Skip to Main Content

Yale Psychiatry Grand Rounds - March 5, 2021

March 05, 2021

"A Novel Computational Framework for the Role of Dopamine in Learning and Memory"

Erin S. Calipari, PhD, Assistant Professor of Pharmacology, Vanderbilt School of Medicine

ID
6251

Transcript

  • 00:00Because my introduction is much less
  • 00:02important than Doctor Calipari's talk,
  • 00:04which will be eagerly awaiting.
  • 00:07So it's it's my absolute pleasure
  • 00:10to introduce Doctor Erin Calipari
  • 00:13who's today's grand round speaker.
  • 00:16She is someone who has really been
  • 00:19a pioneer and incredibly prolific
  • 00:22scientists already in the area of
  • 00:25addiction and using a rodent models to
  • 00:30understand the neurobiological basis
  • 00:32of important constructs underlying
  • 00:35addiction that are relevant to.
  • 00:38Human subjects she did her
  • 00:40undergraduate degree at the University
  • 00:42of Massachusetts in Amherst,
  • 00:44where she has got her BS in both psychology
  • 00:47and biology and already began working
  • 00:50with Doctor Gerald Meyer using rodent models.
  • 00:53And then she did her graduate work at Wake
  • 00:57Forest School of Medicine with Sarah Jones,
  • 01:00where she began to.
  • 01:03It really immerse herself and neurochemistry,
  • 01:06an other aspects of in vivo manipulations
  • 01:09and measurements related to behavior.
  • 01:12She then went on as a postdoctoral fellow
  • 01:15and then a instructor at the Mount Sinai's
  • 01:19Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai,
  • 01:23where she worked with Eric Nestler.
  • 01:26And there she began to develop
  • 01:28a number of lines of research,
  • 01:31including examination of.
  • 01:33Particularly molecular consequences
  • 01:35of drug addiction,
  • 01:37and particularly intracellular
  • 01:39signaling as a consequence of drug
  • 01:43addiction that may maintain long term
  • 01:46structural and behavioral adaptations
  • 01:49to drugs of abuse and also was really
  • 01:52key in a number of important papers
  • 01:55on sex specific effects of addictive,
  • 01:58addictive substances like cocaine and
  • 02:01those experiments are particularly
  • 02:04notable because they.
  • 02:05Involved many, many levels of evaluation,
  • 02:07not only in rodents but also in
  • 02:10human subjects.
  • 02:11She has won a number of awards already.
  • 02:14I want to note a few notable ones.
  • 02:17In particular she is an awardee of the DP.
  • 02:21One Avenir Award in genetics,
  • 02:23and epigenetics from the National
  • 02:25Institute on Drug Abuse,
  • 02:27and that's someone who the director
  • 02:29of Naida pulls out as having research
  • 02:32that is extremely innovative and
  • 02:34at the edge of the.
  • 02:36Molecular basis of addiction.
  • 02:38She's an associate member of the AC NP.
  • 02:41Oh I should have noted that she's
  • 02:43also an assistant professor in
  • 02:45the department's particularly in
  • 02:46the Department of Pharmacology
  • 02:48at Vanderbilt University,
  • 02:50but also has appointments in
  • 02:52the departments of Psychiatry
  • 02:53and Behavioral Sciences.
  • 02:55Of course relevant to our Department
  • 02:57and the Department of Molecular
  • 02:59Physiology and Biophysics.
  • 03:00So again, even with her appointments,
  • 03:03you can see how her work spans areas of
  • 03:06investigation from the very molecular
  • 03:08and cellular through the pharmacological too.
  • 03:11The area of drug addiction
  • 03:13relevant to psychiatric illness.
  • 03:15So back to her awards.
  • 03:17Just because they're so notable she is,
  • 03:20has been awarded a Whitehall Foundation
  • 03:23research grant and are said Young
  • 03:25Investigator Award and a K99 to
  • 03:28R00 pathway to Independence Award,
  • 03:30and even back in her days
  • 03:33as a graduate student.
  • 03:34The Knighted Director's Award,
  • 03:36which acknowledged her innovative
  • 03:38work from the beginning,
  • 03:40an addiction.
  • 03:41She's an editorial boards relevant
  • 03:43to this Department.
  • 03:44Both the editorial board of
  • 03:46Neuropsychopharmacology and
  • 03:46the Journal of Neuroscience,
  • 03:48and she's been incredibly
  • 03:49prolific at every stage of her
  • 03:52career, showing how which I love that
  • 03:54not only does she have great ideas,
  • 03:57but she carries them through all the way
  • 03:59to really publishing beautiful papers,
  • 04:02and it's been exciting to watch
  • 04:04the evolution of the work.
  • 04:06Do you want to say something that
  • 04:08is not exactly science related,
  • 04:10as she was recounting during?
  • 04:12The first discussion she does have one area
  • 04:15that she's interested in outside of science,
  • 04:17or that she's able to devote time
  • 04:19to as an assistant professor,
  • 04:21and that is she is a powerlifter and
  • 04:24I have seen a video of her doing.
  • 04:27Plus, which I'm very, very envious of,
  • 04:29but I do want to highlight that her most
  • 04:32recent achievement in powerlifting is
  • 04:34that she has just deadlifted 300 pounds.
  • 04:36Now if that isn't an achievement,
  • 04:38I don't know what is.
  • 04:39So thank you very much for being
  • 04:42with us here.
  • 04:42Erin Dr.
  • 04:43Calipari,
  • 04:43it is a pleasure to welcome you
  • 04:45to the Department and I'm looking
  • 04:47forward to hearing we're all looking
  • 04:49forward to hearing your talk.
  • 04:51Thank you so
  • 04:52much. That was so nice.
  • 04:53And yeah, I know I tried it.
  • 04:55You know the pandemic has
  • 04:57made me start to try to find.
  • 04:59Things to occupy my time that aren't
  • 05:01just sitting at home and working and
  • 05:03so that's that's what I've been doing.
  • 05:06But I'm I'm actually really excited
  • 05:07to present for a number of reasons.
  • 05:09One, because I get to see some people
  • 05:12outside of my immediate family in my lab.
  • 05:14But to this, this work is some kind
  • 05:17of work that's been developing in
  • 05:19my lab of the last couple of years.
  • 05:22That focuses on kind of what
  • 05:24dopamine is doing,
  • 05:25and so I think you know for somebody who
  • 05:28studies addiction and psychiatric disease.
  • 05:31The reason this is so important
  • 05:33is because domains at the core
  • 05:35of a lot of these disorders,
  • 05:37and you know specifically addiction
  • 05:39where you see deficits in dopamine,
  • 05:41and I think it's really important
  • 05:43to understand for people what
  • 05:45those deficits mean.
  • 05:46You know if dopamine is encoding
  • 05:48reward and reduction in dopamine
  • 05:50may mean something very different.
  • 05:52Then if it don't mean is encoding
  • 05:54some other aspect of learned behavior,
  • 05:56which I'm probably going to show you today.
  • 06:00So you know,
  • 06:00I'll kind of show some stuff in the weeds,
  • 06:03but also kind of tide into big
  • 06:05picture and so please like stop
  • 06:07me if things aren't clear or you
  • 06:10have thoughts or comments as we go.
  • 06:12OK,
  • 06:13so the focus of my lab is really
  • 06:15understanding if you can see my slides right.
  • 06:18OK,
  • 06:18good is understanding how neural
  • 06:20experience or how neural circuits
  • 06:22integrate experiences to drive
  • 06:23behavior and so you know in life
  • 06:26or in animals you know we have
  • 06:28experiences that have negative outcomes.
  • 06:30Things that have positive outcomes
  • 06:32and what happens is that these
  • 06:34experiences change the way our brain
  • 06:36response to stimuli in the future to
  • 06:38increase the probability of behaviors
  • 06:40that result in good outcomes.
  • 06:41And decrease the probability of
  • 06:43behaviors that result in negative outcomes.
  • 06:46And the reason I'm so interested
  • 06:49in this is because this is kind of,
  • 06:52you know,
  • 06:53the fundamental core of how we
  • 06:55make decisions.
  • 06:56But it's also dysregulated,
  • 06:57and almost every psychiatric disease state,
  • 07:00and so you know for somebody
  • 07:02who studies addiction,
  • 07:04you know drug associated stimuli,
  • 07:06or overvalued relative to
  • 07:07negative consequences.
  • 07:08An alternative reinforcers depression.
  • 07:10You have reduced motivation.
  • 07:11Valuation of rewards.
  • 07:12Reward learning or things like
  • 07:14anxiety and stress disorders where
  • 07:16these negative outcomes may be
  • 07:18overgeneralized to neutral cues and contexts,
  • 07:20which is,
  • 07:21you know things like PTSD,
  • 07:23and so you know this kind of
  • 07:25fundamental process by which we
  • 07:27attribute value to things that our
  • 07:29environment is really a core of
  • 07:31how we should be thinking about
  • 07:33treating people with disorders
  • 07:35where this is dysregulated.
  • 07:37So you know, I'm, you know,
  • 07:39going way back to the simple,
  • 07:41you know half of my lab studies.
  • 07:43You know how to drug change the brain,
  • 07:45but the other half of my
  • 07:47labs that he's just
  • 07:48kind of. How do these same systems
  • 07:50work in a normal situation?
  • 07:51And so the first thing is kind of,
  • 07:54you know, how do we learn to
  • 07:56make these adaptive decisions?
  • 07:57And so we use things in our
  • 07:59Virat environment to do this.
  • 08:01So maybe there's contextual cues
  • 08:02that help you figure out when things
  • 08:05are dangerous and when they aren't,
  • 08:06and so if you have something like the
  • 08:09sound of a helicopter in a hometown
  • 08:11may be very different than the cell of
  • 08:14sound of a helicopter in a war zone.
  • 08:16We have things like discrete cues,
  • 08:18which would be the sound of a
  • 08:19helicopter itself with drug addiction.
  • 08:21It's these cues that are associated
  • 08:23with the drug taking experience,
  • 08:25and so we learn to associate those.
  • 08:27And then there's also, you know.
  • 08:29What we do in response to these stimuli,
  • 08:31and so you know my background is
  • 08:33really focused on reinforcement
  • 08:34learning and how these stimuli in
  • 08:36the environment drive animals to
  • 08:37make decisions in different context.
  • 08:39So what are they going to do?
  • 08:41Are they going to, you know,
  • 08:43are they reinforce?
  • 08:43Are they going to do something or
  • 08:45they going to avoid something?
  • 08:47And how we can understand the neural
  • 08:50circuitry that underlies this
  • 08:51decision to kind of seek out or avoid
  • 08:54different things in the environment?
  • 08:56And so you know,
  • 08:57I think this is a really important thing,
  • 08:59right?
  • 09:00Is that we learn to make predictions and
  • 09:02so our actions have some sort of outcome.
  • 09:05It changes the state of our
  • 09:07environment and basically what we
  • 09:09do is we learn to do something and
  • 09:11so this is kind of guiding these.
  • 09:13These associations aren't just there,
  • 09:14they're guiding how we navigate
  • 09:16an environment. And so.
  • 09:18How do we do this?
  • 09:21Well,
  • 09:21we need to encode the value or
  • 09:24salience salience is kind of like how
  • 09:26attention grabbing something is of
  • 09:28unexpected outcomes and so you get ice cream.
  • 09:31That's great.
  • 09:32It's awesome.
  • 09:33We need to know whether it's good
  • 09:35or bad and how good or bad it is.
  • 09:38How attention, yes, are you advancing slides?
  • 09:40Yes, sorry,
  • 09:41this has happened to me before and
  • 09:44I have no idea why it does this.
  • 09:47Let me try this again.
  • 09:49It's OK if you guys followed them.
  • 09:51Yeah, I thought the intro was all very clear,
  • 09:54but I thought maybe you were dancing.
  • 09:56We didn't know. Yeah, this is
  • 09:57this is happened to me before. I have no.
  • 10:01Idea when or why this does this?
  • 10:04Let me try this one more time.
  • 10:06So where I made it 'cause it's on,
  • 10:09zoom on the shorter side because
  • 10:11I don't think people love watching
  • 10:13zoom for two hours. So OK.
  • 10:15So now if I move the slides they move OK.
  • 10:19Well, there were pictures you guys
  • 10:21have experience with all of this stuff,
  • 10:23so that's fine.
  • 10:24So now we're into the important
  • 10:25bit so it's good you saw this. OK,
  • 10:28so nothing like this is like pandemic level.
  • 10:30Like everything,
  • 10:30something has to go wrong every time.
  • 10:32Otherwise, like you know, it's not.
  • 10:34It's not real, so OK,
  • 10:35so you have to encode some information.
  • 10:37We need to know whether it's good
  • 10:39or bad and how intense it is.
  • 10:41Is this something we should
  • 10:43really pay attention to?
  • 10:44Or is this something that's not as important?
  • 10:46We need to make predictions
  • 10:48about when that's going to occur.
  • 10:50And so you know you have an ice cream truck.
  • 10:52You predict whether the ice
  • 10:54cream will be there or not.
  • 10:55But not only do we need to make predictions,
  • 10:58we need to be able to update
  • 11:00these when they change.
  • 11:01So when something no longer is associated,
  • 11:03we need to be able to adapt.
  • 11:05If the update this so that we can change
  • 11:07our behavior when the environment is
  • 11:09not the same as we learned previously,
  • 11:11and so this is a really, really critical
  • 11:14aspect of learning and behavior.
  • 11:15So I'm going to kind of go.
  • 11:17There's going to be some computation in here,
  • 11:19but what I'll tell you is most of
  • 11:21it is is more of a framework for
  • 11:23how people think about how these
  • 11:25these computations are being done,
  • 11:27and if you don't care about the computation,
  • 11:29which I've met, many people who say,
  • 11:31oh,
  • 11:31whatever we've used these to
  • 11:32design experiments,
  • 11:33and so it's not like you
  • 11:34need to know the math.
  • 11:36It's more of a kind of framework
  • 11:38for how we designed experiments.
  • 11:40So this kind of prediction
  • 11:42based learning was formalized.
  • 11:43You know,
  • 11:44originally by Rescorla Wagner in 1972,
  • 11:46and there's been a bunch of kind
  • 11:48of adaptations of this and allow
  • 11:51the model to do other things.
  • 11:53But really,
  • 11:53what this is is it's a mathematical
  • 11:56model that allows us to kind
  • 11:58of formalize how animals learn,
  • 12:00and So what happens in this model
  • 12:02is that if you have something
  • 12:04like an unexpected outcome,
  • 12:06that is an error in prediction,
  • 12:08you predicted nothing, something was there.
  • 12:10You made an error and what happens
  • 12:13overtime is your prediction gets
  • 12:15better and then there's less error.
  • 12:17So essentially what happens is the
  • 12:20associative strength or how well
  • 12:22you how well something predicts
  • 12:24something goes up and the error in
  • 12:26that prediction goes down and so
  • 12:28basically the way the model works is
  • 12:31that as you learn the prediction of
  • 12:34that Q and the outcome increases.
  • 12:36But any errors you make go down
  • 12:39and so essentially
  • 12:40you get this. Increase in the
  • 12:42predictive response and a decrease in
  • 12:44the error or the mistakes from that,
  • 12:46and so this is kind of how animals learn.
  • 12:49It can map learning rates in
  • 12:51a lot of different contexts.
  • 12:53You know learning about
  • 12:54Accuen award extinction.
  • 12:55All of these and so people have really
  • 12:58been searching for what is a circuit
  • 13:01in the brain that does this math.
  • 13:03And that's been a kind of really big
  • 13:06focus of specifically the dopamine field.
  • 13:09And other fields too.
  • 13:10I think a lot of people are
  • 13:11starting to see that these kinds of
  • 13:13computations are done in a variety
  • 13:15of circuits across the brain.
  • 13:17So the dopamine system is important
  • 13:19for any a lot of reasons.
  • 13:21These neurons you know that we focus on
  • 13:24originate in the ventral tegmental area,
  • 13:27so we're focusing on more reward
  • 13:29associated circuits rather than things
  • 13:31that are associated with motor.
  • 13:33So we're looking in for this
  • 13:35particular project.
  • 13:36The nucleus accumbens,
  • 13:37the core region,
  • 13:38and So what these dopamine neurons
  • 13:41are really important for survival.
  • 13:43Lesioning them present prevents
  • 13:44this kind of associative learning.
  • 13:46An also reinforcement learning.
  • 13:49And the thing people been kind of really
  • 13:52focus on is that this domain neurons
  • 13:54respond in a fashion that mimics this.
  • 13:57This mathematical model I just showed you,
  • 13:59and so essentially this kind of
  • 14:01originated and within a lots of other
  • 14:04people have shown these patterns.
  • 14:05So this originative with Wolfram Schultz
  • 14:07and I'm just showing the the original.
  • 14:10But people within the domain
  • 14:12field have done this with all
  • 14:14kinds of other approaches as well.
  • 14:16But essentially what they see
  • 14:18is this kind of same math.
  • 14:20We went hoping that this didn't
  • 14:23just move because OK,
  • 14:24the slides are still advancing, right?
  • 14:27Yeah, OK, OK.
  • 14:28So essentially what happens is
  • 14:29you have an unexpected reward.
  • 14:32Dopamine firing goes up.
  • 14:34You predict that reward dopamine firing
  • 14:37now goes up to the queue that predicts it,
  • 14:40but not the reward,
  • 14:41because the prediction of that
  • 14:43reward is basically perfect.
  • 14:44And now if the reward is omitted,
  • 14:47what happens is the dopamine
  • 14:49response to the queue goes up,
  • 14:51but there is now a decrease in that
  • 14:54domain response when it's omitted,
  • 14:56signaling the negative error that,
  • 14:58uh, from that prediction.
  • 14:59And so this is they.
  • 15:02You know, originally this,
  • 15:03this first paper.
  • 15:04They said, wow,
  • 15:05that looks a lot like reward prediction,
  • 15:07error learning,
  • 15:08and so this is kind of formed the
  • 15:10basis of the domain field dopamine
  • 15:13does reward prediction learning.
  • 15:15So here's the kind of maybe issue with that.
  • 15:19If you do stress work,
  • 15:21anything else you know the domain
  • 15:23does not only respond to rewards
  • 15:26and reward predictions,
  • 15:28it's involved in things like punishment,
  • 15:30which is an aversive learning parameter.
  • 15:33Motivation, fear, safety transitions,
  • 15:35aversive learning.
  • 15:37All kinds of there's been a
  • 15:38lot of Association,
  • 15:39aversive learning and these
  • 15:40fields have been kind of.
  • 15:42It was a separate,
  • 15:43but there's kind of the reward.
  • 15:44Prediction people and then the
  • 15:46people who studied anxiety,
  • 15:47depression looking at Microdialysis,
  • 15:48showing that dopamine does
  • 15:49go up to aversive stimuli,
  • 15:50and so these kind of have been a
  • 15:52little bit at odds with each other.
  • 15:55But they kind of are,
  • 15:56you know, different fields,
  • 15:57so people haven't really looked
  • 15:59at them in the same context.
  • 16:01And so essentially,
  • 16:02I think some of the disconnect also
  • 16:04comes from this kind of fundamental
  • 16:06process of about domain neurons.
  • 16:08That's actually my favorite
  • 16:09part of GOP neurons.
  • 16:11Many studies have looked
  • 16:12at VTA cell body firing.
  • 16:14They use electrophysiology.
  • 16:15They say we don't need it goes up.
  • 16:18It goes down.
  • 16:19And there's this inference that
  • 16:21that means dopamine release.
  • 16:23That is,
  • 16:23projection targets is going to be the
  • 16:26same as what the firing looks like.
  • 16:29But dopamine terminals are so cool
  • 16:31because they're regulated at the
  • 16:33terminal level by **** synaptic.
  • 16:34So things that are regulated by Domi
  • 16:37itself but also header, synaptic,
  • 16:39regulators things like glutamate,
  • 16:40GABA.
  • 16:41A favorite of this Department,
  • 16:43acetylcholine and so these.
  • 16:44These things actually can elicit
  • 16:46dopamine release from the terminals,
  • 16:47independent of cymatic firing.
  • 16:49And so if you want to understand
  • 16:51what dopamine release another
  • 16:52projection target is doing,
  • 16:54you need to actually record
  • 16:56dopamine and the ultimate wrists.
  • 16:57You have a few of those as well,
  • 17:00have been doing this for a really long time,
  • 17:03but there's a lot of kind of
  • 17:06limitations to voltammetry and we'll
  • 17:08kind of talk about those as we go,
  • 17:10but our goal was really too.
  • 17:13Record dopamine,
  • 17:13but be able to dissociate
  • 17:15these kind of things.
  • 17:16People have seen in the aversive
  • 17:18field with the things people
  • 17:20have seen in the reward fields.
  • 17:22Why is dopamine look like it's doing
  • 17:24both of these at the same time?
  • 17:27So my background isn't reinforcement
  • 17:29learning and what we did is we we
  • 17:31like to develop behavioral tasks to
  • 17:33parse the things that we're interested.
  • 17:35So we developed this task,
  • 17:37which is not really the task
  • 17:38itself is an innovative behavioral
  • 17:40pharmacology and reinforcement learning.
  • 17:42People have been doing this for years.
  • 17:45Essentially what we do is we
  • 17:46have a queue that comes on.
  • 17:48In one phase,
  • 17:49that tells animals if they know
  • 17:50spoke during this Q and in
  • 17:52this example is white noise.
  • 17:54But we counterbalance and change
  • 17:55that they will get sucrose.
  • 17:57This is like normal positive reinforcement.
  • 17:59You know you treat your teacher dog,
  • 18:01that's it. They get a reward.
  • 18:03What we taught the animals in
  • 18:05the other face is that a separate
  • 18:07queue comes on and they have
  • 18:09the same behavioral response.
  • 18:10They know spoke,
  • 18:11but they know spoke to prevent a
  • 18:13series of shocks from being delivered,
  • 18:15so it's called negative reinforcement.
  • 18:18The reason that using these is so cool
  • 18:20is because they have the exact same action.
  • 18:23So if dopamine just encodes
  • 18:24the motivated response,
  • 18:25these will look the same.
  • 18:28They have the same outcome value.
  • 18:30The outcome is positive.
  • 18:31Avoiding something negative is positive.
  • 18:32Getting something positive is positive,
  • 18:34but there's different stimuli maintaining
  • 18:36these behavioral events and so
  • 18:38essentially what we've associated here is
  • 18:40the kind of motivated action from this
  • 18:42stimulus value in the outcome value,
  • 18:43and the question is in this sounds
  • 18:46more complicated than it is,
  • 18:47and I'm going to tell you the story
  • 18:50is that we're going to be able to see
  • 18:52if dopamine responds to just rewards
  • 18:55if it's just involved in motivation,
  • 18:57or if it's doing something maybe slightly.
  • 19:00It's a more complicated,
  • 19:01but it's actually simpler.
  • 19:03We need a way to record dopamine
  • 19:06during this task.
  • 19:07Aversive foot shocks are electrical signals.
  • 19:09All of the previous domain recording
  • 19:11techniques on fast time scales
  • 19:13were used on electrical systems,
  • 19:14and so the problem with this is all
  • 19:17the voltammetry techniques people
  • 19:18use before you couldn't record
  • 19:20responses to foot shocks,
  • 19:22and So what we've been using is
  • 19:25a fluorescent dopamine sensor.
  • 19:26This one is called delight.
  • 19:28It was developed at UC Davis by
  • 19:30Lindsay Angela and what this is is
  • 19:33it's a modified D1 dopamine receptor
  • 19:35that when it binds to dopamine it.
  • 19:38Laura says.
  • 19:38And so this fluorescent sensor is
  • 19:40really great because we can inject
  • 19:42it in with a virus into the brain.
  • 19:44We build fiber, photometry, systems.
  • 19:46I know a lot of people are using these,
  • 19:49but what it allows us to do is in awake
  • 19:51and behaving animals during these
  • 19:53discrete aspects of this behavioral task,
  • 19:55is record fluctuations in joking that
  • 19:57happened through this kind of fluorescent
  • 19:59response that isn't interfering?
  • 20:00Electrical signals and also the
  • 20:02great thing about these optical
  • 20:03sensors is they have really great
  • 20:05signal to noise and so you can get
  • 20:07single trial responses which with
  • 20:08a lot of voltammetry in the past,
  • 20:10which is my backgrounds you didn't get,
  • 20:12you had to average responses and
  • 20:14what I'll show you is a lot of what
  • 20:16we see is these really rapid changes
  • 20:18in dopamine that are happening on
  • 20:19the trial by trial basis that we
  • 20:21think are really critical for
  • 20:23this behavioral response.
  • 20:26So we started with kind
  • 20:27of what everyone is done.
  • 20:29Before this you know it's always good
  • 20:30when you start with like new tools to
  • 20:33make sure you see what everybody else's.
  • 20:35And So what we did is we recorded domain
  • 20:37responses during the pre training session.
  • 20:39The first time the animals had been
  • 20:41in these operating chambers and post
  • 20:42training after the animals had learned
  • 20:44and so not surprising animals learn to
  • 20:46know spoke during a queue for sucrose we
  • 20:48can change the length of the queue to
  • 20:50make the task more or less difficult.
  • 20:52We kind of did this so we had some
  • 20:55dynamic range of whether they did the.
  • 20:57Miss trials or not,
  • 20:58and so we did some machine learning.
  • 20:59I won't show you then.
  • 21:01This was actually a really
  • 21:02great tool for that,
  • 21:03but what we do is we see kind of the
  • 21:05same thing everyone else is seen.
  • 21:07Early on,
  • 21:08when the animals go into the sucrose port,
  • 21:11so red in here this is more
  • 21:13domain response over trials.
  • 21:14When they go into the Super sport,
  • 21:16you get this robust domain
  • 21:18response to the sucrose.
  • 21:20Overtraining this signal
  • 21:21moves back to the cube.
  • 21:23That's very predictive.
  • 21:23So now you get this really robust
  • 21:26domain response to the queue,
  • 21:27but not as much of a domain
  • 21:30response to the sucrose.
  • 21:32Great, it looks just like that equation.
  • 21:34I showed you.
  • 21:35Dopamine goes up to the Q goes
  • 21:37down to the error signal.
  • 21:40All is well in the reward domain
  • 21:43does reward based learning field.
  • 21:45But then we moved on to this
  • 21:47other behavioral task.
  • 21:48So again,
  • 21:49the animals can know spoke
  • 21:50during this Q and they do it to
  • 21:53avoid a series of foot shocks.
  • 21:54So if they don't press during
  • 21:56the Q they get shocked.
  • 21:58There's a series of shocks they
  • 21:59can press anytime during this
  • 22:01series to terminate the shocks,
  • 22:02and so we did the same thing
  • 22:04we recorded early in Leanne.
  • 22:06Learning animals actually learn.
  • 22:07When I started my lab a bunch of behavior.
  • 22:10People told me that animals
  • 22:11will never do this.
  • 22:12Mice do this really great.
  • 22:14They'll learn really rapidly
  • 22:15to press on the nose,
  • 22:17poke that active know spoke
  • 22:18to avoid the shocks.
  • 22:19And they actually at the end
  • 22:21of these trials are doing this,
  • 22:23that they are not getting shots at all,
  • 22:25and so they learn this very fast.
  • 22:27And it's actually really robust task
  • 22:29for generating motivated behavior.
  • 22:30Um?
  • 22:31OK,
  • 22:31so the first thing we saw which goes
  • 22:34in the face of dopamine encoding
  • 22:36rewards is that you get this really
  • 22:39robust positive response to a foot shock.
  • 22:43So dopamine goes up when
  • 22:45aversive stimuli are encountered.
  • 22:47Um,
  • 22:48other people have seen this,
  • 22:49but what's really interesting is I'll
  • 22:51remind you of what that model looked like.
  • 22:53If dopamine is doing reward based learning.
  • 22:56We didn't get this robust
  • 22:58response to the Q like we did with
  • 23:02sucrose overlearning.
  • 23:03We did get a decrease in the response.
  • 23:05We have the safety queue
  • 23:07that came on when the
  • 23:08animals avoided the negative consequences.
  • 23:10So at the end of the trial.
  • 23:13It did go down over learning like
  • 23:15you'd expect of an error signal,
  • 23:17but here's the problem.
  • 23:18The safety queue domain response
  • 23:19was the biggest on the first
  • 23:21trial they ever encountered it
  • 23:22before they could know its value,
  • 23:24and so we were kind of a little
  • 23:26bit hesitant about that.
  • 23:27But we said OK, but maybe this looks kind of.
  • 23:30Maybe it fits.
  • 23:31But then what we found was that Adobe
  • 23:34response to the foot shot during
  • 23:36these trials was not only positive,
  • 23:38it actually increases.
  • 23:40Animals got better at the task and so.
  • 23:43We've looked and we said,
  • 23:45OK, this doesn't really fit.
  • 23:46People had seen this safety Q response
  • 23:48before and said look doping doesn't
  • 23:50work of RP in aversive contexts.
  • 23:52But we looked at the other parameters
  • 23:55that this doesn't really make sense.
  • 23:57So now we have this situation
  • 24:00where dopamine responses in the
  • 24:02nucleus accumbens track these
  • 24:05prediction error based computations.
  • 24:07But only in contexts that are reward
  • 24:10based and so everyone has really kind
  • 24:13of design these experiments to parse
  • 24:15weather domain does RP not does all
  • 24:18of what doping does fit these computations.
  • 24:21So we had a problem.
  • 24:24This reward based Association model
  • 24:26was just too simplistic to account
  • 24:28for what domain was doing in the same
  • 24:31animals in his behavioral tasks.
  • 24:33And what we started looking through
  • 24:35the literature is a lot of people.
  • 24:37What they did is once we have this RP
  • 24:39hypothesis reward prediction error.
  • 24:41Apophysis people started saying OK,
  • 24:42well reward fish based dictionary does.
  • 24:44This. Does dopamine look like this?
  • 24:46And the issue with that is that
  • 24:48dopamine does a lot of stuff that
  • 24:50reward prediction error cannot do,
  • 24:52and so we ended up doing all
  • 24:54this broad prediction error.
  • 24:55Modeling those that math cannot
  • 24:56do negative reinforcement.
  • 24:57So we ended up at this problem where
  • 24:59if we wanted to understand what domain
  • 25:01was doing from a computational model.
  • 25:03These models didn't even make
  • 25:06the computations we needed.
  • 25:07So.
  • 25:08We decided to I have a
  • 25:10postdoc who's fantastic,
  • 25:12who's a computational psychologist.
  • 25:13I will not take credit for
  • 25:15developing the model.
  • 25:16This is not my backgrounds,
  • 25:18but we've had a really great
  • 25:20synergistic relationship.
  • 25:21And So what we did is we created a
  • 25:23complex model of learning and memory.
  • 25:26You have the theoretical components of
  • 25:28this model that are developed from site.
  • 25:30Many years of psychology research
  • 25:32and then what we can do is we can
  • 25:35record domain responses and many many
  • 25:37Contacts and we can map the domain responses.
  • 25:40On to the parameters of this model
  • 25:42that we know is capable of modeling
  • 25:45the behavioral outputs we have.
  • 25:47So I'm not going to go into
  • 25:49details like super details,
  • 25:50but I'm really happy for people if
  • 25:52they have questions to talk more
  • 25:54essentially with the model does is
  • 25:56it models the behavioral responses,
  • 25:58the outcomes,
  • 25:59the predictions like people have done before.
  • 26:01We actually have those prediction
  • 26:02based learning algorithms.
  • 26:03But one thing it has that's actually
  • 26:05was been has been found over years.
  • 26:08So you really critical component of learning.
  • 26:10Is this perceived salience term?
  • 26:11And so this is kind of you know how
  • 26:14attention grabbing something is and
  • 26:15so it's really highly influenced by
  • 26:17things in the environment like novelty.
  • 26:20The first time you experience an unexpected
  • 26:22aversive foot shock or something,
  • 26:23it's it's more attention grabbing
  • 26:25the 10th time you present it,
  • 26:26and so this that with this term does
  • 26:28is it influences how we learn based
  • 26:31on these other factors that are
  • 26:33not included in these other models
  • 26:34and what it does is it's able to
  • 26:37make really accurate predictions of
  • 26:39what animals will do in the future.
  • 26:42Again, we use this model to figure out
  • 26:44what experiments would dissociate these
  • 26:45different factors from one another.
  • 26:47So after I show you this,
  • 26:48you can ignore the model stuff
  • 26:50if you want and just look at the
  • 26:52experiments we run to parse.
  • 26:54Kind of what's going on.
  • 26:56The really important thing about this
  • 26:58model is the simulations from the model.
  • 27:00The behavioral output
  • 27:01simulations are in grey,
  • 27:03and the behavioral data itself
  • 27:04is in blue and you can see it can
  • 27:07start to model these things that
  • 27:09couldn't be modeled before,
  • 27:11so things like the animals to train
  • 27:13to know spoke for sucrose and then
  • 27:15we have we introduce an aversive
  • 27:17foot shock all the sudden.
  • 27:19The animals responding goes down.
  • 27:20That's what the model suggests.
  • 27:22We can model how animals will learn
  • 27:24and negative reinforcement Contacts
  • 27:26the removal of an aversive stimulus.
  • 27:28And so we're now able to model the
  • 27:31behavioral outcomes of these more
  • 27:33complex behaviors but use these same
  • 27:35kind of computations to not punch to me so.
  • 27:38What I'm going to show you is that
  • 27:40dopamine does not track reward
  • 27:42prediction error.
  • 27:42It tracks perceived salience,
  • 27:44which is just kind of like how
  • 27:46attention grabbing a stimulus is.
  • 27:48Perceived salience is the perception
  • 27:51of housing question.
  • 27:52Yes,
  • 27:53just a quick question.
  • 27:55How different is this model from
  • 27:58Pierce Hall Macintosh model of?
  • 28:02It's it's actually very similar,
  • 28:04so it includes the.
  • 28:05So the thing about the Pearsall Macintosh
  • 28:07models for the people who don't know is
  • 28:09they include these attentional terms,
  • 28:11which are really actually important
  • 28:12for things like Leighton ambition,
  • 28:14things that these other models can't do.
  • 28:16It includes the same kind of
  • 28:18computational terms, and this perceived
  • 28:19salience term is essentially kind
  • 28:21of what was added to that model.
  • 28:23It's based on a neural net model,
  • 28:25so it's a little bit different in the math,
  • 28:27but it's the same kind of idea,
  • 28:30and that's it that the
  • 28:31attentional value of things.
  • 28:32Are going to influence the associative
  • 28:34strength and how animals learn,
  • 28:35and so that's that's what we're adding
  • 28:38that allows us to do these things.
  • 28:40And then on top of that, we've added these.
  • 28:42They operate probabilistic responses because
  • 28:44pavlovi and learning is not probabilistic,
  • 28:45and so we've added that to that.
  • 28:47To that kind of framework.
  • 28:49So it's very, very similar to that.
  • 28:51Thank you for that question.
  • 28:52I don't go into too much detail 'cause
  • 28:54people don't always know about these models,
  • 28:57but that's actually kind of the
  • 28:58framework by which we're using it.
  • 29:00More appears Hall that the Macintosh,
  • 29:02though.
  • 29:04OK, so we can model the behavior we need,
  • 29:07so so again,
  • 29:08this perceived salience is kind of
  • 29:10like a driving animals attention
  • 29:11towards States and so it's really
  • 29:13highly affected by the in physical
  • 29:15intensity of a stimulus and the
  • 29:17novelty and environment that
  • 29:18changes how you attend to stimulate.
  • 29:21And So what we did is we started.
  • 29:24We said OK,
  • 29:25if it's tracking just the saliency,
  • 29:27it should increase with physical
  • 29:29intensity of a stimulus.
  • 29:30Whether it's positive or negative.
  • 29:32And so this is a increasing
  • 29:34series of foot shocks on the left.
  • 29:37That is,
  • 29:38the simulations from that perceived
  • 29:40salience term of our model,
  • 29:41and on the right is the actual
  • 29:44dopamine recorded responses to
  • 29:46increasing intensities of foot shocks
  • 29:47and what you see is that dopamine
  • 29:50increases with foot shock intensity.
  • 29:52It also increases when we
  • 29:55increase the volume of sucrose.
  • 29:57So a better than expected appetitive.
  • 30:00Reward or the volume of quiet Night,
  • 30:02which is a bitter taste scent,
  • 30:04and so everyone always asks.
  • 30:06And it's a great question.
  • 30:07Foot shocks are kind of
  • 30:09weird aversive stimuli,
  • 30:10so for many of these tasks we have
  • 30:12worked in other things that are aversive.
  • 30:15But not, you know,
  • 30:16painful.
  • 30:16We can argue all day about whether
  • 30:18foot shock is pain or something else,
  • 30:20but we see the same pattern with
  • 30:22other types of aversive stimulation,
  • 30:24and so this is ruling out simple,
  • 30:26rewarding coding by dopamine,
  • 30:28because dopamine in the incumbents
  • 30:29is going up to both appetitive
  • 30:31and aversive stimuli.
  • 30:32So it cannot be just rewards.
  • 30:36So the other thing that influences
  • 30:39perceived salience is novelty.
  • 30:40So how much experience
  • 30:42you have with something.
  • 30:43So what we did is we took foot
  • 30:46shocks of the same intensity.
  • 30:49Intensity is not changed and we repeated
  • 30:51them in a series on a fixed interval,
  • 30:54so every every 60 seconds
  • 30:56the animals got a foot shot.
  • 30:59What we found is that doping goes down
  • 31:02to the foot shots even though the
  • 31:04intensity of the foot shock stays.
  • 31:07Constant, so it's also not just
  • 31:10encoding only the intensity,
  • 31:12it's encoding other aspects
  • 31:14like novelty as well,
  • 31:16and so you get decreased domain
  • 31:19responses to repeated exposures
  • 31:22of stimulate both aversive.
  • 31:24And neutral simulate,
  • 31:25and so this is an auditory tone,
  • 31:28so we've done this with tones,
  • 31:30lights, and white noise.
  • 31:32This is an example from white noise,
  • 31:34but what you see which is
  • 31:37important is the first exposure
  • 31:39of a stimulus that is neutral,
  • 31:41elicits dopamine and repeated exposures
  • 31:44of that stimulus go down overtime.
  • 31:47So you're getting decreased dopamine
  • 31:49when animals are exposed repeatedly
  • 31:51to stimuli in the environment,
  • 31:53regardless of whether they have
  • 31:55positive value, negative value,
  • 31:57or are what we think of as neutral.
  • 32:01I'm.
  • 32:02So another aspect that I think,
  • 32:05and this is really important experiment
  • 32:07because it really rules out this
  • 32:09reward prediction error based learning.
  • 32:11So what we did is we trained animals to
  • 32:13know spoke during a discriminative cue.
  • 32:16So auditory cue comes on.
  • 32:17If they respond they get sucrose.
  • 32:21Without any signal,
  • 32:22we now switch it to the same auditory cue.
  • 32:25If they press,
  • 32:27they get shocked,
  • 32:28so we're switching that the Q is
  • 32:30the same and it now represents
  • 32:33a worse than expected outcome.
  • 32:36And so.
  • 32:36What should happen in a task
  • 32:38like this is that the animals
  • 32:41in this grezar simulation,
  • 32:42the animal should reduce
  • 32:44their behavioral responding.
  • 32:46They do not surprisingly,
  • 32:47this is a traditional kind
  • 32:48of punishment task.
  • 32:49Animals will reduce their behavior.
  • 32:52But what our model predicts,
  • 32:54this perceived salience model.
  • 32:55Is that because there's unexpected
  • 32:57information and it's novel?
  • 32:59There should be increase
  • 33:01in dopamine to this Q.
  • 33:03Prediction error responding with
  • 33:05say it should be decreased because
  • 33:07it's a worse than expected outcome.
  • 33:10A reward prediction error, excuse me.
  • 33:12So what we're going to look
  • 33:14at here is this Q,
  • 33:16which is the last Q that predicted sucrose,
  • 33:18so this is before the animals
  • 33:20got gotten shocked.
  • 33:21So this Q still has that predict sucrose,
  • 33:24and then we're going to look at the next
  • 33:27Q right after the first foot shock.
  • 33:30And what we find is first,
  • 33:33this foot shock causes a
  • 33:35positive domain response.
  • 33:36So it doesn't matter what kind of task,
  • 33:40but shocks are being presented and
  • 33:42they're always words resulting
  • 33:44in positive domain responses.
  • 33:46What the dopamine response to this
  • 33:48discriminative cue actually goes up,
  • 33:50even though it's it represents a
  • 33:53worse than expected outcomes and
  • 33:55so dopamine is increasing anytime
  • 33:58information is novel or salient
  • 34:00to the animal.
  • 34:01And it's increasing even if
  • 34:03the outcome is worse than
  • 34:05expected or better than expected,
  • 34:07and one of the key aspects of this
  • 34:10experiment is dopamine is going up,
  • 34:12even though the animal's
  • 34:14behavior is going down,
  • 34:15so increases in dopamine don't just
  • 34:17mean motivated behavior or approach
  • 34:19because we're getting increases in
  • 34:21dopamine here that correlate with animals
  • 34:24inhibiting a behavioral response,
  • 34:25and so this kind of saliency.
  • 34:27What it'll do, is it helps animals make
  • 34:31adaptive updating of responses were.
  • 34:33List of what the context of those responses
  • 34:36are or the behavioral response necessary.
  • 34:39So I'm gonna show I think
  • 34:41I've one more experiment,
  • 34:43so this experiment is
  • 34:45actually really important,
  • 34:46because it's kind of shows how much these
  • 34:49kind of novel salients events that don't
  • 34:53necessarily acquire value R to animals.
  • 34:56So what we did is we did an experiment
  • 34:59where we associated ECU with a foot shock,
  • 35:02so just fear conditioning.
  • 35:04But what we did is on some of
  • 35:07the trials we just put a random
  • 35:10irrelevant house light on.
  • 35:12And what the model predicts is that because
  • 35:14there is novelty in the environment,
  • 35:16there will be an increase in dopamine
  • 35:18response on these trials where novel
  • 35:20information is added even though previous
  • 35:23work has shown that novel irrelevant
  • 35:25information will not acquire value.
  • 35:27So the Q that we're adding this random
  • 35:29irrelevant light won't acquire value
  • 35:31because the animals already associated
  • 35:34the previous Q with the foot shot.
  • 35:36And So what we find is on trials
  • 35:38where we add this novel light.
  • 35:41There is a very large increase
  • 35:43in the domain response,
  • 35:44even though that novel light
  • 35:46won't acquire value itself.
  • 35:47And So what this does is it rules
  • 35:50out the simple Attribution of some
  • 35:52sort of balance to a queue or
  • 35:55associative strength of that Q.
  • 35:57It means that you're getting
  • 35:59increases in dopamine responses.
  • 36:00They don't necessarily correspond with
  • 36:02the animal making Association between
  • 36:05that queue and the outcome in some cases.
  • 36:11The last thing, which again for
  • 36:12people who are are really deep
  • 36:14in the prediction based field.
  • 36:16One thing that people can
  • 36:17say at this point is, well,
  • 36:19maybe doping is doing prediction error
  • 36:21but not reward prediction error.
  • 36:22So it's going up every time
  • 36:24there is an error in prediction.
  • 36:26This is actually really good good thought
  • 36:28and we thought this too and we said OK.
  • 36:30Well let's let's see if that is the
  • 36:33case and we were kind of Gnostic here.
  • 36:35We were saying, OK,
  • 36:36let's just figure out what it does.
  • 36:38We're not trying to.
  • 36:40Pusha theory we're saying, well,
  • 36:41how does the data fit together?
  • 36:44So saliency or perceived salience?
  • 36:46What it would suggest is that when
  • 36:48you have a stimulus like a foot shock,
  • 36:52you should have the biggest opening response,
  • 36:55because when the stimulus is
  • 36:56present and there it's the most
  • 36:59salient 'cause most intense.
  • 37:00But if you have a prediction of
  • 37:03that during extinction or omission,
  • 37:05there should still be a positive
  • 37:08doping response,
  • 37:08but it should be lower than when the
  • 37:11stimulus is physically there a prediction.
  • 37:14Error hypothesis would be that
  • 37:16when you have an omission,
  • 37:17this response should be higher
  • 37:19than when the stimulus is there
  • 37:21because it signals an error.
  • 37:25We did this experiment.
  • 37:27What we found was that there's a
  • 37:29positive domain response at the time
  • 37:31of the predicted foot shock when
  • 37:33it's omitted, so it's not there,
  • 37:35but the response of the foot shock
  • 37:38itself is higher than when it's omitted.
  • 37:40And so this also rules out
  • 37:43other competing theories,
  • 37:44which is that domain does
  • 37:45prediction error learning,
  • 37:46but it's not reward based.
  • 37:50So. And I showed you a lot of stuff and,
  • 37:54well, I kind of rule kind of come
  • 37:56back and say like why should you care?
  • 37:59So essentially what we did is we did
  • 38:01a number of experiments to rule out
  • 38:03these kind of competing factors of what
  • 38:05dopamine is doing in learning and memory.
  • 38:07Don't mean release is doing.
  • 38:08I'm not saying the VTA cell
  • 38:10bodies don't do this.
  • 38:11Maybe they do,
  • 38:11but there's integration of information
  • 38:13at the level of the terminal that
  • 38:15dictates how doing is actually
  • 38:16releasing these brain regions and
  • 38:17what I am saying is that dopamine
  • 38:19release in the nucleus accumbens,
  • 38:21core Maps on true perceived.
  • 38:22Salience not prediction error or value.
  • 38:27Our models are modeled behavior,
  • 38:28we just use it to generate experiments
  • 38:30we should run to parse different
  • 38:32aspects of domain encoding,
  • 38:33and So what you can do is use
  • 38:35these kind of predictions to make
  • 38:37experiments with other circuits as well,
  • 38:39which I think is kind of an interesting
  • 38:42way to approach the question.
  • 38:45But what I'm showing you is that
  • 38:47even in reinforcement context,
  • 38:48pavlovi in context,
  • 38:49this isn't a value based prediction signal,
  • 38:51and these same signals are
  • 38:53there in punishment tasks in
  • 38:54negative reinforcement tasks,
  • 38:55and so it's actually really
  • 38:57interesting that you're seeing
  • 38:58this kind of dopamine signal.
  • 39:00It's very critical in driving behaviors,
  • 39:02just not in the way that I
  • 39:04think we predicted before.
  • 39:07So why should we care?
  • 39:08I think understanding what domain is
  • 39:11doing is really important for disease,
  • 39:13and so if you want to understand what
  • 39:15dopamine is doing and what deficits in
  • 39:18dopamine in a patient mean for that patient,
  • 39:21it really requires a kind of
  • 39:23holistic understanding.
  • 39:24What domains doing across contexts and
  • 39:26internal States and things like that,
  • 39:28and so you know when you have a model and
  • 39:32you say doesken dopamine fit this model,
  • 39:35the answer might be yes.
  • 39:37But it kind of leaves out that aspect of.
  • 39:39But what is domain doing in other
  • 39:42contexts of the model can't fit and
  • 39:44so understanding the components that
  • 39:46dry of these behaviors is really
  • 39:49critical to understanding this.
  • 39:51But I think that maybe more
  • 39:53important thing for for kind of
  • 39:55human health is is from, you know,
  • 39:57my primary field which is
  • 39:59addiction an it's understand.
  • 40:00The difference between a dopamine
  • 40:02signal that signals were worn
  • 40:04and what a salience signal does.
  • 40:05So a reward signal.
  • 40:07If you have a deficits in a reward signal,
  • 40:09you may say you know we don't
  • 40:11want to increase those and people
  • 40:13suffering from substance use disorder
  • 40:15because if we do that may increase
  • 40:17the rewarding value of stimuli
  • 40:19in the environment like drugs.
  • 40:21But the issue is with the salience signal.
  • 40:24If you have deficits,
  • 40:26it's going to slow the rate of learning
  • 40:29for everything in the environment,
  • 40:31so it could explain why people
  • 40:34are compulsive because they don't
  • 40:36respond to negative outcomes.
  • 40:38It would explain why they have trouble
  • 40:41learning. The adaptive alternatives.
  • 40:42Are there an?
  • 40:43It would explain why extinguishing
  • 40:46drug associations is much slower,
  • 40:48and so in if it's a saliency signal,
  • 40:51we may want to.
  • 40:52Increased opening so that people can
  • 40:54learn adaptively in all of these contexts.
  • 40:57And so again,
  • 40:58I'm not saying that like you know,
  • 41:00this is the end all be all dopamine is
  • 41:02in lots of projection targets and it
  • 41:04does lots of things in different areas.
  • 41:07And we're in one single area,
  • 41:08but I think kind of taking a step
  • 41:10back and thinking about what these
  • 41:12domains signatures really mean
  • 41:14and what those deficits would
  • 41:15mean to a behaving individual,
  • 41:17as I think it's really important
  • 41:19component of conceptualizing
  • 41:20what these you know psychiatric
  • 41:22deficits mean to people and how
  • 41:24to best treat them.
  • 41:25Anyway, so with that I'll
  • 41:27end with thanking my lab,
  • 41:29so Ganesh clue his background
  • 41:30is in computational psychology,
  • 41:31so he's like the modeler and he's
  • 41:34really like driven this you know.
  • 41:35Together he's a Pavlovian guy.
  • 41:37I was a reinforcement person.
  • 41:39I think this was like one of
  • 41:40those projects that was this great
  • 41:42synergism between two people who
  • 41:44just we're really excited about.
  • 41:46Kind of figuring out what's going on.
  • 41:48Jennifer, Zachary and Patrick and
  • 41:50Stephanie were are grad students
  • 41:52that were working on this project
  • 41:54and put a lot of time into it.
  • 41:56Cody Siciliano and Lindsay Ann Lynn
  • 41:58was was really nice and was helping us
  • 42:01get the delight these optical sensors
  • 42:03up and running in the lab fairly
  • 42:06early in Cody's at optical engineer.
  • 42:07So he helps a lot at Vanderbilt with
  • 42:10getting these working correctly,
  • 42:12validating them an my funding and I
  • 42:14can take any questions you may have.
  • 42:17So thank you so much.
  • 42:22Thank you so much Aaron.
  • 42:23If anybody doesn't want to pipe
  • 42:25up with the just asking questions,
  • 42:27please put them in the chat and
  • 42:30I can read them out for Aaron.
  • 42:33Like can I start with one?
  • 42:35Go for it. So in terms of those
  • 42:38projections you were discussing
  • 42:40and you're looking in the core,
  • 42:42do you think that just with predictions
  • 42:44of what the core versus shell of the
  • 42:47incumbents does in activations of the
  • 42:49core versus Shell does to behavior?
  • 42:51Do you think that one might be
  • 42:54more important than the other in
  • 42:56the prediction error?
  • 42:57This is actually really
  • 42:59great question an I should
  • 43:00have pasted this.
  • 43:01Actually I can do it now.
  • 43:04I we have shell data so so.
  • 43:06The answer is probably yes in some contexts,
  • 43:09although when we we started to
  • 43:12look through the shell data.
  • 43:15So I just wanted to side when
  • 43:17we started to look through the
  • 43:20new data or the shell data.
  • 43:22It did not look like what
  • 43:24I would expect either.
  • 43:28Basically. It still doesn't
  • 43:31look like prediction error.
  • 43:33So we still get a positive
  • 43:36domain response to the shock.
  • 43:38We get some scaling with stimulus intensity.
  • 43:41Not quite as much.
  • 43:44People have shown decreases in dopamine
  • 43:46and to aversive stimuli and we have
  • 43:49been working out why that would be when
  • 43:51we aren't seeing them and we think.
  • 43:53And so basically we did show
  • 43:55decreases in domain in some contexts.
  • 43:57Dopamine goes down when animals don't
  • 44:00have to do anything or they have to wait.
  • 44:02So what we did is we design
  • 44:05this other experiment that I'm
  • 44:06like really excited about.
  • 44:08What we did is we trained animals to know
  • 44:11smoker sucrose and then we switched the
  • 44:13contingency so that they had to with.
  • 44:16Hold a response and wait to get sucrose.
  • 44:18So this is kind of like the same reciprocal
  • 44:20thing to fear conditioning right?
  • 44:22You have a queue.
  • 44:23The animal just waits to get shocked.
  • 44:25There's nothing they can
  • 44:26do during that period.
  • 44:27They wait,
  • 44:28we see decreases in dopamine to
  • 44:30fear conditioning Q and two AQ,
  • 44:31where the animal gets sucrose at the end,
  • 44:34but they have to wait to do it.
  • 44:37And So what we think is happening is a
  • 44:39lot of these decreases in domain people
  • 44:42have seen or not necessarily just value.
  • 44:45They have to do with what animals
  • 44:47are doing and what novelty in
  • 44:49salience do in an environment
  • 44:51is they increase exploration.
  • 44:53So if you need to decrease exploration
  • 44:55and just wait for something to happen,
  • 44:58domain goes down.
  • 44:59So we get doping reductions even when
  • 45:02the outcome is positive when the task
  • 45:04design mimics that of the aversive
  • 45:06tasks where people have seen reductions.
  • 45:09And a lot of the like you know,
  • 45:11there's a lot of great work from
  • 45:13like Mitchell White Men looking at
  • 45:15aversive like wine in the mouth.
  • 45:17It's unavoidable.
  • 45:17The animals are just waiting there as well.
  • 45:20And so I think there's also
  • 45:21differences in relative,
  • 45:22you know,
  • 45:23in his designs he has positive and
  • 45:25negative stimuli in the same task,
  • 45:27which are a little bit different
  • 45:28than having an animal behaves.
  • 45:30So obviously there's there is some
  • 45:31sort of value based computation,
  • 45:33but we think they're done and
  • 45:35really specific context.
  • 45:38Thank you.
  • 45:39I think the other question go ahead.
  • 45:44But they are zoom etiquette.
  • 45:46Go ahead, I'll jump in after you.
  • 45:49OK, thanks for bringing talker
  • 45:50and that was really really cool.
  • 45:52So one thing that you can do with
  • 45:55these models is sort of see if they
  • 45:57can predict particular phenomena
  • 45:58and one of the ideas I think that's
  • 46:01becoming more and more prevalent
  • 46:02about a sort of teleological idea
  • 46:04about what the dopamine system
  • 46:06might be for is not rewards,
  • 46:08not punishments as you've argued,
  • 46:09but actually the causal
  • 46:10structure of the world.
  • 46:12Yeah, and so does your model
  • 46:14predict things like sensory
  • 46:15preconditioning where?
  • 46:16There was
  • 46:16no value at all.
  • 46:18Initially you use that information
  • 46:19later to imbue or impede value.
  • 46:21Yes, yes, so actually this is one
  • 46:23of the other powers of this model is
  • 46:25it can do sensory preconditioning,
  • 46:28Layton addition,
  • 46:28so these are two things that that even
  • 46:31the temporal difference models cannot do,
  • 46:33and the problem is dopamine does them.
  • 46:36So if dopamine does these
  • 46:37in a computational model,
  • 46:39can't that cannot be the
  • 46:41computation domain is doing,
  • 46:42and so we we have our next at
  • 46:45once we get this out the door.
  • 46:47I'm trying to find my.
  • 46:49We haven't started doing sensory
  • 46:50preconditioning yet because of the fact that
  • 46:53it's a little bit more of a pain in mice,
  • 46:56and I think we're going to
  • 46:58have to switch to rats.
  • 47:00Mice aren't like the best set
  • 47:02like attending to things so,
  • 47:04so lame.
  • 47:05Inhibition is something that
  • 47:06our model does do.
  • 47:08So late inhibition is actually
  • 47:10this really interesting novelty
  • 47:11based learning constructs were
  • 47:13essentially pre exposed stimuli,
  • 47:14acquire values slower than simulated.
  • 47:16Have not been pre exposed so familiar
  • 47:18stimuli take longer to because you're
  • 47:20basically unlearning the no Association.
  • 47:23So a lot of these different models
  • 47:25you know we brought this up earlier.
  • 47:28I was asked about Pierce Hall Macintosh.
  • 47:30Like all of these models have
  • 47:33added these components to do this
  • 47:35and our model does this and the
  • 47:37sensory preconditioning.
  • 47:39I showed you the dobine goes
  • 47:41down to repeated shocks.
  • 47:42Um?
  • 47:42We can get pre exposed stimuli to
  • 47:45have less associative value and
  • 47:47what's really interesting is that
  • 47:50the dopamine response to these pre
  • 47:53exposed stimuli is much lower and it
  • 47:56also tracks over the pre exposure period.
  • 47:59So these these kind of non value based
  • 48:01learning constructs were previous
  • 48:03experience is changing the way that
  • 48:05stimuli can drive future behavior
  • 48:07or sensory preconditioning were two
  • 48:10irrelevant simulate form associations
  • 48:12that can then be associated later.
  • 48:14Our model does it,
  • 48:15and dopamine still Maps onto that
  • 48:17perceived salience term in those contexts.
  • 48:19And this is actually why we were so set on.
  • 48:21You know,
  • 48:22the first experiments really well
  • 48:23it could be other things too,
  • 48:25and then we started going
  • 48:26into these latent addition and
  • 48:27sensory preconditioning ideas,
  • 48:29because those really can't be other things.
  • 48:31I mean,
  • 48:31it could be there's other components of it,
  • 48:34but I think it is more strong
  • 48:35with the other stuff.
  • 48:37It does too, that that's what it's doing.
  • 48:39But it's that's a great, I think.
  • 48:41Those are like the killer like knife
  • 48:43in the coffin experiments, right?
  • 48:44Because they just.
  • 48:45Those other models cannot do them,
  • 48:47so yeah, that's a great great point.
  • 48:51I think we had a question,
  • 48:52thanks. Those are really great talk here
  • 48:54and thanks for taking us through all that.
  • 48:56And you have touched on my question
  • 48:58a little bit because you started.
  • 49:00I mean, even with the default
  • 49:02Pomeranians when he starts to
  • 49:03look at nucleus accumbens shell.
  • 49:04But one thing I was curious about with your
  • 49:07model is how how global do you think it is?
  • 49:10And you hinted as we talked about the
  • 49:12VTA component as well. So you know,
  • 49:14going back to some of the Bromberg,
  • 49:16Martin work about different VTA
  • 49:17neurons responding differently
  • 49:18to balance versus silence.
  • 49:19You think this Maps onto multiple regions
  • 49:21using this sub regions within the.
  • 49:23The common score is a global number
  • 49:25as a discrete to specific places.
  • 49:27Yeah, so this is a great question,
  • 49:29so I'm lucky to have married very well in
  • 49:32my life and my partner is a two photon guide.
  • 49:35When he does,
  • 49:36he does 2 photon imaging through grin lenses,
  • 49:39cranial windows, you name it and
  • 49:40so one thing we're working on now.
  • 49:43The really power of of these kind
  • 49:45of optical imaging approaches is
  • 49:47that you can record as small as
  • 49:49you want or as big as you want,
  • 49:51and so depending on your.
  • 49:53Microscope and resolution so
  • 49:54we're moving into these.
  • 49:55Either I love slice work.
  • 49:57This is like my background so
  • 49:59we're moving into these kind of.
  • 50:00In vivo in slice imaging approaches
  • 50:02to understand better domain
  • 50:04regulation across big and small areas.
  • 50:06Because the thing about domain
  • 50:07neurons that so you know,
  • 50:09like kind of weird about
  • 50:11them is their projection.
  • 50:12Like you know, Arborization is insane.
  • 50:14If you fill a single domain on the PTA
  • 50:17and look at the field that it populates,
  • 50:20it's like half the straight up.
  • 50:22But then if you look at these specific
  • 50:24release sites on these neurons,
  • 50:26it's not releasing dopamine
  • 50:27at everywhere every time.
  • 50:29It's depending on all these different things.
  • 50:31So this.
  • 50:32Release structure is so complicated and
  • 50:33I think part of the reason people have
  • 50:36been so like oh volume transmission
  • 50:38is our ability to really look at these
  • 50:40granularities between these components.
  • 50:42And so we're starting to go,
  • 50:44you know, start big.
  • 50:45We're just saying,
  • 50:46OK,
  • 50:46if we do image in a bigger field or
  • 50:49with multiple sites at the same time,
  • 50:51are we seeing differences?
  • 50:52We do see differences so I think there
  • 50:55are differences in these VTA neurons in
  • 50:57what what they're doing in different areas.
  • 50:59So I don't think like all dopamine is.
  • 51:02This I think don't mean to the core is this,
  • 51:05but it also makes sense that domain
  • 51:06in the core that's been tide more to
  • 51:08instrumental responding than like the shell.
  • 51:10That's like these acquisition.
  • 51:11And like Valeant space kind of learning
  • 51:13would look like a perceived salience term,
  • 51:15right?
  • 51:15That makes way more sense for something
  • 51:17that is involved in punishment,
  • 51:18negative reinforcement,
  • 51:19positive reinforcement,
  • 51:19which are the same motivated responses,
  • 51:21independent violence.
  • 51:22So we're doing some more stuff in the
  • 51:24shell. You know, I'm not sold that the
  • 51:26shell doesn't do value because I don't think
  • 51:28foot shocks are the best way to do stuff.
  • 51:30I think foot shocks are weird.
  • 51:32Stimulus that are really powerful initially,
  • 51:34but we didn't really evolve
  • 51:36to respond to foot shocks,
  • 51:37so we're starting to go in
  • 51:39more with things like Quy 9.
  • 51:41We've been developing.
  • 51:42You can make them liquor lic ometer hot,
  • 51:45so we've been doing like thermal,
  • 51:47like not pain, but thermal sensitivity
  • 51:49curves so that you can look at thermal
  • 51:51stimuli that reduced responding.
  • 51:53But without this, like foot shot component,
  • 51:55so we're trying to parse this out.
  • 51:57I'm not sold that,
  • 51:58it's just like every dopamine responses
  • 52:00that I think it's more complicated,
  • 52:02but I think we need better resolution
  • 52:05techniques to really parse that.
  • 52:07And hopefully over the next I don't know.
  • 52:09However long my career last
  • 52:10will see will start to get it.
  • 52:11Some of those questions and
  • 52:12other people are doing that too.
  • 52:14I mean,
  • 52:14there's some really great work
  • 52:15coming out where people are
  • 52:16using those like single synapse.
  • 52:18You know,
  • 52:18you know Uncaging and Eli and
  • 52:19all kinds of crazy stuff,
  • 52:21so I'm excited to see where the field goes.
  • 52:23Yeah,
  • 52:23that's really excited how
  • 52:24great you are looking
  • 52:25at. I'm glad you're looking
  • 52:26into it. Sounds like
  • 52:27you thought about it already. Definitely
  • 52:28I went a little bit. Now
  • 52:29the question is just like do we
  • 52:31have the tools and then the next
  • 52:33thing is do we have the month of
  • 52:34money and the people that do it?
  • 52:36And so it's like you know.
  • 52:38You you see what you can do and
  • 52:39with the resources you have so.
  • 52:44There is a question in the chat by
  • 52:47from Denise, Baghdad and Denise.
  • 52:48Do you want to read it out or
  • 52:50would you like me to ask it?
  • 52:53Good morning, great talk,
  • 52:56so I just wanted to understand something.
  • 53:00Maybe it's not a great question.
  • 53:03Let me just say it.
  • 53:06So nicotine reinforcement is
  • 53:09generally considered as positive
  • 53:11reinforcement enforcement.
  • 53:12However, it's also discussed
  • 53:14about like the weather.
  • 53:17Nicotine reinforcement is
  • 53:18negative reinforcement.
  • 53:20Becausw of nicotine withdrawal.
  • 53:22The compost.
  • 53:23Open intake and taking is actually
  • 53:27contributes to nicotine reinforcement,
  • 53:29so I am interested in whether your
  • 53:33model could dissect the positive or
  • 53:36negative reinforcement for the nicotine.
  • 53:39It's I know it's your.
  • 53:44Shock, but this is the one molecule.
  • 53:47You know, so could have both.
  • 53:49So how we put the fact
  • 53:51that's a great question.
  • 53:53So one of the things in the addiction
  • 53:55field is that there are all these
  • 53:57series of negative reinforcement, right?
  • 53:59Like opioid withdrawal alcohol,
  • 54:01withdrawal.
  • 54:01All of these are negative
  • 54:03reinforcement concepts,
  • 54:03but no one actually does
  • 54:05negative reinforcement.
  • 54:06We make the inference that is negative
  • 54:08reinforcement from the fact that it
  • 54:10causes withdrawn animals are taking
  • 54:12it with during the withdrawal period.
  • 54:15It's OK,
  • 54:15so it's a hard question.
  • 54:17I think the first step would
  • 54:19be to look at how you know if
  • 54:22negative reinforcement processes,
  • 54:23like for avoiding shocks,
  • 54:25are changed after nicotine.
  • 54:26So one of the things that we're
  • 54:28working with with Cody Siciliano is
  • 54:30looking at how alcohol changes animals
  • 54:32motivation for negative reinforcers,
  • 54:34and so that's like the first step.
  • 54:37I think this is like it's
  • 54:39a hard thing to parse.
  • 54:41Nicotine is also, I know it's like I do.
  • 54:45A cholinergic regulation
  • 54:46of dopamine terminals.
  • 54:47So nicotine is like in my mind,
  • 54:49but like we don't do nicotine
  • 54:51reinforcement stuff.
  • 54:51It's also this really interesting
  • 54:53molecule because it regulates
  • 54:54like how dopamine is released
  • 54:56in a really interesting way.
  • 54:57That's not just like up,
  • 54:59it's changing like phasic responses
  • 55:00to stimuli in the environment.
  • 55:02And so thinking about the
  • 55:04interaction between those,
  • 55:05it's like we're doing some work
  • 55:07with sex differences in that
  • 55:08system is like so much more
  • 55:10complicated than I want it to be.
  • 55:12Like with cocaine,
  • 55:13it's like it binds to the transporter.
  • 55:15Show me goes up.
  • 55:17Can we reduce that nicotine is like Oh
  • 55:19well in some cases domain goes down
  • 55:22some cases it goes up and so it's
  • 55:24just such a complicated question.
  • 55:25I think the behavioral stuff we do
  • 55:27can start to parse how processes and
  • 55:29animals are changing by exposure,
  • 55:31and I think that's the first step
  • 55:33and then the next step is trying.
  • 55:35We're trying to develop task
  • 55:36to figure out how to do this.
  • 55:38So one thing we've been thinking
  • 55:41about is doing.
  • 55:42Old school drag discrimination so
  • 55:44animals will actually press before
  • 55:46like to tell you an internal state
  • 55:48is X or Y and what we want to do
  • 55:50is we've been thinking about doing
  • 55:52this with optogenetics, right?
  • 55:53Does an optical stimulation of a
  • 55:55circuit substitute for X drug X
  • 55:57state and I think that some of these
  • 55:58withdrawal effects you could see if,
  • 56:00like nicotine withdrawal,
  • 56:01substituted for some of these other things,
  • 56:03and if that was a critical component
  • 56:05of reinforcement isn't hard question.
  • 56:07I think that that's it can
  • 56:09start answering that question,
  • 56:10but I've been thinking about this a lot
  • 56:12and I'm not sure how to specifically.
  • 56:14Parse when an animal is doing something
  • 56:17for two things at the same time.
  • 56:18What component is what I?
  • 56:20I wish I had better answer.
  • 56:22I'm excited about the question,
  • 56:23but I don't have the answer for you.
  • 56:29Is there time for one more Marina
  • 56:32or do we directly is that Beth?
  • 56:34No, it's less less sorry.
  • 56:37I had one too. OK first Liz then Jane.
  • 56:42So Aaron, that was such a beautiful
  • 56:43talk and I love all the different
  • 56:46behavioral experiments that
  • 56:47were inspired by your model.
  • 56:48And one of the powers of this
  • 56:50model is obviously you could
  • 56:52take that salience term out.
  • 56:53And guess how behavior would be
  • 56:55altered by it in the future.
  • 56:57So I'm curious whether you're going
  • 56:59to start looking at blocking these
  • 57:00signals and seeing whether they
  • 57:02match the expectations that the
  • 57:04model would make in particular,
  • 57:05that when you were showing the responses
  • 57:07to the light cue during conditioning,
  • 57:09which shouldn't be involving any learning
  • 57:11like what's the point of that signal?
  • 57:14Our behavior could come from it.
  • 57:15I'm
  • 57:16so excited to just ask me this one.
  • 57:18OK, so there's.
  • 57:21I'm lazy and I don't want to.
  • 57:22Maybe I'll do this why I
  • 57:24should have put these in here.
  • 57:25I didn't think people were going to have.
  • 57:27Not that I didn't think you
  • 57:28would have great questions,
  • 57:29but I didn't think you guys were to ask
  • 57:32questions that I had like specific data for.
  • 57:34OK, so two things. First thing first.
  • 57:37Wow, that looks terrible.
  • 57:39We did do experiments to
  • 57:40eliminate this signal.
  • 57:41You can see I'm like really crafty with this.
  • 57:44This is not my OK so we First things first.
  • 57:47Yes, I'll tell you what I think
  • 57:49that signal is doing and then two.
  • 57:51Well, two.
  • 57:52I'll show you the optic Jenner.
  • 57:54Other we're not.
  • 57:55We're almost done with the
  • 57:56Histology so take this with it.
  • 57:58This is preliminary preliminary ish.
  • 58:00We did two experiments where we
  • 58:02inhibited the signal using what our
  • 58:04model would predict as the condition
  • 58:06response that would dissociate
  • 58:07it from these other components.
  • 58:09Injected Halo rhodopsin in TH
  • 58:11positive neurons in the VTA and
  • 58:13then inhibited the terminal.
  • 58:14So we're only inhibiting
  • 58:15dopamine releasing terminals.
  • 58:16Any comments?
  • 58:18We either inhibited during a Q
  • 58:22predicting fear conditioning.
  • 58:25Or we inhibited or?
  • 58:27We know we stimulated during.
  • 58:28Sorry this is my fault.
  • 58:30We stimulated during these are
  • 58:32two different things.
  • 58:33We stimulated during a fear conditioning
  • 58:36Q or we stimulated channelrhodopsin
  • 58:38during an emitted but expected shock.
  • 58:41If you stimulate and this gets
  • 58:43your questions during a Q,
  • 58:45that's a fair condition.
  • 58:46Q You actually get less freezing,
  • 58:49so this is the opposite of what you
  • 58:51would expect from associative strength,
  • 58:53but it kind of person.
  • 58:55We basically points to this question when
  • 58:58there's novel stimuli in the environment,
  • 59:00you increase exploration.
  • 59:01All of our data,
  • 59:03like the novel Q.
  • 59:04All of our data show that
  • 59:06when you add novelty,
  • 59:08you increase dopamine and the increased
  • 59:10dopamine is associated with more exploration.
  • 59:12And less freezing.
  • 59:13And so these novelty terms,
  • 59:15what they're doing is they're
  • 59:17helping animals to adaptively
  • 59:18learn by increasing exploration.
  • 59:20And like here, decreasing freezing.
  • 59:22So in the same animal,
  • 59:23the queue that wasn't stimulated has just
  • 59:26as much freezing is the wifey group,
  • 59:28and when it's stimulated they freeze less
  • 59:31and that's what our model would predict.
  • 59:33The other thing we did is we show
  • 59:36that we can prevent extinction,
  • 59:38freezing extinction by
  • 59:39stimulating dopamine to the Q,
  • 59:41and so we both prevent extinction.
  • 59:43We prevent extinction by basically
  • 59:45like increasing the salience of that
  • 59:47event so that it doesn't go away,
  • 59:49which is not the same as you'd expect
  • 59:52by these prediction error terms.
  • 59:54So what we think is happening
  • 59:55is that these novel stimuli
  • 59:57are increasing dopamine that
  • 59:58increase in dopamine promotes and.
  • 01:00:00Exploration term,
  • 01:00:01we also did some like deep lab cut based,
  • 01:00:04you know,
  • 01:00:05machine learning algorithms to
  • 01:00:06look at orienting responses.
  • 01:00:08It's not associated with
  • 01:00:09general motor activity,
  • 01:00:10it's associated with orientation
  • 01:00:12towards the novel stimulus.
  • 01:00:13And so we think that these these
  • 01:00:15this saliency term in the Commons
  • 01:00:17is changing the way the animals are
  • 01:00:19interacting with the environment
  • 01:00:21rather than just the associated value.
  • 01:00:23But the problem is these are such
  • 01:00:26complex things to dissociate that.
  • 01:00:28I understand why people solve
  • 01:00:29the data before instead.
  • 01:00:31Oh, it's that balance goes up to rewards,
  • 01:00:33goes down to a fear conditioning Q
  • 01:00:35that looks like violence to me too.
  • 01:00:37You only start to see that
  • 01:00:38it can't be balanced.
  • 01:00:39When you do these kind of really
  • 01:00:41in the weeds like someone showed
  • 01:00:43this in 1950 in psychology,
  • 01:00:44we're gonna do this again with optogenetics
  • 01:00:46kinds of experiments which I don't know.
  • 01:00:48I think those are the fun experiments,
  • 01:00:50but does that answer your question?
  • 01:00:51Yes, thank you, awesome.
  • 01:00:53I think James next and then Rick.
  • 01:00:55Hi that was a great talk.
  • 01:00:59So my question is.
  • 01:01:02You talk about increasing dopamine,
  • 01:01:05and in most of your experiments where
  • 01:01:08you're actually measuring dopamine,
  • 01:01:10you're looking at.
  • 01:01:12Using Delight an if you go back to the
  • 01:01:16not the old psychology experiments,
  • 01:01:19but the 80s dopamine literature.
  • 01:01:21People think about tonic versus phasic,
  • 01:01:24dopamine and a lot of your experiments
  • 01:01:26seem to be focused more on what
  • 01:01:30the phasic dopamine signal is.
  • 01:01:32An I'm wondering whether you have
  • 01:01:35some way that you can simultaneously
  • 01:01:38look at dopamine tone because it
  • 01:01:40may be that things like novelty.
  • 01:01:43Might actually be linked to some
  • 01:01:45of those and you get you know
  • 01:01:47interactions between the two,
  • 01:01:49and you know when you're you're seeing
  • 01:01:51your increase with the delight.
  • 01:01:53What baseline is that on?
  • 01:01:54This is,
  • 01:01:55I'm like you guys
  • 01:01:56are like making my day.
  • 01:01:58I have like I'm this is
  • 01:02:00such a great question.
  • 01:02:02So this is like our first like thing.
  • 01:02:04We're getting out the door.
  • 01:02:06We have a bunch of extra data where what
  • 01:02:09we've been doing and this is the thing.
  • 01:02:12OK, so voltammetry.
  • 01:02:13Is background subtracted?
  • 01:02:14So you can't really get both tonan,
  • 01:02:17phasic stuff in the same experiment.
  • 01:02:19So what people historically done as
  • 01:02:21they said, microanalysis histone,
  • 01:02:23gigha Ruth and voltammetry is
  • 01:02:24is phasic an my the 80s domain.
  • 01:02:27Literatures like where I started my
  • 01:02:29career so I'm very excited about that.
  • 01:02:31Do you like this kind of nice because
  • 01:02:34you have some photobleaching but you
  • 01:02:36can control for that and you don't
  • 01:02:39know what the the problem with it
  • 01:02:41is that you don't know the number.
  • 01:02:44So with microdialysis you get an amount.
  • 01:02:46With voltammetry you calibrate your probe,
  • 01:02:47you have an estimated amount with delight.
  • 01:02:51I haven't found a great way to
  • 01:02:53figure out what the number is,
  • 01:02:54but you can look at relative
  • 01:02:57changes over the session.
  • 01:02:58I don't have this data up and is easier
  • 01:03:01way because we're putting it into
  • 01:03:02something so we have a manuscript that
  • 01:03:05we're getting together and its focus
  • 01:03:07on novelty based changes and joking signals,
  • 01:03:10and so it's focused on Lane in addition,
  • 01:03:12But what we're looking at is
  • 01:03:14the phasic response relative
  • 01:03:15to longer changes in dopamine,
  • 01:03:17and I don't want to call it tone
  • 01:03:20because it's over like 10 minutes.
  • 01:03:22Not like ours,
  • 01:03:23but it's definitely not what you
  • 01:03:25would call a phasic fast response.
  • 01:03:27What novelty in the environment does?
  • 01:03:29Is it increases that phasic response?
  • 01:03:31But then it does it.
  • 01:03:32The baseline is much higher.
  • 01:03:34So what you have is this shift and
  • 01:03:36what we think is happening is that
  • 01:03:38the novelty is changing the state of
  • 01:03:41the system so that if the next thing
  • 01:03:43that's encountered in that situation
  • 01:03:45the domain response will be bigger.
  • 01:03:47Now one of the things I've always
  • 01:03:49been interested in over my career
  • 01:03:51is what matters for the animal that
  • 01:03:53change from baseline or the peak.
  • 01:03:55And so we're trying to get into that.
  • 01:03:57Now to say like, OK, that increasing.
  • 01:04:00Slide does that just increase the peak?
  • 01:04:03Or does that actually still amplify
  • 01:04:05more this signal to noise and so these
  • 01:04:09novelty terms are definitely changing.
  • 01:04:11What I would call.
  • 01:04:13I don't want to,
  • 01:04:15but they are definitely changing
  • 01:04:16these slower baseline fluctuations in
  • 01:04:18dopamine over longer periods of time,
  • 01:04:20and we think that's really important
  • 01:04:22for the effects of novelty on
  • 01:04:24other types of learning,
  • 01:04:25and so this dopamine,
  • 01:04:26perceived salience,
  • 01:04:27perceived failings is influenced by novelty,
  • 01:04:29so anything that changes novelty
  • 01:04:31will change this as well,
  • 01:04:32and so it's really important in
  • 01:04:34these novelty based learning
  • 01:04:36things on these both slow and fast
  • 01:04:38timescales in a way that is,
  • 01:04:40I think,
  • 01:04:40consistent with what people have seen
  • 01:04:42with microdialysis and voltammetry,
  • 01:04:44but in a more you are able to more granularly
  • 01:04:46relate them with this, But again,
  • 01:04:48you don't have the amount, so I kind of,
  • 01:04:50you know, it's hard because there's no
  • 01:04:52like calibration like you can't say,
  • 01:04:54oh, this is the amount,
  • 01:04:55and I think that's where I hesitate
  • 01:04:57a little bit to make these really
  • 01:04:59strong conclusions about like what.
  • 01:05:00It is, I know that it's changed from
  • 01:05:02the minute one of this session.
  • 01:05:04The question is like exactly what is that?
  • 01:05:06But there are those changes and I think
  • 01:05:08that's a great point that people.
  • 01:05:10I think a lot of people have gone into
  • 01:05:12these kind of optical measurements and they.
  • 01:05:15That was ignored,
  • 01:05:16but they're not necessarily rooted in these,
  • 01:05:19like microdialysis.
  • 01:05:19Will Tanistry fields,
  • 01:05:21and so they don't understand that
  • 01:05:23there has been a ton of work parsing
  • 01:05:26what these tonic changes mean?
  • 01:05:28How tonic dopamine is regulated relative
  • 01:05:31to like fast release like the domain
  • 01:05:33transporters? My favorite protein because
  • 01:05:35I guess the question I guess I'm asking is,
  • 01:05:39would you see some more reward evidence
  • 01:05:42of more reward prediction error if
  • 01:05:45you somehow subtracted what the?
  • 01:05:47Basil Change is an are you missing some
  • 01:05:50of that? Because your tonic salience?
  • 01:05:52I mean, you started out talking a lot about
  • 01:05:55how dopamine could be doing this and that,
  • 01:05:58but it also could be doing all of it.
  • 01:06:01You know it's not mutually exclusive.
  • 01:06:04It could be.
  • 01:06:05It could be let me.
  • 01:06:07I do have a data that answers
  • 01:06:09that question because actually a
  • 01:06:11really astute reviewer asked us.
  • 01:06:13Now I need to find it. It wasn't me.
  • 01:06:17They they they asked us saying,
  • 01:06:18oh, there it is, we did this,
  • 01:06:21so this is actually a really,
  • 01:06:22really good question.
  • 01:06:23Why is this coming up?
  • 01:06:25Oh I'm like not looking at the right file.
  • 01:06:27Maybe if I can get it in like
  • 01:06:302 seconds I'm going to write,
  • 01:06:31But basically we did what you
  • 01:06:33your question is a good one.
  • 01:06:35I think one of the things that they ask,
  • 01:06:37which is a great question.
  • 01:06:39We'd already been thinking about
  • 01:06:40this so we were like, OK, cool was.
  • 01:06:43If these changes in baseline are the
  • 01:06:45reason that we don't see changes.
  • 01:06:47In in a, let's see file new release.
  • 01:06:51All these will be in here now. Insert.
  • 01:06:55Side OK. OK, So what we did here?
  • 01:07:01Is we had that repeated shock
  • 01:07:04experiment and their question
  • 01:07:05was kind of what yours is like.
  • 01:07:07Well, maybe the difference is the baseline,
  • 01:07:09like the baseline is changing overtime
  • 01:07:11and so we calculated the shocks in
  • 01:07:14the original stuff I showed from like
  • 01:07:16a 2 second window before the event
  • 01:07:18and then we went back and calculated
  • 01:07:20all the shocks from a global baseline
  • 01:07:23at the very beginning of each trial.
  • 01:07:25And what we found is that
  • 01:07:27the data is correlated,
  • 01:07:28like very highly correlated.
  • 01:07:29And if we look at the baseline change.
  • 01:07:32Over that trial it is not changing,
  • 01:07:34so it can't explain all of it.
  • 01:07:36Like I understand,
  • 01:07:37I do agree that there is a lot of
  • 01:07:39baseline stuff that's this where we
  • 01:07:41are like kind of taking out here that I
  • 01:07:43think does paint a more complex picture,
  • 01:07:45but I think for a lot of these
  • 01:07:47things that would say OK,
  • 01:07:48this is definitely not our PE.
  • 01:07:50The baseline is not the factor
  • 01:07:52that's driving all of it,
  • 01:07:53but I think that's something actually
  • 01:07:54that people should be thinking
  • 01:07:56about in these papers and everyone
  • 01:07:57does imaging now and they're all
  • 01:07:59doing everything is baseline,
  • 01:08:00But there's like slower changes
  • 01:08:01that are totally left out of that.
  • 01:08:03They're going to change the way
  • 01:08:05you interpret the data.
  • 01:08:06But that's I mean it's a great question.
  • 01:08:08It will look into this more
  • 01:08:09'cause I think maybe it does.
  • 01:08:11All of that is totally reasonable.
  • 01:08:13Explanation and it might be like nice at
  • 01:08:15a different synapses and the same area,
  • 01:08:16and so like how to parse those.
  • 01:08:18I think it's important.
  • 01:08:21Last question from Rick.
  • 01:08:23The outstanding thank you.
  • 01:08:25Very much so this is I'm not
  • 01:08:27sure exactly this question,
  • 01:08:28but since there's so much regulation
  • 01:08:30and release at terminals Strike and dip
  • 01:08:33compared to mean cell body activity,
  • 01:08:34do you think that other neurotransmitters
  • 01:08:36responsible for the specific
  • 01:08:38components of your learning model
  • 01:08:39and like dopamine release the end
  • 01:08:41result and integration of those?
  • 01:08:43And can you speak this one?
  • 01:08:45I can think of right off the bat.
  • 01:08:47So one thing we think is really
  • 01:08:49important for this is acetylcholine
  • 01:08:52regulation of dopamine release.
  • 01:08:54We we have started to do some of this
  • 01:08:56where we're starting on the slice level
  • 01:08:58trying to workout these parameters
  • 01:09:00because it's like calling regulation
  • 01:09:02of domain releases actually really
  • 01:09:04kind of cool regulatory mechanism
  • 01:09:06because it's one of the few that really
  • 01:09:09robustly releases domain from terminals,
  • 01:09:11totally independent of the semantic activity.
  • 01:09:13And you can even get this in
  • 01:09:15isolated terminals and slices.
  • 01:09:17And so we think that there is probably,
  • 01:09:19you know, maybe it's the.
  • 01:09:21Maybe it's both questions.
  • 01:09:23Maybe Domain was released.
  • 01:09:24In response to RP signals,
  • 01:09:26but also attentional signals
  • 01:09:27that are coming from these,
  • 01:09:29maybe like that's the first thing I think
  • 01:09:32of acetylcholine is like attention arousal,
  • 01:09:34but here's the thing.
  • 01:09:36Those are all integrated.
  • 01:09:37What the domain signature finally is,
  • 01:09:39and so I think that it is
  • 01:09:41probably both in some contexts,
  • 01:09:43right?
  • 01:09:44It's probably both acetylcholine
  • 01:09:45regulated domain release and RPE
  • 01:09:47regulated cymatic activity that leads
  • 01:09:49to this kind of saliency based term.
  • 01:09:51One thing I'll tell you some
  • 01:09:53ants about it as we see pretty
  • 01:09:56interesting sex differences.
  • 01:09:57In terminal regulatory
  • 01:09:58mechanisms through the A4 beta,
  • 01:10:00two containing nicotinic receptors
  • 01:10:02that are on those terminals,
  • 01:10:04and So what we're trying to do first
  • 01:10:06is outline the mechanism or how this
  • 01:10:09is different and then take that
  • 01:10:11model into this and say do those
  • 01:10:14differences predict differences
  • 01:10:15in these learning parameters
  • 01:10:16because it's really hard to get it.
  • 01:10:19Don't mean terminal regulation by
  • 01:10:21acetylcholine in vivo because it's
  • 01:10:22your calling regulates other cell
  • 01:10:24types that also regulate dopamine release.
  • 01:10:26So it regulates GABA release.
  • 01:10:28That regulates terminals and so,
  • 01:10:30like without some sort of like biological
  • 01:10:33system that you know is different.
  • 01:10:35It's really hard to isolate just
  • 01:10:37the effects of cetyl choline from
  • 01:10:40the effects of acetylcholine media
  • 01:10:43dopamine terminal regulation.
  • 01:10:44I hope tools come along.
  • 01:10:45There's like darts and stuff
  • 01:10:47which are awesome.
  • 01:10:48Those are coming and like we'll see.
  • 01:10:49But like anyway I'm like this is a that's
  • 01:10:51a great question I'm excited about so.
  • 01:10:55Well thank you were coming up to 11:30
  • 01:10:57and we peppered you with questions
  • 01:10:59we really appreciate the great talk
  • 01:11:01and the time that you spent with us.
  • 01:11:03Thank you to everyone for your attention
  • 01:11:05and we hope to hear more about the
  • 01:11:08next steps in the research. Yeah,
  • 01:11:09thank you guys so much.
  • 01:11:11These questions were awesome man.
  • 01:11:12I had a great time
  • 01:11:14chatting with everybody so
  • 01:11:15thanks. Thanks Erin.