensitive periods.
  • 07:53Try to understand,
  • 07:54are there ages in the course
  • 07:56of the lifespan when our life
  • 07:58experience matters more?
  • 07:59And could that differentially
  • 08:01predict risk for depression?
  • 08:02And then finally,
  • 08:03I'm someone that really believes
  • 08:05in and committed to translation,
  • 08:07so I don't want to just do ivory tower.
  • 08:10Client,
  • 08:10so to speak,
  • 08:10I want to figure out how
  • 08:12to get our findings out to make a difference.
  • 08:14So that's where we've also been doing
  • 08:16work to try to build novel infrastructure
  • 08:19for scientific knowledge translation.
  • 08:20And I'm proud to partner with Josh Rothman,
  • 08:23a colleague in psychiatry,
  • 08:24around a birth cohort work that
  • 08:26we're doing where we're deliberately
  • 08:28from the beginning trying to design
  • 08:30it to not just observe but also
  • 08:32intervene in those participants.
  • 08:33So what I want to do in this talk is tell you
  • 08:38more about two specific aspects of my labs.
  • 08:40Work related to childhood adversity.
  • 08:43So the first is work that we've
  • 08:44been doing to try to identify these
  • 08:46sensitive periods in development.
  • 08:48And then I'll also transition into
  • 08:50telling you more about what we're
  • 08:52doing to try to overcome measurement
  • 08:54challenges that exist in capturing
  • 08:56childhood diversity exposure.
  • 08:58And then at the end,
  • 08:59I'm not a clinician,
  • 09:00but I'll try to talk a little
  • 09:02bit about some of the clinical
  • 09:04implications and applications I
  • 09:06think might exist for this work.
  • 09:08So let me just also clarify at
  • 09:10the beginning because one of the
  • 09:11questions you might have is,
  • 09:13you know, childhood adversity,
  • 09:14trauma, aces,
  • 09:15do all of these things mean the same thing?
  • 09:18So from my perspective,
  • 09:19I tend to use the language of
  • 09:21childhood adversity because I
  • 09:23think it's more encompassing.
  • 09:25Childhood adversity is generally
  • 09:27defined as circumstances or events
  • 09:29that threaten children's physical
  • 09:31and psychological well-being and
  • 09:33their deviations from what you would
  • 09:35expect kids who are typically.
  • 09:37Developing should experience.
  • 09:38I think of aces as being a a set
  • 09:42of 10 markers that have been most
  • 09:44well studied in the context of of
  • 09:47adverse childhood experiences studies,
  • 09:49and so those are sometimes overlapping
  • 09:52with the adversities that we study,
  • 09:54but tend to sometimes not include
  • 09:56all of them.
  • 09:57Some of the adversities we study
  • 09:59could be stressors,
  • 10:00some could be traumas and toxic.
  • 10:02Stress to me really differentiates
  • 10:04the context surrounding the stressors
  • 10:06and traumas.
  • 10:07The kids are going through and
  • 10:09whether or not they have buffers,
  • 10:10mainly those protective adults
  • 10:12who can help buffer those effects
  • 10:14of those stressors for them.
  • 10:16So hopefully that's clarifying in
  • 10:18terms of just getting a better feel
  • 10:20for how I think about adversity.
  • 10:22So in terms of talking about identifying
  • 10:25sensitive periods in development.
  • 10:26So one of the big questions that
  • 10:29I think exists for the field is,
  • 10:31you know,
  • 10:31how does the timing of adversity
  • 10:33shape risk for depression or any
  • 10:36other adverse health outcome?
  • 10:37And if you turn to the literature,
  • 10:39you'll see that there's been a lot
  • 10:40of different theories that have
  • 10:42been proposed on this topic.
  • 10:43So a basic model is an exposure model.
  • 10:46And this model simply states that
  • 10:48people who've been exposed to
  • 10:50adversity have an increased risk of
  • 10:52an adverse health outcome relative
  • 10:54to people who are not exposed.
  • 10:56There's also accumulation models,
  • 10:58and in its simplest presentation,
  • 11:00I'm showing here a basic dose
  • 11:03response relationship.
  • 11:04So the more adversity,
  • 11:05the more at risk you become.
  • 11:07Now that could be exposure
  • 11:09to the same type repeatedly,
  • 11:11or it could be different types of exposure.
  • 11:14There's also recency models and
  • 11:17recency models. Oops, sorry.
  • 11:19Recency models focus on the time
  • 11:21since the onset of the event.
  • 11:26So a recency model says that your risk
  • 11:30of an adverse health outcome is greatest
  • 11:33shortly after you've been exposed,
  • 11:35but then your risk decreases over time.
  • 11:38And then finally there's
  • 11:39a sensitive period model.
  • 11:41And a sensitive period model is
  • 11:43really asking, are there specific
  • 11:44age stages in the course of the
  • 11:46lifespan when our experience matters?
  • 11:48More so over the years on and very
  • 11:52symbolically marking my 100th publication.
  • 11:55I love the symbolism.
  • 11:56Of that, we've spent, you know,
  • 11:58my group has spent a lot of time over
  • 12:00the last however many years to try to
  • 12:02disentangle which of these different
  • 12:04theories might best apply to our data.
  • 12:06And the reason that we've been doing that
  • 12:08is because we think it has important
  • 12:10implications for how we intervene.
  • 12:12So if the data we find are
  • 12:14consistent with an exposure model,
  • 12:16that suggests that we can
  • 12:17intervene at any time,
  • 12:18and it suggests that our goal is to
  • 12:20try to partition the population,
  • 12:22so to speak,
  • 12:23in terms of people who've been exposed
  • 12:25in those who haven't been exposed.
  • 12:26If our data are consistent with
  • 12:28an accumulation model,
  • 12:29then that points us to want to try
  • 12:32to intervene early before people are
  • 12:34accruing those adverse exposures.
  • 12:36If it's a recency model,
  • 12:38it suggests that we want to intervene
  • 12:40quickly,
  • 12:41but it might also suggest that maybe
  • 12:43doing nothing would be OK too.
  • 12:44These symptoms might naturally resolve
  • 12:47or risk would resolve overtime,
  • 12:49and if it's a sensitive period,
  • 12:50model suggests that we want to
  • 12:52intervene during or maybe shortly
  • 12:54before those time periods of
  • 12:57increased sensitivity.
  • 12:57So we think about sensitive periods
  • 12:59as being both high risk periods or
  • 13:02windows of vulnerability when adverse
  • 13:04life experiences are more harmful.
  • 13:06But they could also be windows
  • 13:08of opportunity,
  • 13:09when enriching interventions
  • 13:11could yield greater impact.
  • 13:13So we've been working over the
  • 13:15last several years to try to bring
  • 13:17more research evidence to this.
  • 13:19And my goal ultimately is to try to
  • 13:22enable policymakers and clinicians
  • 13:24to have better data to act,
  • 13:26to know not just what to what
  • 13:28to do to intervene,
  • 13:29but specifically when and to try to do
  • 13:31that on a high resolution timescale.
  • 13:33So in other words,
  • 13:35let's get more granular than just
  • 13:37saying early or saying 1000 days.
  • 13:40The 1st 1000 days rather.
  • 13:43So I want to tell you a little bit
  • 13:45more about the the first work that
  • 13:48we did this was back when I was a
  • 13:51postdoc and was a data set called
  • 13:53AD Health that's now following it
  • 13:55started studying kids when they
  • 13:57were in middle school,
  • 13:58and they've now been following
  • 14:00them through adulthood.
  • 14:02And the way that the data were
  • 14:04recorded in AD health gave us the
  • 14:06chance to ask this question about
  • 14:08sensitive periods because people
  • 14:09were asked were you exposed to
  • 14:11physical abuse and sexual abuse?
  • 14:13And if so,
  • 14:14how old were you when that first happened?
  • 14:17Now,
  • 14:17I know there's measurement challenges here,
  • 14:19and I'm going to come back to that.
  • 14:20But just to give you a sort of
  • 14:22intuition for how we've approached
  • 14:24these sensitive period studies.
  • 14:25So we code people based on whether
  • 14:28they've been exposed to adversity
  • 14:30during these different periods.
  • 14:32And what we end up finding is that
  • 14:34compared to people who were never exposed,
  • 14:37kids who were exposed to physical
  • 14:39abuse generally across the board
  • 14:41have an increased risk of depression.
  • 14:44Compared to kids who are unexposed,
  • 14:46but when we start to compare kids
  • 14:48based on the timing of their exposure,
  • 14:50we do see some within group differences.
  • 14:52So here we see that kids who are
  • 14:54exposed as preschoolers for the first
  • 14:56time have an increased risk of high
  • 14:58depressive symptoms compared to kids
  • 14:59who were exposed for the first time in
  • 15:02adolescence and similarly for sexual abuse.
  • 15:05We find generally across the
  • 15:07board this increased risk,
  • 15:08but here too these potential
  • 15:10sensitive periods this was shifted
  • 15:12to be latency or school age period.
  • 15:14Relative to preschool or relative
  • 15:17to the prepubertal period?
  • 15:20Over the years we've searched
  • 15:22broadly for sensitive periods,
  • 15:24trying to see the level where
  • 15:25they might operate.
  • 15:26So the earliest work that we did
  • 15:28was looking at childhood adversity
  • 15:29in relation to depression and
  • 15:31other forms of psychopathology.
  • 15:33But then we started,
  • 15:34this is really based on my
  • 15:36interest in genetics,
  • 15:37starting to think about these
  • 15:39intermediate phenotypes.
  • 15:40So in other words,
  • 15:42are there these measures that we can
  • 15:44get that are maybe more proximal to risk
  • 15:47based on their timing of occurrence?
  • 15:50In other words,
  • 15:51these measures that are maybe
  • 15:53capturing more of the biology of
  • 15:55the the short-term effects of
  • 15:57exposure to childhood adversity.
  • 15:59And maybe these are the level,
  • 16:01this is the level where we might see
  • 16:03more signal and might be more readily
  • 16:05able to identify sensitive periods.
  • 16:07So we've looked at a number of
  • 16:09different intermediate phenotypes
  • 16:10and I'll tell you more about those.
  • 16:12You know everything from how kids
  • 16:14cope with stress to executive
  • 16:16function and more recently,
  • 16:18molecular targets.
  • 16:21Throughout this work,
  • 16:22we've also been focused on trying to
  • 16:24develop and apply better analytic tools,
  • 16:26particularly to analyze longitudinal
  • 16:28data where you have these repeated
  • 16:30measures of adversity and where
  • 16:32sometimes they're highly correlated.
  • 16:34So this is an approach that we've
  • 16:36been working on with my colleague
  • 16:38Andrew Smith out of the University
  • 16:40of West of England in the UK.
  • 16:42So it's called the structured
  • 16:44life course modeling approach,
  • 16:45or the slick comma and slick
  • 16:48comma is incredibly cool.
  • 16:50It works really well when you
  • 16:53have repeated measures data.
  • 16:55It can work when you have measures that are
  • 16:58measured close in time or more distally,
  • 17:00in time.
  • 17:00What I also really like about it is
  • 17:03that it forces you to have ideas up
  • 17:05front about what you think you might see.
  • 17:08So it's not just a a fishing expedition,
  • 17:10but you have to have some idea
  • 17:12about what you think might be the
  • 17:14theory at play so that you can then
  • 17:17encode your theories into testable
  • 17:19hypotheses. And So what the what?
  • 17:21The way that it works is that it
  • 17:24essentially allows you to identify
  • 17:25from the combination of theories
  • 17:27that you've identified beforehand,
  • 17:29which set explain the most amount
  • 17:31of variation in your outcome.
  • 17:33So it basically works in three stages.
  • 17:35So the first thing you do is you take
  • 17:37all of your theoretical models and
  • 17:39then you encode them into a variable.
  • 17:41So, for example, if you're going
  • 17:43to test a sensitive period model,
  • 17:45you code people based on being exposed
  • 17:48during that time period versus outside.
  • 17:51An accumulation model,
  • 17:52you're coding the number of
  • 17:54exposures and so on and so forth.
  • 17:56And these aren't the only life course models,
  • 17:58I should say, that you could test.
  • 18:00But there's other kinds of models too,
  • 18:02like mobility models.
  • 18:03So where a kid might have social
  • 18:05support and then they don't at the
  • 18:07next time and then it comes back again.
  • 18:10And So what you end up doing is you
  • 18:12take all of these variables and then
  • 18:14you bring them into a regression model.
  • 18:16And the way the regression model is
  • 18:18working is it's in a very sequential
  • 18:19fashion where you're trying to.
  • 18:21Identify the amount of variation
  • 18:23in your outcome or R-squared that
  • 18:25is explained by the greatest
  • 18:28combination of variables and.
  • 18:29So what you can see in this example
  • 18:32is where the model keeps fitting
  • 18:34until it gets to this combination
  • 18:36of both accumulation and exposure
  • 18:38during that third time period.
  • 18:40And So what you're able to also do is
  • 18:42you can look at these elbow plots,
  • 18:44which is what I'm showing here in the middle,
  • 18:46but then you can also evaluate
  • 18:49fit quantitatively using post
  • 18:51selective inference.
  • 18:52And so we've used the slick law
  • 18:54over the years and and also other
  • 18:56analysis to look at you know,
  • 18:58psychopathologies,
  • 18:59suicide risk,
  • 19:00sleep and and other more intermediate
  • 19:04phenotypes.
  • 19:04I want to tell you more about the
  • 19:06work that we've been doing where
  • 19:08we've been engaged in the most
  • 19:10work so far and that's related to
  • 19:13DNA methylation and epigenetics.
  • 19:15So these are chemical tags that are
  • 19:17essentially added to your genome.
  • 19:19They don't change how your DNA sequence.
  • 19:23Is is shaped, but they change.
  • 19:25They have the potential to change
  • 19:27how your genes function.
  • 19:28So they're one pathway through which
  • 19:31adversity might end up affecting
  • 19:33depression and other adverse health outcomes.
  • 19:36So one of the main studies that we've
  • 19:38been using for this is a study called alpac,
  • 19:41where the Avon Longitudinal
  • 19:43Study of Parents and Children,
  • 19:45which for any of you who might
  • 19:46be shopping a data set,
  • 19:47is a wonderful data set they have.
  • 19:50It's a birth cohort and the kids are,
  • 19:52the kids are now in their.
  • 19:5330 So there's you know 30 years of data.
  • 19:57They also collected as part of the
  • 19:59sub sample about 1000 mother child
  • 20:02pairs with DNA methylation data.
  • 20:04And so that was what we ended
  • 20:06up analyzing here.
  • 20:07So we had repeated measures of
  • 20:09exposure to different types
  • 20:10of adversity, things that were happening
  • 20:13within the household up through markers of
  • 20:15neighborhood disadvantage and we coded those
  • 20:17based on the timing of occurrence and then
  • 20:20we looked at these markers of adversity.
  • 20:23In relation to DNA methylation,
  • 20:26a type of epigenetic modification at about
  • 20:30500,000 different sites across the epigenome.
  • 20:33And so we applied the slickman and we asked,
  • 20:35you know, what's the best theoretical
  • 20:37model that might explain the variation
  • 20:40that we see in these epigenetic marks?
  • 20:42Is it accumulation, recency,
  • 20:44sensitive period or maybe a combination?
  • 20:48And what we did was we ended
  • 20:50up analyzing the data.
  • 20:51So on the X axis here is chromosome,
  • 20:54on the Y axis is the negative
  • 20:56log of the P value.
  • 20:57So it's basically the test
  • 21:00of statistical significance.
  • 21:02This is a Manhattan plot.
  • 21:04So ideally it looks like Manhattan
  • 21:05where you see these skyscraper like
  • 21:07effects emerging from the data.
  • 21:09You don't want a Dutch plot,
  • 21:10you don't want it to look flat because
  • 21:13these are basically regions where you're
  • 21:15seeing you know interesting signal.
  • 21:17So and then because we test literally
  • 21:21500,000 different associations,
  • 21:23we correct for that testing.
  • 21:25So anything that's considered to
  • 21:26be above that line is considered
  • 21:29epigenome wide significant.
  • 21:30And so we ended up finding 46 loci
  • 21:33that were distributed throughout
  • 21:35the epigenome as being potentially
  • 21:38impacted by adversity.
  • 21:40And when we dug deeper into these results,
  • 21:43what we ended up finding that
  • 21:45more than half of the loci.
  • 21:47We identified were influenced
  • 21:49by exposure to adversity,
  • 21:50specifically between ages three to five.
  • 21:53I actually didn't expect that we'd find
  • 21:55such strong evidence for sensitive periods.
  • 21:57I was thinking accumulation might
  • 21:59be as important, but it wasn't.
  • 22:01Here we actually didn't identify any loci.
  • 22:04What's also interesting is that
  • 22:06these DNA differences weren't
  • 22:07actually present at birth.
  • 22:08So we looked at whether they happened
  • 22:10in cord blood and they didn't.
  • 22:12And we've been now working.
  • 22:14We're probably about a month off or
  • 22:16so from wrapping up efforts around.
  • 22:17A meta analysis that we've been doing
  • 22:20to try to replicate and extend these
  • 22:22findings and other datasets to see
  • 22:25if they hold and by and large spoiler
  • 22:27alert is I think most of the data,
  • 22:29most of the evidence we are seeing
  • 22:32is for sensitive periods relative
  • 22:34to these other models.
  • 22:36One thing I also want to say too is,
  • 22:39you know,
  • 22:39one of the questions that I think is,
  • 22:41is really fair is these methods
  • 22:43seem really complicated, you know,
  • 22:45is is the juice worth the squeeze
  • 22:47so to speak?
  • 22:48Do you actually get more if you,
  • 22:49you know,
  • 22:50if you get all these repeated
  • 22:52measures and you model it with this
  • 22:54sophisticated modeling approach,
  • 22:55the answer is yes.
  • 22:57So we are able to identify with
  • 22:59the slick ma more signal that we
  • 23:01would have missed had we just
  • 23:03coded people as exposed versus.
  • 23:05Unexposed.
  • 23:06So I think hopefully you hear
  • 23:08a message here of you know
  • 23:10it is worth it to do this
  • 23:12more repeated measures data collection and
  • 23:15use these these more complicated methods.
  • 23:20We've also been working and this
  • 23:22is work led by Alex Lucier,
  • 23:23a postdoc in my group, because we have
  • 23:26longitudinal methylation data in alpac.
  • 23:29So not just looking at methylation at age 7,
  • 23:32but he's also been expanding it
  • 23:34to methylation at age 15 to 17.
  • 23:37So we can understand these patterns
  • 23:39of stability and change across time.
  • 23:41And these, these data are really interesting
  • 23:43and I'm just going to present a a little
  • 23:46bit of what we've been finding here.
  • 23:48So what I'm showing here.
  • 23:49Are the 46 low side that I showed before,
  • 23:53so these were the top low side
  • 23:55that we identified at age 7.
  • 23:57Now we're looking at them at age 15 and
  • 23:59saying do we still see them being you
  • 24:02know largely important and generally
  • 24:04what we find is that the direction of
  • 24:07change or the the pattern of direction
  • 24:11of association is generally the same
  • 24:14but the results are attenuating slightly.
  • 24:16So had we run an epigenome Wide Association
  • 24:19study we wouldn't have identified these.
  • 24:21Game low Sci at age 15.
  • 24:25What we're finding actually now
  • 24:27is a new set of loci at age 15,
  • 24:31so 41 in total and interesting.
  • 24:34These are also underscoring the
  • 24:36importance of this early childhood
  • 24:38of this age three to five period.
  • 24:40So we didn't see them before at 7,
  • 24:42but now we're starting to see them.
  • 24:43So sort of interesting to think about maybe
  • 24:46potential sleeper effects or latency effects.
  • 24:49We don't really know what's
  • 24:50necessarily going on here,
  • 24:51but we're we're starting to
  • 24:53try to unpack this and ask,
  • 24:54you know what might be giving rise to these?
  • 24:56These patterns.
  • 24:57We've also looked,
  • 24:59you know,
  • 25:00at these data and we can find
  • 25:02so far at least six different
  • 25:04patterns of adversity associated
  • 25:06methylation differences across time.
  • 25:08And these patterns essentially
  • 25:10reflect differences that emerge
  • 25:11early versus later in development,
  • 25:14those that happen among people
  • 25:15who are exposed to adversity.
  • 25:17But what's interesting here is we
  • 25:19see differences based on whether
  • 25:21you were exposed during the
  • 25:22sensitive period we think might be
  • 25:24impactful versus outside of it.
  • 25:26And there's some cases where
  • 25:27people who were exposed.
  • 25:29During the sensitive period,
  • 25:31look like people who were unexposed.
  • 25:34And then we're also seeing
  • 25:36differences in in age differences
  • 25:39based on age and assessment.
  • 25:41And I think this is really interesting
  • 25:43in terms of thinking about again
  • 25:44a kind of is the juice worth the
  • 25:46squeeze question of you know,
  • 25:48is it worth us getting these repeated
  • 25:50measures of methylation and and I think
  • 25:52at least from what we're seeing it is.
  • 25:57So. Umm. You might also be wondering,
  • 26:02OK, this is interesting,
  • 26:04adversaries predicting methylation,
  • 26:05but what's actually
  • 26:06happening in terms of health?
  • 26:09And Alex, who's a postdoc,
  • 26:10as I mentioned, and Brooke Smith,
  • 26:12who was a data analyst in my group,
  • 26:14have been doing a mediation analysis,
  • 26:16mediation analysis to essentially
  • 26:18ask is adversity leading to changes
  • 26:21in these DNA methylation signatures
  • 26:23that then predict risk for depression
  • 26:26and what we're looking at here.
  • 26:29So we could basically calculate
  • 26:30all of these different paths.
  • 26:32Using regression and what we're looking
  • 26:34for is to try to identify, you know,
  • 26:37how much of the association is explained
  • 26:40by these methylation signatures.
  • 26:42And So what we found overall is so far
  • 26:4570 total mediators that were identified
  • 26:49across these different adversities,
  • 26:51corresponding to 667 unique CPG
  • 26:55sites that that each explained
  • 26:58between 10 and 71% of the variation.
  • 27:02In risk for depression.
  • 27:04So you can see that there's differences in,
  • 27:06you know,
  • 27:07how much is being explained across
  • 27:09these different types of adversities.
  • 27:11And then what I think is maybe really
  • 27:14interesting is that the epigenetic
  • 27:16adaptation that we're seeing is not uniform.
  • 27:19So when we plot the direction of these
  • 27:22different associations and whether
  • 27:24adversities associated with increased
  • 27:26methylation or decreased methylation,
  • 27:29we're seeing a lot of variation.
  • 27:31So what this is essentially showing
  • 27:33is that most of what we're finding
  • 27:37are effects where methylation changes
  • 27:39are actually protective against.
  • 27:42Depression.
  • 27:43So adversity is associated with a
  • 27:46methylation change that protects
  • 27:48people from developing depression.
  • 27:50We've also been finding that some
  • 27:52sites that we've identified are linked
  • 27:54to cortical development and and other
  • 27:56aspects of brain development and we've
  • 27:58been able to replicate some of the
  • 28:01LOCI and some independent cohorts.
  • 28:03And I think this is another area
  • 28:05that's just ripe for investigation
  • 28:07because it's counterintuitive.
  • 28:09I think most of us would expect
  • 28:11that these things are, you know,
  • 28:13more deleterious.
  • 28:13But it might be that we're,
  • 28:15our bodies are trying to reach homeostasis.
  • 28:17And so we're some of the damage
  • 28:20that's done is protective and
  • 28:22some of it is also harmful.
  • 28:24I also want to just kind of
  • 28:26zoom out and sort of share with
  • 28:28you the last set of work around
  • 28:30sensitive periods in terms of this,
  • 28:32this review paper that John Schaefer,
  • 28:34who's a a postdoc collaborator of
  • 28:37mine and I worked on around the
  • 28:39question of sensitive periods.
  • 28:41So I've been really surprised that the
  • 28:44data we've been seeing for methylation
  • 28:47has been so consistent for sensitive
  • 28:50periods and we wanted to know you know,
  • 28:52does this really extend to.
  • 28:54Other domains.
  • 28:54So we ended up publishing this review.
  • 28:57It just came out a couple weeks ago
  • 28:59looking at a range of different outcomes.
  • 29:02So psychopathology,
  • 29:03neuroimaging, epigenetics,
  • 29:05psychophysiology and behavior.
  • 29:07It's defined by our doc,
  • 29:09the research domain criteria.
  • 29:11So we found 118 unique cross-sectional
  • 29:15observational studies.
  • 29:17Most of these studies focused
  • 29:19on psychopathology as at least
  • 29:21one of their outcomes,
  • 29:22so depressive symptoms or diagnosis
  • 29:25or other PTSD or so on and so forth.
  • 29:30Other ones we're looking at are
  • 29:31other R DOC domains and a handful.
  • 29:33We're also looking at more neural indices.
  • 29:37What we ended up finding was that most
  • 29:39studies did report a timing difference.
  • 29:42In other words, they reported that kids
  • 29:45exposed to maltreatment in one time
  • 29:47period had an increased risk relative
  • 29:48to kids exposed at another time period.
  • 29:51But when we dug deeper into
  • 29:53these timing effects,
  • 29:54we essentially didn't find any consistent
  • 29:57evidence for peak periods of vulnerability.
  • 30:00So it's not as though we saw three to five or
  • 30:046 to 8 is this time period of vulnerability.
  • 30:07This was also very surprising.
  • 30:09So we didn't see that these
  • 30:12biological markers, you know,
  • 30:14the neural indices or other indicators were
  • 30:18any better able than the symptom measures
  • 30:21to identify potential sensitive periods.
  • 30:24We also didn't see any
  • 30:26differences based on study rigor.
  • 30:27So if you had a study where you
  • 30:30compared your models to accumulation
  • 30:32models or you were a larger study,
  • 30:35we didn't see any differences based on that.
  • 30:37Neither we did interestingly share find
  • 30:41that there were similarities in terms of
  • 30:44internalizing and externalizing symptoms.
  • 30:46They did share peak periods of vulnerability,
  • 30:49but specific types of maltreatment did not.
  • 30:51So this maybe speaks to maltreatment
  • 30:54types having potential different impacts
  • 30:57with respect to sensitive periods.
  • 30:59Studies were also split with respect
  • 31:02with respect to sex differences.
  • 31:05We also generally saw a huge risk of bias.
  • 31:09Most of these studies were under powered
  • 31:11and so as a result we ended up providing
  • 31:14a set of recommendations at the end
  • 31:16that we hope will guide future studies,
  • 31:19including but not limited
  • 31:20to issues of of measurement,
  • 31:22which I'm going to turn to next.
  • 31:24So in terms of the issue of measurement,
  • 31:28so this is something I've been
  • 31:30frustrated about for for a while.
  • 31:33So we know that current measures
  • 31:35of childhood adversity have
  • 31:37some pretty serious limitations.
  • 31:39So what we most often do in in research
  • 31:41studies is we ask people and this
  • 31:43is also in clinical practice too.
  • 31:45We ask people retrospectively.
  • 31:47So when you're an adult or maybe an an
  • 31:50adolescent, we ask you how old you know,
  • 31:52did you experience these adverse
  • 31:54events and so.
  • 31:55You might imagine that there's
  • 31:57a lot of potential bias here.
  • 31:58So, you know,
  • 31:59it's subjects to people's memory.
  • 32:01It's subject to whether they're
  • 32:03comfortable disclosing what are
  • 32:05oftentimes very painful events.
  • 32:07So it's no surprise that, you know,
  • 32:09there might be bias in these
  • 32:11retrospective measures.
  • 32:11The other thing that we can also
  • 32:13do is go prospectively.
  • 32:15So we can ask parents,
  • 32:17oftentimes moms,
  • 32:18whether their child is exposed
  • 32:20to certain kinds of events.
  • 32:22But this is another area where
  • 32:24there's potential problems.
  • 32:26So moms might not want to talk
  • 32:28about painful events or events,
  • 32:30particularly when she's the perpetrator
  • 32:33of those sources of adversity.
  • 32:35For adolescence,
  • 32:36there might be some adversities
  • 32:37that parents don't know about,
  • 32:39and I think This is why it's maybe
  • 32:41there's no surprise that when you
  • 32:43ask both children and their parents,
  • 32:45you see very low levels of
  • 32:47agreement between the two of them.
  • 32:49Another source of data would
  • 32:50be the official reports,
  • 32:51like health and Social service records,
  • 32:53but we know that those are also
  • 32:55dramatic undercounts of people's.
  • 32:57Exposure to adversity and they probably
  • 33:00only get about 30% of all true cases.
  • 33:03So, so sort of borne from these
  • 33:05frustrations and a very serendipitous
  • 33:07conversation I had with a colleague
  • 33:10that I started thinking about baby
  • 33:12teeth and this idea that maybe baby
  • 33:14teeth could serve as fossilized records
  • 33:17of people's early life experiences.
  • 33:19So we published this paper.
  • 33:22Back in 2020 in biological psychiatry,
  • 33:24where we outline this hypothesis
  • 33:26and so we said we basically put
  • 33:28forward this teeth conceptual model,
  • 33:31this idea that teeth are as encoding
  • 33:34experiences to transform health.
  • 33:36And So what we said is that
  • 33:37you have this exposure,
  • 33:38so a psychosocial stressor,
  • 33:41it disrupts some biological process.
  • 33:44It leaves behind an imprint of that
  • 33:47biological process somewhere and
  • 33:48that that predicts health outcomes.
  • 33:50And So what we were saying.
  • 33:52That essentially primary tooth
  • 33:53development might be altered as a
  • 33:56result of this adversity and that could
  • 33:59then therefore be captured in baby
  • 34:01teeth that started forming prenatally.
  • 34:04So let me tell you a little
  • 34:06bit more about teeth.
  • 34:07I could talk an entire talk about teeth
  • 34:09because they're like absolutely fascinating,
  • 34:11but in the interest of time,
  • 34:12I won't do that.
  • 34:13But,
  • 34:14but just to give you a little bit more of a
  • 34:16flavor for teeth and in how cool they are.
  • 34:18So,
  • 34:18so most of us are born with 20 primary teeth.
  • 34:21These are our baby.
  • 34:22Death or milk teeth,
  • 34:23they start forming during about the
  • 34:25second trimester of life and then they
  • 34:27continue forming over the first few
  • 34:29years of life and then around age 5 or six,
  • 34:32they fall out.
  • 34:32They're the only part of our
  • 34:34body that actually falls out
  • 34:35as part of a healthy process.
  • 34:37And then they're replaced
  • 34:39by 32 permanent teeth.
  • 34:41And those form postnatally up
  • 34:43through about mid adolescence.
  • 34:45And so teeth are also amazing because
  • 34:47they record the timing of their
  • 34:50incremental growth, so the outside.
  • 34:52Part of our tooth is called the
  • 34:54Crown and that's comprised of the
  • 34:56enamel that we hopefully brush
  • 34:58twice a day in our underlying
  • 35:00dentin and then the pulp and root.
  • 35:02And the way that teeth develop
  • 35:04is really very much reminiscent
  • 35:06of a circadian like process.
  • 35:08So there are cells called ameloblasts
  • 35:10and those are the cells that form
  • 35:12enamel and they're basically acting
  • 35:14in a in a circadian like process
  • 35:16to lay down this matrix of enamel.
  • 35:18And as every sort of passage of time goes on,
  • 35:22it leaves.
  • 35:23Behind an imprint of that recording.
  • 35:26So this is similar to the way that
  • 35:28tree rings develop and that every
  • 35:30year of the trees development
  • 35:31you see a new ring recorded.
  • 35:33Well, our teeth have very similar lines.
  • 35:36There are sets of lines that
  • 35:38correspond to about weekly
  • 35:39development, and then lines that also
  • 35:42correspond to about daily development.
  • 35:44What's also unique is that this recording
  • 35:46of development is found across evolution,
  • 35:48so we see similar tree similar rings
  • 35:51within the teeth across different species.
  • 35:54And teeth also record insults or disruptions
  • 35:56that happen during their development.
  • 35:58So in this way we can think about teeth
  • 36:01just telling us not just whether a stressor
  • 36:03occurred in development but potentially when.
  • 36:05And this can happen on both a low
  • 36:08resolution time scale where you can
  • 36:10see for example these white marks or
  • 36:12these enamel hypoplasia or concentrated
  • 36:14to those two central incisors.
  • 36:16So that maybe speaks to something
  • 36:18that was happening as those particular
  • 36:21teeth were forming,
  • 36:22but then you can get even more granular
  • 36:24and look really at a high resolution.
  • 36:26Time scale and leverage what we know
  • 36:28about those tree ring like structures.
  • 36:30So you could take a tooth,
  • 36:31cut it in half,
  • 36:32take thin sections of the tooth,
  • 36:34put it on a slide,
  • 36:36put it under a microscope and
  • 36:38look at the incremental formation
  • 36:40of that tooth development.
  • 36:42And one of the lines that you can look
  • 36:44at among others is this neonatal line.
  • 36:47So this is a line that actually
  • 36:49differentiates the time of our birth.
  • 36:51So it differentiates prenatal enamel
  • 36:53from post Natal enamel and it's often.
  • 36:56Is in studies of archaeology and
  • 36:58anthropology as as a way of differentiating
  • 37:01those different time periods.
  • 37:03But then there's also other
  • 37:04lines that you can look at,
  • 37:05and these are generally referred
  • 37:06to as stress lines.
  • 37:08Whether they happen prenatally,
  • 37:09anthropologists don't really know.
  • 37:11So this is part of what
  • 37:12we're trying to look at, Umm.
  • 37:14And also these lines occur,
  • 37:17as I said, at different time scales.
  • 37:19So.
  • 37:19So you can get pretty granular with teeth.
  • 37:22Most of the work that's been done
  • 37:24so far around teeth as markers of
  • 37:27stress really focus on Physiology,
  • 37:29physiological stressors,
  • 37:31so disease, malnutrition.
  • 37:33The process of our birth and the recording
  • 37:35of that neonatal line and most of this
  • 37:38happens in archaeological populations
  • 37:39and there is a little bit that's
  • 37:41going on in more modern populations.
  • 37:44But what I think is interesting
  • 37:46is that there are primate studies
  • 37:48that have shown that teeth might
  • 37:50record psychosocial stress.
  • 37:51So Simona Lemmers is a postdoc in my group.
  • 37:54She's a biological anthropologist.
  • 37:56She's been doing some of this work
  • 37:58and essentially shows that different
  • 38:00kinds of events that happen within.
  • 38:03For her study,
  • 38:04it was mandrills.
  • 38:05So you can see these stress lines appearing
  • 38:08shortly after the occurrence of a stressor.
  • 38:11So, for example,
  • 38:13you separate the offspring.
  • 38:14From the mother and you'll see that the
  • 38:18baby tooth will show evidence of that
  • 38:21calendar timed event appearing in the tooth.
  • 38:24So it sort of suggests that maybe there
  • 38:27is something going on about teeth
  • 38:30recording these early life stressors.
  • 38:32So now going from, OK,
  • 38:35so maybe early life stress can
  • 38:37get recorded in teeth.
  • 38:38Well,
  • 38:38what about teeth as a marker
  • 38:40of mental health?
  • 38:40Well, there's been work mostly in
  • 38:43environmental health showing that pesticides,
  • 38:45things that you can ingest or inhale,
  • 38:48those can appear in teeth.
  • 38:50And those are also indicative
  • 38:51of mental health risks.
  • 38:52So for example,
  • 38:54studies have focused on heavy metals
  • 38:57and lead like lead and and other things,
  • 39:01and showing risk for a range of.
  • 39:02Different psychiatric disorders,
  • 39:04autism spectrum disorder,
  • 39:05schizophrenia and the like.
  • 39:08But there really hasn't been a
  • 39:10lot that's been done specifically
  • 39:12in child psychiatry, I think,
  • 39:14and in depression in particular.
  • 39:16And I think this is where teeth Perot
  • 39:18may provide this enormous and unique
  • 39:21opportunity for primary prevention.
  • 39:23So if you think back to the beginning
  • 39:25of the talk where I shared that 20 to
  • 39:2740% of people will have had a first
  • 39:30onset of depression before age 21.
  • 39:31And you think about what I just shared
  • 39:33in terms of the timing of tooth formation
  • 39:36happening early in development,
  • 39:37you can think about every time.
  • 39:39Point when teeth are lost as a
  • 39:41potential opportunity to intervene.
  • 39:43So the first time happens when
  • 39:45teeth are naturally exfoliated,
  • 39:46they fall out of your mouth around
  • 39:48school age, you know, 5 or 6.
  • 39:50Instead of throwing those in the garbage,
  • 39:53what if they were potentially used to
  • 39:55help guide primary prevention efforts?
  • 39:57Similarly,
  • 39:58second opportunity comes for
  • 40:00orthodontic work.
  • 40:01So in the US about 20% of kids will
  • 40:04have at least one tooth extracted
  • 40:06to make room for those braces.
  • 40:08Here too is another opportunity and
  • 40:10also at a time where we start to
  • 40:12see upkicks in risk for depression
  • 40:14and other forms of psychopathology.
  • 40:16And then the last time comes with
  • 40:18wisdom tooth removal surgery.
  • 40:20So this also happens in that transition
  • 40:23to from adolescence to adulthood,
  • 40:25kids are starting to live outside
  • 40:27of the home.
  • 40:27For the first time we start to
  • 40:29see psychosis and other major
  • 40:31psychiatric disorders.
  • 40:32So here imagine again if we might
  • 40:34be able to use these as potential
  • 40:37biomarkers in combination with other
  • 40:39tools to help identify kids at risk.
  • 40:41So so seeing all of these this
  • 40:44under you know under studied area
  • 40:46and seeing the potential I decided
  • 40:48several years ago that I wanted
  • 40:50to become the science tooth fairy
  • 40:52and and try to study baby teeth.
  • 40:55So this is a the cover of a children's.
  • 40:58Look, we actually wrote,
  • 40:59so when kids are recruited into our study,
  • 41:02we use this book as a way of talking
  • 41:04with kids and family about why they
  • 41:06should donate their teeth to us.
  • 41:08I have copies of the book too,
  • 41:09if anyone's interested in it.
  • 41:12I'll just share a very high level,
  • 41:13a couple of ideas part in the pond,
  • 41:15but I just have to. I love puns.
  • 41:17Well, we've been sinking our teeth into,
  • 41:19in terms of this teeth conceptual model.
  • 41:21So Simona Lemmers, Mona lawyer,
  • 41:242 postdocs in my lab and Ryan Lisanne,
  • 41:26who's a pediatric dental resident
  • 41:29at Children's.
  • 41:31So we've been doing work on
  • 41:32the empirical side,
  • 41:33you know, can we see evidence of
  • 41:36markers in in teeth being predicted
  • 41:38by exposure to early life stress?
  • 41:41We published a paper.
  • 41:42Last year showing that markers of Mom
  • 41:45depression and social support were
  • 41:47associated with that neonatal line
  • 41:49and in the direction we expected.
  • 41:51So more stressful births in the
  • 41:54form of higher psychopathology,
  • 41:56wider neonatal line,
  • 41:57conversely more social support,
  • 42:00narrower neonatal line.
  • 42:02And that was also pack.
  • 42:05We also started a study,
  • 42:06we just finished recruitment for
  • 42:08it in the spring called strong
  • 42:09the stories teeth record of
  • 42:11newborn growth where we have.
  • 42:12In recruiting the moms,
  • 42:13recruiting the offspring of women
  • 42:15who are pregnant or raising a
  • 42:17newborn during the timing of
  • 42:19the Boston Marathon bombing.
  • 42:20So the idea here is we have a calendar
  • 42:22dated major stressful life event.
  • 42:24Can we see evidence of that
  • 42:27recorded in kids teeth?
  • 42:29We've also been doing work to then link
  • 42:31what we see in teeth with mental health.
  • 42:33So we published a paper showing
  • 42:35that markers derived from micro
  • 42:37CT of enamel volume and thickness
  • 42:39predicted levels of psychopathology
  • 42:41symptoms in kindergarten age kids.
  • 42:44And then we also have some work
  • 42:46looking at kind of the timing
  • 42:48and pacing of these growth marks
  • 42:50predicting weight gain in adolescence.
  • 42:53And then the last thing that
  • 42:54we've been working on are more
  • 42:55feasibility kinds of studies.
  • 42:56So teeth are new biomarkers,
  • 42:58you know we need to.
  • 42:59You know,
  • 43:00it's scientists and clinicians
  • 43:01how we should be talking about
  • 43:02them with parents and families,
  • 43:03particularly help us understand
  • 43:05how we can use them.
  • 43:06So we've also been doing studies
  • 43:08to try to understand.
  • 43:09What do people think about teeth?
  • 43:12I I laugh when I get emails about them.
  • 43:14Sometimes Mom will say, you know,
  • 43:16dear Doctor Dunn, I've read about your study.
  • 43:19I saved my child's baby teeth.
  • 43:20And then it's either one of two things.
  • 43:23I'm so glad I saved them or you.
  • 43:25Isn't that gross?
  • 43:26I don't know why I saved them.
  • 43:27Something along those lines.
  • 43:29So.
  • 43:29I think there's a lot that we
  • 43:31can potentially learn here,
  • 43:31and I think that will be important
  • 43:33for building a solid foundation
  • 43:35for this work to unfold.
  • 43:37So let me just wrap up by saying a
  • 43:39little bit more on the translational
  • 43:41side in terms of where I see this
  • 43:44work potentially going in terms of
  • 43:46promoting resilience and trying
  • 43:48to reduce health disparities.
  • 43:50You know we talked very early on
  • 43:52in the pandemic about us living
  • 43:54through unprecedented times.
  • 43:55I don't feel like we,
  • 43:56you hear that as much now,
  • 43:57but I still think we very much are.
  • 44:00So you know,
  • 44:01we have all these stressors that
  • 44:02people experience before the pandemic,
  • 44:05you know add on these additional stressors.
  • 44:07That people are experiencing as
  • 44:09a result of the pandemic.
  • 44:10But I also think simultaneously
  • 44:12we're seeing these shifts that are
  • 44:15happening largely as a result of the
  • 44:17civil rights movement around racial
  • 44:19equality and some institutional
  • 44:21practices that are
  • 44:22starting to shift where.
  • 44:27Oh, OK. Thank you.
  • 44:29Umm, where we're seeing some
  • 44:31movement to potentially better
  • 44:33address some of these areas.
  • 44:34And I think where we are as scientists
  • 44:36and also as clinicians is that,
  • 44:38you know, we have the chance to really,
  • 44:40I think, develop a deeper and more meaningful
  • 44:43research agenda to try to understand,
  • 44:45you know, opportunities to identify
  • 44:48ways to promote health and reduce
  • 44:51risk and build some interventions
  • 44:53to really promote resilience.
  • 44:56I think there's at least two.
  • 44:57Main starting points for
  • 44:58where we can go in this front,
  • 45:01I think one is we spend a ton
  • 45:03of time focusing on the bad.
  • 45:05We do a lot of work and adversity and trauma,
  • 45:08you know,
  • 45:09and I think the resilience world,
  • 45:10there's definitely been a
  • 45:11lot of work in this area,
  • 45:13but I don't think that the
  • 45:15resilience work has necessarily
  • 45:16been as integrated in areas of
  • 45:18biology where I think it could.
  • 45:20So I was sharing with some of you that
  • 45:22we just had a grant that hopefully
  • 45:24will get funded that will allow us
  • 45:26to look at the biological embedding.
  • 45:28Of protective factors.
  • 45:29And I think this is something that
  • 45:31we need to bring in as part of our
  • 45:33research model so that we're not just
  • 45:35studying risk because we know that
  • 45:37risk alone doesn't predict outcomes,
  • 45:39but it's a constellation
  • 45:41of different factors.
  • 45:42I think the other thing too is that
  • 45:45we also need to do more to develop
  • 45:47and implement tools to measure
  • 45:49childhood adversity and differentiate
  • 45:51exposure from the biological
  • 45:53consequences of that exposure.
  • 45:54And I think this is really, really hard,
  • 45:57but I'm hoping maybe we're baby.
  • 45:59Keith and and some of our epigenetic
  • 46:01work can go and I think this is really
  • 46:03critical because you might find that
  • 46:05some kid has the exposure but seems
  • 46:07to be doing OK you know there's
  • 46:09individual differences in this adversities,
  • 46:11not deterministic.
  • 46:12So I think being able to disentangle
  • 46:15these is going to be really critical.
  • 46:17In terms of the applications and
  • 46:20implications of the epigenetics
  • 46:21and exfoliated primary teeth work,
  • 46:24you know, I'm an epidemiologist,
  • 46:25so I don't always have the the
  • 46:27the fortune of being able to talk
  • 46:30with parents and families.
  • 46:31But when I do,
  • 46:32I'm always struck by the questions they ask.
  • 46:35And they always ask two things.
  • 46:37The first thing is they want answers.
  • 46:38They want to know why did my loved
  • 46:41one develop a mental health issue?
  • 46:43They want to know if you know their
  • 46:45child being exposed at this age,
  • 46:46you know, caused this,
  • 46:47and then they also want hope.
  • 46:49They want to know what can be done
  • 46:50to prevent some mental health
  • 46:52issue and someone else they love.
  • 46:54And so I think, you know,
  • 46:55what if baby teeth,
  • 46:57when paired with existing tools
  • 46:58and insights like family history
  • 47:00and genetic and other markers,
  • 47:02could provide answers to some
  • 47:04of these burning questions.
  • 47:06And you know,
  • 47:07they these things are something
  • 47:09that naturally
  • 47:09fall out of our mouth and most times they're
  • 47:12either stored or they're thrown away.
  • 47:14But what if instead, these really hidden
  • 47:16in plain sight objects could be used
  • 47:19to give new insights that could help
  • 47:21identify people that might be at risk,
  • 47:23and use the data from that to target
  • 47:27towards specific strategies for prevention?
  • 47:30And I think this is where maybe one day
  • 47:33we might be able to add methylation
  • 47:36signatures or these epigenetic signatures
  • 47:38and teeth as part of our screening tools.
  • 47:41So, you know, imagine a world where somewhere
  • 47:43in the future a child loses a tooth,
  • 47:45whether it falls out,
  • 47:47it's lost for orthodontia
  • 47:49or wisdom tooth surgery.
  • 47:51And that tooth is taken to a healthcare
  • 47:54provider who sends it off then to
  • 47:57a specialized lab and that that
  • 47:59lab is then able to combine.
  • 48:01Data from other omic markers,
  • 48:03genetic markers and epigenetic markers
  • 48:05and survey data about early life
  • 48:08stress and other stressors and more
  • 48:10about the family context and pair that
  • 48:13with family history data and that you
  • 48:15could then use that to then identify
  • 48:17people who might be at highest risk and
  • 48:20connect them with preventative treatments.
  • 48:22I think there's a lot we have
  • 48:23to do on this space,
  • 48:24but I think it's really promising when
  • 48:26we think about what we know already,
  • 48:28we know that exercise is protective,
  • 48:30we know that social support.
  • 48:31Protective.
  • 48:31So can we get that data in the hands
  • 48:34of people and create interventions
  • 48:35that really leverage that so that
  • 48:38we can try to reduce risk?
  • 48:39And it might also be someday too
  • 48:41that we're able to shift these
  • 48:43methylation signatures too.
  • 48:44So we see something turning on
  • 48:45that might be deleterious,
  • 48:47maybe there's an intervention,
  • 48:49biological or not,
  • 48:51that can also produce those shifts.
  • 48:53And then in just my last slide,
  • 48:55I'll also say too that I think one thing
  • 48:58that we also want to be mindful of IS,
  • 49:01is.
  • 49:01This idea of of screening and I
  • 49:03think there's a lot of interest
  • 49:06in people doing screening.
  • 49:07I think we have to be careful around
  • 49:10screening though and and we published this,
  • 49:12this commentary a couple months
  • 49:15ago where we tried to just put a
  • 49:17little bit of context around this
  • 49:18area of screening because I think
  • 49:20we're at this tipping point where
  • 49:22there's the potential,
  • 49:23the real potential for screening for
  • 49:25childhood adversity to do potential
  • 49:27more harm than it does good.
  • 49:29So in this commentary.
  • 49:30We just described some recommendations
  • 49:32that folks should consider when
  • 49:34deploying these kinds of screenings.
  • 49:36And you know,
  • 49:37being very clear about what things
  • 49:39measure and and deploying screening
  • 49:41at the right time and making sure that
  • 49:43there are appropriate interventions to use.
  • 49:45And also just creating systems that
  • 49:47are nimble and adaptable knowing
  • 49:49that the science of adversity and
  • 49:51resilience is changing and therefore
  • 49:52we want to be able to leverage
  • 49:54that best evidence in support of
  • 49:56of future interventions.
  • 49:57So with that, just to thank everyone who's.
  • 50:01And part of my career journey
  • 50:05and my collaboration team.
  • 50:07Immigration is good because
  • 50:09science is a global enterprise.
  • 50:11And thank my outstanding lab
  • 50:14members and sources of funding and
  • 50:16I'm happy to take any questions
  • 50:18you might have. Thank you.
  • 50:26Wonderful. Thank you so much Doctor
  • 50:28Dunn and fantastic mix of topics there.
  • 50:31And I know that we've already
  • 50:32got some questions on the chat.
  • 50:33Are there any questions in
  • 50:33the room to get us started?
  • 50:42And so one question that we had in
  • 50:44the chat and actually from Doctor
  • 50:45Martin was can you talk about the
  • 50:47parallels between telomere length and
  • 50:49some of those markers that you're
  • 50:51observing in teeth? Oh, there is.
  • 50:58I thought you were maybe going to ask
  • 51:00about parallels between Umm Tillman
  • 51:02or length and epigenetic aging.
  • 51:05We don't. What are you? So. Umm.
  • 51:09So I think that this is an area
  • 51:12where I don't know that I've seen
  • 51:15a lot of very good comparisons.
  • 51:18There's the epigenetic clocks
  • 51:19that people tend to use.
  • 51:21There's now, there's now about
  • 51:231/2 a dozen dozen of them.
  • 51:26Some of them are correlating with each other,
  • 51:28some of them aren't.
  • 51:29It depends on what tissue type you get,
  • 51:31whether you have buckle cells
  • 51:33or saliva or blood.
  • 51:34So I think part of what we as a
  • 51:37field have to grapple with is trying
  • 51:39to build studies that allow us to
  • 51:42better understand similarities and
  • 51:43differences in these markers and then
  • 51:46also piece together that with the context of.
  • 51:48Development because as I shared,
  • 51:50a lot of these markers also vary,
  • 51:52you know,
  • 51:53over over the course of lifespan,
  • 51:55telomeres and teeth,
  • 51:56I haven't thought about it and we
  • 51:59haven't done anything on that just yet.
  • 52:01I think teeth are really understudied
  • 52:03and an area where there's a lot
  • 52:05of a lot that we can learn.
  • 52:07I don't know if there maybe there's
  • 52:10something in that circadian process
  • 52:13that can be indicative of of aging
  • 52:16related processes or something,
  • 52:18but I have we haven't gotten.
  • 52:20To that yet, but but it's a great question,
  • 52:22something to think about.
  • 52:30I just had a quick question about.
  • 52:33The you brought up measure difficulties with
  • 52:36measurement and bringing it back to age,
  • 52:38and age being kind of just
  • 52:40a proxy for development,
  • 52:41and then you're interested in
  • 52:43looking for sensitive periods.
  • 52:47I noticed in across the development
  • 52:50age was bent in and I think routinely
  • 52:54about two year increments and I'm
  • 52:57wondering if that is was informed
  • 52:59by by research or because that
  • 53:02really can either hinder or help.
  • 53:04Finding these sort of sensitive periods,
  • 53:08if something falls in between
  • 53:09one of those bins or so I just
  • 53:11wondering if you could speak to
  • 53:13how those are are gathered.
  • 53:15I love your question and doing
  • 53:17sensitive period work in relying on
  • 53:19age I think as your questions may be
  • 53:22saying is just an imperfect measure.
  • 53:24So we we tend to use the most.
  • 53:29The narrowest age we can and then we
  • 53:33afterwards Bennett into developmental stages.
  • 53:36So in other words we try to leverage.
  • 53:38So we have differences based on month of age.
  • 53:41So we have eight months and you know 17
  • 53:44months or whatever and then we'll group
  • 53:45after we do the analysis into just a
  • 53:48developmental stage and the thinking there
  • 53:50is that's just sort of how we think,
  • 53:52we think of you know based on school
  • 53:54age and non school age or preschool
  • 53:57period or what have you so.
  • 53:59It's really just meant to try to
  • 54:01help better translate that work.
  • 54:03I think really to understand
  • 54:05sensitive periods,
  • 54:05we need to have measures of plasticity.
  • 54:08And in order to have measures of plasticity,
  • 54:10we need to know what plasticity
  • 54:12actually is and how what we mean
  • 54:14by it and how we define it.
  • 54:16So I have a postdoc in my group
  • 54:18that's actually working on that.
  • 54:19That's just saying can we get all on the
  • 54:21same page about what we mean by plasticity.
  • 54:24So the plan is to write a paper on that
  • 54:26and then to follow that with a paper on,
  • 54:28OK, now that we're hopefully maybe.
  • 54:30More on the same page about plasticity.
  • 54:32Can we then start to think
  • 54:35about markers of plasticity?
  • 54:36Because we're a lot of us are
  • 54:38really interested in plasticity.
  • 54:39But plasticity means something really
  • 54:41different to a neuroscientist who
  • 54:43thinks about it at a synaptic level and
  • 54:46someone who's thinking about it in the
  • 54:48context of like stroke recovery for example,
  • 54:51and and rehabilitation and
  • 54:52those kinds of outcomes.
  • 54:54So I think this is another,
  • 54:56I think this is a Holy Grail
  • 54:58for our field is to really,
  • 54:59I think if we nail the sensitive.
  • 55:01Period question and we did that through
  • 55:03plasticity and had a good markers of that.
  • 55:05I think that would be pretty,
  • 55:06pretty amazing.
  • 55:07Thank you.
  • 55:12Aye, thank you for such an amazing talk,
  • 55:16so I'm very curious about.
  • 55:19The effects that you observe on
  • 55:22the protective effects of the DNA
  • 55:25methylation changes that specific loci.
  • 55:28Could you elaborate a little bit
  • 55:30more how they were defined and also?
  • 55:32Will that be dependent depending on?
  • 55:36That it occurred during the sensitive
  • 55:39periods and that you like look at that
  • 55:42during that specific time whereas.
  • 55:44You know, compared to auto hold for example.
  • 55:46How would that compare? Um, like?
  • 55:50I will argue that maybe adulthood we
  • 55:53will observe more deleterious effects.
  • 55:56But you know, I wonder about.
  • 55:59Your thoughts on that and I
  • 56:00have a second question,
  • 56:01but if you want to answer that,
  • 56:03thank you for just asking one at a time.
  • 56:05That's that's great.
  • 56:07I think so.
  • 56:08I think there's a lot to still unpack
  • 56:10in this mediation work and there's not
  • 56:12been from what we've seen any other
  • 56:14work that's been done in this space.
  • 56:17And it took a lot for us to
  • 56:19just figure out the methods,
  • 56:20you know,
  • 56:21because you're bringing these
  • 56:22methods that have been developed
  • 56:24typically for what we call like a
  • 56:26small data setting and then you're
  • 56:27applying it to data where you have,
  • 56:29you know, as you know,
  • 56:31500,000 different associations that you can,
  • 56:34you know, study.
  • 56:34So a lot of the time we spent,
  • 56:37you know,
  • 56:37was was built in on that and then we
  • 56:40carried forward our sensitive period
  • 56:42work to try to bring in more of this
  • 56:45information about these different
  • 56:46life course models to try to.
  • 56:48Understand, you know these effects.
  • 56:51I don't.
  • 56:51I was not expecting to see such variation.
  • 56:55I think the next set of questions
  • 56:57that will be really key is, you know,
  • 56:59we just looked at depression at one
  • 57:01point in time in late adolescence.
  • 57:03We can look at later markers of
  • 57:05depression to see if this persists.
  • 57:08And I think, you know,
  • 57:10I come from the camp of let's see it
  • 57:12once and if we see something interesting,
  • 57:14let's try to see it again in
  • 57:16another data set and try to.
  • 57:18Replicate it.
  • 57:19And then that's where let's if we do that,
  • 57:22then let's start digging in on biology.
  • 57:24Let's get into cell culture models,
  • 57:26let's get into animal models.
  • 57:28Let's, you know,
  • 57:29really try to probe this to see if this is,
  • 57:32you know, real and what might be
  • 57:35some of the the consequences.
  • 57:37Thank you.
  • 57:38And then make my second question is sort
  • 57:40of a follow-up of their previous question,
  • 57:43how these sensitive fears
  • 57:44are defined in terms of?
  • 57:48The age or and following up on on
  • 57:53the definition of of that in terms of
  • 57:56like have you considered biological
  • 57:58age like not only tell me your land
  • 58:01but also epigenetic aging and I
  • 58:04always wonder what does that mean
  • 58:06during childhood because we often see
  • 58:09accelerated at beginning of aging in
  • 58:11adults being associated with trauma
  • 58:13but that what does that really mean.
  • 58:17In childhood and if these could be?
  • 58:20A marker for biological aging to
  • 58:23define better sensitive periods.
  • 58:25Yeah, that's a great question.
  • 58:27So believe it or not,
  • 58:28I think it's counterintuitive in a way
  • 58:30for us to think about kids as aging,
  • 58:32but they are.
  • 58:33And we, we did a study actually in
  • 58:36alspach where we showed that some
  • 58:38early life markers of stress were
  • 58:41associated with accelerated aging at
  • 58:43age 7 by as much as seven months.
  • 58:45So a 7 year old could look,
  • 58:48you know, Cellularly older than us.
  • 58:507 year old by they would look 7 point you
  • 58:53know seven years with seven months added on.
  • 58:56So I think for us we wanted to
  • 59:00have in order to do the sensitive
  • 59:02period work you need to either have.
  • 59:05You ideally have repeated measures so
  • 59:08that you can get these markers of timing,
  • 59:12not that you're relying on
  • 59:14retrospective reports.
  • 59:15So we there's not a lot of data sets
  • 59:17that have that repeated methylation data
  • 59:19where you could derive those repeated scores.
  • 59:22But we just got a grant last year where
  • 59:25we're doing work in a South African
  • 59:27cohort and where we're going to have,
  • 59:29we're going to be driving epigenetic
  • 59:32signatures at 1/3 and five and I think
  • 59:34that would be a. Great opportunity.
  • 59:36I'm glad you said this.
  • 59:38I think this is something we should
  • 59:40look into there and see if if how
  • 59:42similar or different it is relative
  • 59:43to findings you get for age.
  • 59:46So I know we're almost at time,
  • 59:48but I didn't realize Doctor Lombroso
  • 59:49that your hand was up actually was
  • 59:51fading into the background there.
  • 59:52So Paul, please, please ask your question.
  • 59:56So can you hear me?
  • 59:58Yes. Yes. OK great.
  • 59:59I I that was a fantastic talk and
  • 01:00:02then specifically because it was
  • 01:00:05introducing such a for me anyway a
  • 01:00:07novel area couple of questions that
  • 01:00:10I try to get my head around this.
  • 01:00:13If a child has early onset
  • 01:00:15depression or childhood psychosis
  • 01:00:20or early childhood onset diabetes,
  • 01:00:24are you saying that that there will be a
  • 01:00:26marker for for in in the teeth of this event.
  • 01:00:30And are the epigenetic.
  • 01:00:33Findings,
  • 01:00:35I would imagine they're all different
  • 01:00:38in these three very distinct disorders.
  • 01:00:41Just to help me understand,
  • 01:00:42you probably already mentioned this, but
  • 01:00:45no, I think it's a good,
  • 01:00:47I think it's a good question.
  • 01:00:48Pardon me, I don't know.
  • 01:00:49I think I'm going to look
  • 01:00:51here even though you're here.
  • 01:00:53Trying to answer you, but Umm,
  • 01:00:56so in terms of what we've seen so
  • 01:00:58far with teeth and psychopathology.
  • 01:01:01So we are correlating marker that's
  • 01:01:04derived from micro CT imaging about
  • 01:01:06how thick the enamel basically marker
  • 01:01:09of enamel volume is and seeing that
  • 01:01:12kids who have thinner volume have
  • 01:01:15higher psychopathology symptoms.
  • 01:01:16I think that's just correlational.
  • 01:01:19Who knows whether this is actually causal.
  • 01:01:22I think we need more studies to try to.
  • 01:01:24Impact that.
  • 01:01:25I think teeth might be a marker.
  • 01:01:28So I think of teeth as the marker
  • 01:01:30of early life stress and then
  • 01:01:33potentially those biological
  • 01:01:34markers of early life stress can
  • 01:01:37be informative for mental health.
  • 01:01:39I don't know that teeth necessarily
  • 01:01:42independent of early life stress would
  • 01:01:44be informative for mental health.
  • 01:01:47However,
  • 01:01:47I think there is maybe a kind
  • 01:01:50of tooth brain access where
  • 01:01:52teeth might be informative.
  • 01:01:54For characterizing and understanding
  • 01:01:57processes of brain development that might
  • 01:02:00be harder to interrogate otherwise.
  • 01:02:03So that's sort of my thought on on that.
  • 01:02:06And then whether the epigenetic
  • 01:02:09signatures are similar across disorders,
  • 01:02:11we've not really looked at that,
  • 01:02:15but I think it's something that
  • 01:02:17that you know the work that has
  • 01:02:19been done is more so where you
  • 01:02:21group kids into internalizing
  • 01:02:23versus externalizing symptoms.
  • 01:02:25Part of the challenge is that you
  • 01:02:28really just need incredibly large
  • 01:02:30sample sizes in order to find potential
  • 01:02:33signal when you bring epigenetic work
  • 01:02:36to psychiatric disorders on the order of,
  • 01:02:39you know, thousands, 10s of thousands.
  • 01:02:42To give you context,
  • 01:02:44you may or may not know about
  • 01:02:47genetic association studies.
  • 01:02:48Those are starting to see results.
  • 01:02:51After 500,000, you know,
  • 01:02:53a million participants,
  • 01:02:54so.
  • 01:02:55I think we're seeing more with epigenetic
  • 01:02:57work starting to emerge with less than that,
  • 01:03:00but it's still the scale is very,
  • 01:03:02very large because these effects
  • 01:03:04are are pretty small.
  • 01:03:09Great. Well, thank you all for
  • 01:03:10this rich discussion and please
  • 01:03:12join me again in thanking Dr Dunn
  • 01:03:13for a wonderful presentation.