Yale Psychiatry Grand Rounds: "Brain Dynamics and Flexible Behaviors"
May 17, 2024May 17, 2024
Ribicoff Lecture. "Brain Dynamics and Flexible Behaviors"
Speaker: Lucina Uddin, PhD, UCLA Brain Research Institute
Information
- ID
- 11688
- To Cite
- DCA Citation Guide
Transcript
- 00:00This really kind introduction and for
- 00:02the invitation to visit here it's I will
- 00:05say it's it's always fun to visit Yale.
- 00:08The the first time I came here
- 00:09was in 2012 and then I was here
- 00:12again in 2018 and now it's 2024.
- 00:15So I hope to see you all again in
- 00:172030 because it seems like every six
- 00:19years I get the opportunity to come
- 00:21and and and meet and just benefit from
- 00:24these wonderful environment in the
- 00:27outstanding discussions and just feel
- 00:29the collegiality every time I'm here.
- 00:31So it's really nice to be back.
- 00:34Thank you again, Denise,
- 00:35for this wonderful introduction.
- 00:36Thank you, John and and everyone
- 00:38else for for the warm welcome.
- 00:40Now I suppose I need to figure out
- 00:42where the slide advancement is.
- 00:43So. Oh, here we go. OK All right.
- 00:47So it's just it's an honor to be
- 00:51here following up this introduction
- 00:53of of RIBICOFF.
- 00:55So I'll talk to you today about our
- 00:58work on brain dynamics and flexible
- 01:01behaviors and I think it's it's of
- 01:03interest for our lab but I think
- 01:05for a lot of people in psychiatry
- 01:07to think about flexibility.
- 01:09We are we focus a lot on children
- 01:11with autism in our own research and in
- 01:14this particular clinical condition we
- 01:16often see this insistence on sameness,
- 01:18behavioral inflexibility.
- 01:19For example,
- 01:20a child might want to wear a particular
- 01:23pair of socks or eat as only yellow
- 01:25foods or you know these kinds of
- 01:28repetitive behaviors that sometimes can,
- 01:30you know this insistence on sameness
- 01:32can sometimes cause challenges
- 01:34for caregivers and and difficulty
- 01:36in in day-to-day life activities.
- 01:39And so I used to kind of introduce
- 01:40my talk saying you know flexibility
- 01:42is important.
- 01:43But during the pandemic I think
- 01:44we all had a a big dose of what
- 01:47it means to be flexible.
- 01:48So we went from our everyday lives and and
- 01:51routines to something completely different.
- 01:53We started giving talks over zoom instead of
- 01:57giving them at at lecture podiums like this.
- 02:00So I'm myself readjusting
- 02:02to being standing up again.
- 02:06So.
- 02:06So we've all sort of experienced this
- 02:08like how changes and and be having
- 02:11difficulties with flexible behaviors can
- 02:12kind of impact our our day-to-day lives.
- 02:15So we try to focus on the lab on
- 02:17thinking about the neural basis of
- 02:19flexibility and we do kind of take a
- 02:22developmental look at these questions.
- 02:23So we think about how brain networks
- 02:26develop and mature in ways that can
- 02:28support increasingly sophisticated
- 02:30types of cognition like flexibility
- 02:32across the lifespan.
- 02:33So we think about how brain
- 02:35networks develop from childhood,
- 02:36adolescence into adulthood,
- 02:38how that underlies cognitive development.
- 02:40And you know basically how can compromised
- 02:43connectivity within and between large
- 02:46scale brain networks be related to
- 02:48developmental neuropathologies.
- 02:49And we try to think about how we
- 02:53can use this basic information
- 02:55about brain structure and function
- 02:58to inform diagnosis and design
- 03:00interventions ultimately.
- 03:01And so I'm really excited to be here in
- 03:04the in the Ribicoff series because
- 03:06we do try to do in our lab a sort of
- 03:10combination of basic neuroscience
- 03:12developmental work and clinical clinically
- 03:14translatable kind of research in the
- 03:17same umbrella or under the same lab.
- 03:20So we use structural and
- 03:22functional neuroimaging.
- 03:23So that includes task functional
- 03:25magnetic resonance imaging,
- 03:27resting state fMRI,
- 03:28diffusion weighted imaging to look
- 03:30at structural connections and also
- 03:32some causal modeling approaches
- 03:34to look at relationships among and
- 03:36between brain regions as they relate
- 03:39to things like executive function,
- 03:40flexible behaviors.
- 03:41And although we we love neuroimaging,
- 03:44we're a network neuroscience lab,
- 03:46we loved all this sort of cutting
- 03:47edge tools that are now available.
- 03:49I think it's important to remember
- 03:51how much of this work just dates back
- 03:54to before we had these fancy tools.
- 03:57So the foundations of what we now
- 03:59call network neuroscience were
- 04:00planted a long time ago.
- 04:02You know before MRI was really
- 04:04very much in vogue.
- 04:05I I love some of these papers from
- 04:08Marcel Maslam who is who has you know
- 04:10is a is a neurologist who were and
- 04:13a neuropsychologist who had these
- 04:16great ideas about brain function.
- 04:18That that really came from looking
- 04:20at patients and lesions and figuring
- 04:22out what the deficits were in
- 04:25in patients with focal lesions.
- 04:27And the interesting conclusions
- 04:28that are nicely summarized in one
- 04:31of his Seminole papers.
- 04:32Or that cognition is served by
- 04:35interconnected neural networks and
- 04:37any complex behaviour is mapped at
- 04:39the level of multifocal neural systems
- 04:41rather than specific anatomical sites.
- 04:43And I, I love these these papers,
- 04:46They're kind of just a history of how
- 04:48the brain works and they really hold up.
- 04:50I think to this day,
- 04:51if you read this,
- 04:51you'll say, hey everything.
- 04:52And he was right.
- 04:54He was right about everything.
- 04:56And I think this is interesting because
- 04:58there was the trend at one point to say,
- 05:00well, OK,
- 05:00we have a lesion to this brain
- 05:01area and that causes this deficit,
- 05:03that brain,
- 05:03you know,
- 05:04there must be a one to one mapping
- 05:06between that function and that structure.
- 05:07But even you can use the same data to
- 05:09make the opposite conclusion is that,
- 05:11you know,
- 05:12brain regions can be relatively
- 05:14specialized and not sort of
- 05:16specifically tied to to one function.
- 05:18And so I think this is some
- 05:21really nice foundational work.
- 05:22There's also one of my favorite papers
- 05:25is from Randy McIntosh in 2004 where
- 05:27he introduced the concept of neural context.
- 05:30The idea that relevant functional
- 05:32relevance of any given brain area
- 05:34depends on the status of other
- 05:36connected areas to that brain region.
- 05:38So kind of not just focusing on the one,
- 05:41one or two areas at a time,
- 05:42but the whole kind of context
- 05:44in which brain function is,
- 05:45is occurring.
- 05:47And we also think a lot
- 05:48nowadays about the time domain,
- 05:50so about how networks need to be
- 05:52understood in terms of interactions
- 05:54as they unfold temporally between
- 05:56between multiple brain regions
- 05:57as they unfold temporarily.
- 05:59A nice paper from Luis Pasoa 10 years
- 06:02ago really drives this point home.
- 06:05So there are some things that many
- 06:07of you are very familiar with.
- 06:09And so back in 1995,
- 06:12Broad Bizwell published a paper
- 06:14showing that you can find these
- 06:16coherent spontaneous fluctuations
- 06:17in different parts of the brain.
- 06:19You focused on the motor cortex.
- 06:21But since that 1995 paper there have been,
- 06:24you know, thousands of studies showing
- 06:26that at this very low frequency the brain
- 06:29seems to recapitulate in the resting state.
- 06:32All of the the brain networks that we
- 06:34can see engaged in tasks like memory,
- 06:36attention, vision, motor processing.
- 06:38At these point O1 to .1 Hertz
- 06:42is low frequency.
- 06:43We can see these oscillations
- 06:45spontaneously occurring.
- 06:46And this is just a ongoing,
- 06:49you know,
- 06:49fMRI of somebody doing nothing
- 06:50at all in the scanner,
- 06:51just kind of asked to just lay still.
- 06:54But it's not sort of random
- 06:56spontaneous activity,
- 06:57but really coherent in these systems
- 06:59that we've been studying for many years.
- 07:01If you wait around a while,
- 07:02you'll see a language network or a vision,
- 07:04a visual cortices.
- 07:06So these kind of spontaneous fluctuations,
- 07:09many labs have been exploiting now for
- 07:11a number of years to try to understand
- 07:13the functional organization of the brain.
- 07:17And as I,
- 07:17as I mentioned before,
- 07:18we all know what flexibility is
- 07:20and we've had to be very flexible
- 07:22across this pandemic period.
- 07:24During that period,
- 07:24I had the time to sit down and
- 07:27write a bunch of invited reviews.
- 07:29So that was my my COVID,
- 07:31my my pandemic was review writing.
- 07:33But it gave me time to really think about
- 07:35what we mean when we study flexibility,
- 07:37what we're what we're using
- 07:38that term to refer to.
- 07:40And in in human sort of cognitive
- 07:43neuroscience and psychology we tend
- 07:44to use things like the Wisconsin
- 07:46cards sort tasks and we ask people
- 07:49to make categorizations and then you
- 07:51know switch the criteria and and we
- 07:53use that as an index of flexibility.
- 07:55But I also think we have all these
- 07:57parallels in behavioral neuroscience.
- 08:00In reversal learning paradigms for
- 08:01example you're just asking animals
- 08:03to make different kinds of stimulus
- 08:05outcome mappings and we we call
- 08:07that behavioral flexibility and
- 08:08I think mainly because we can't
- 08:10guess what animals are thinking.
- 08:12So we have them do a behavior and
- 08:14we see how how they flexibly behave.
- 08:17When we look at developmental psychology,
- 08:19we often come up with things
- 08:21that kids can do like,
- 08:23you know,
- 08:23tell me you know which color is this fish
- 08:25or which direction is the fish pointing.
- 08:26But we do things like task switching
- 08:28to engage the development of
- 08:30flexible behaviors in in children.
- 08:32But all of these kinds of paradigms tend
- 08:36to engage frontal parietal networks.
- 08:38They tend to engage singular insular
- 08:41cortices and frontostriatal systems and
- 08:43and many of you have been, you know,
- 08:47working in this field for years.
- 08:48So I'm not telling you anything new.
- 08:50What I do think is interesting
- 08:52about flexibility is that
- 08:53you can see difficulties,
- 08:54oops, across many different
- 08:56kinds of clinical conditions.
- 08:58So we focus a lot on autism and early
- 09:00life conditions like ADHD where you get
- 09:02things like and autism specifically,
- 09:04you get restricted and repetitive behaviors.
- 09:06But then you have things
- 09:07like anxiety and depression,
- 09:08which come along more during
- 09:10adolescence that are associated
- 09:11with repetitive negative thinking.
- 09:13Worry in the case of anxiety or
- 09:15rumination in the case of depression
- 09:17are kind of inflexible thought patterns.
- 09:19So it's another way of thinking about
- 09:22flexibility and and difficulties
- 09:24with flexibility.
- 09:24And of course,
- 09:26OCD is another great example of
- 09:28a case where you really have
- 09:30difficulty coming out of a routine.
- 09:32And then even later in life,
- 09:33things like Alzheimer's and
- 09:35and Parkinson's are associated
- 09:36with cognitive rigidity.
- 09:38So, you know,
- 09:38these are maybe not all the same thing
- 09:40we're seeing at these different stages,
- 09:42but there's something about getting
- 09:44stuck in a particular behavioral
- 09:46pattern or thought pattern that we
- 09:48can see perhaps as a, you know,
- 09:50transdiagnostic, you know, difficulty.
- 09:52And so for us, we think,
- 09:55well, let's try to figure out,
- 09:56you know,
- 09:56what in the brain is allowing
- 09:58flexibility in the first place.
- 10:00And so a lot of the work that
- 10:02I won't talk about today is on
- 10:03thinking about development of brain
- 10:05networks involved in flexibility.
- 10:06So we'll do things like look at MRI
- 10:09data collected from participants
- 10:10between the age of 6 and 85 S really
- 10:13kind of lifespan data sets and
- 10:15look at changes in brain signals
- 10:17as they associate with, you know,
- 10:19like flexibility of brain
- 10:20networks across the lifespan.
- 10:22We'll look at things like differences
- 10:24in children versus adults in the
- 10:26strength of certain circuits or the
- 10:28effective connections between brain
- 10:29regions and how they change with development.
- 10:31And we'll get how network interactions
- 10:33again change across the lifespan,
- 10:35for example,
- 10:36between age of 6 and 85.
- 10:38There's a nice Nathan Klein
- 10:40Institute data set that that
- 10:42provides a lot of these data and
- 10:44they're publicly available now.
- 10:45So lots of things are are you know
- 10:48analysis are available and we're
- 10:50we've we've shown kind of how these
- 10:52differences in brain organization
- 10:54across the lifespan differentially
- 10:56relate to executive function.
- 10:58So like some of these brain variability
- 11:01metrics you know are associated
- 11:03with better executive function and
- 11:05adolescence but worse executive
- 11:06function and old age for example.
- 11:08And I know you can't see that slide,
- 11:10so I apologize.
- 11:11I'll make it bigger next time, I promise.
- 11:13But what we do do in this scanner
- 11:16is sometimes we just test.
- 11:18You've tried to develop a paradigm
- 11:19that can be used across children,
- 11:21across adults, across clinical populations.
- 11:24And we took this one paradigm from the
- 11:27developmental psychology literature
- 11:28called the flexible item selection task.
- 11:30And what you do here is you ask
- 11:32somebody to pick two things that go
- 11:34together and they might say, well,
- 11:35the 2nd and the 4th card are both blue,
- 11:38so they go together on color.
- 11:40And then you might say, OK,
- 11:41now pick another two things that go
- 11:42together and they might say, well,
- 11:44the 3rd and the 4th are both rabbits.
- 11:46I think that's right. And so it's OK.
- 11:48Pick another set of things
- 11:49that go together and say, well,
- 11:50the 1st and the 4th, they both have one
- 11:52item so that they're both showing one.
- 11:54So that's something that goes together.
- 11:56And this requires, you know,
- 11:57working memory, inhibition,
- 11:59all of these processes, attention,
- 12:00but also of course flexibility.
- 12:02The further you get along these dimensions,
- 12:04the harder it is to come up
- 12:06with things that go together.
- 12:07So we tried to put this in
- 12:09the scanner and we said, OK,
- 12:10here's just a control version of it,
- 12:12just hit the two buttons that
- 12:13are highlighted.
- 12:14So OK, just follow along.
- 12:16So we spent some years validating this task,
- 12:20having adults do it,
- 12:21putting people in the scanners,
- 12:22looking at their accuracy
- 12:24inside of the scanner,
- 12:26outside the scanner,
- 12:27all the typical validation stuff come by,
- 12:30created an efficiency metric to look at
- 12:32how good people are at this task and
- 12:34how much better they get at it over time.
- 12:36So Dejani did this work in our lab
- 12:39and found as one might expect really
- 12:42robust brain activation and flexibility
- 12:45trials in lateral prefrontal cortex,
- 12:48parietal cortices,
- 12:49cerebellum and basal ganglia and the
- 12:52anterior cingulate and the anterior
- 12:55ansula favorite brain region of mine
- 12:58that will come up again and really,
- 13:01really strong and robust
- 13:02activation across these regions.
- 13:04So,
- 13:04so we know as as from lots of
- 13:06literature that these are all
- 13:08very much involved in in flexible
- 13:09thinking and flexible behaviors.
- 13:11What I think is interesting to
- 13:13note though is for those fans
- 13:15of neurosynth and meta analysis.
- 13:17If you look at if you put a term
- 13:19like shifting or working memory
- 13:20for example into the neurosynth to
- 13:22look at automated across studies,
- 13:24what are the brain regions that are
- 13:26involved in shifting or I'm sorry,
- 13:28flexibility, you'll see these regions,
- 13:30right, the the same ones I just mentioned,
- 13:32frontal parietal cingulance or
- 13:33if you go into neurosynth,
- 13:35then you type in updating or
- 13:37you know working memory,
- 13:38whatever phrase you want to use,
- 13:40you'll see similar kinds of activation.
- 13:43And then if you type in inhibition
- 13:44again you'll see a lot of these
- 13:46overlapping brain regions.
- 13:47So they're really kind of broadly
- 13:50involved in these different
- 13:51components of executive function.
- 13:53It's hard to,
- 13:54I guess the point is it's hard to
- 13:57pull out of flexibility specific
- 13:59activations when almost all the
- 14:01tasks that try to tap flexible
- 14:03thinking also involve attention,
- 14:04working memory, inhibition and you know,
- 14:06everything else.
- 14:07So it's it's all not as clean as,
- 14:10as you know,
- 14:11we like to pretend,
- 14:12but you can do things like there's
- 14:14connectivity modelling approaches that
- 14:16let you get at some of these questions.
- 14:18One of them we've worked on
- 14:20here with Katie Gates at UNC,
- 14:22it's called group iterative
- 14:24multiple model estimation or Gimme.
- 14:26It's kind of a an iterative search
- 14:28algorithm that tries to fit a
- 14:30structural equation model to describe
- 14:32an individual connectome using user
- 14:34specified regions of interest.
- 14:36So if you take for example of all
- 14:38of these nodes that are activated
- 14:40in the flexible item selection task,
- 14:42you can look at OK,
- 14:43which sort of nodes kind of are most
- 14:46directly activated by this paradigm and
- 14:48which are sort of secondarily activated.
- 14:51So here we found that inferior
- 14:53frontal junction is sort of directly
- 14:56activated by flexible thinking
- 14:57or flexible item selection.
- 14:59And then there's information flow
- 15:01to other regions including the
- 15:03dorsilateral prefrontal cortex,
- 15:05anterior cingulate and others.
- 15:07And so there's kind of some regions
- 15:10that are more influential than others
- 15:12in this in this type of flexibility.
- 15:15So there's kind of ways of of teasing
- 15:18apart some of these activations and
- 15:20looking at more specific information flow.
- 15:22There's also a great deal of
- 15:24individual variability,
- 15:25like even though all subjects tend to
- 15:28activate these brain nodes during this test,
- 15:30there's group level paths or
- 15:32connections that are consistent,
- 15:34but a lot of sub sub sub group level
- 15:38paths meaning like some subjects engage
- 15:40these connections and others don't.
- 15:42So there's a lot of kind of more nuance
- 15:46to these blobs and activation patterns
- 15:49than we might initially realize.
- 15:51So we think about,
- 15:53for us at least,
- 15:54how cognitive flexibility involves
- 15:56the coordination among multiple
- 15:57brain regions that are all known to
- 15:59play a role in executive function,
- 16:01adults and children.
- 16:02I didn't show the maps from children,
- 16:04but children between the ages of
- 16:058 to 12 also show these kind of
- 16:08similar activations of lateral
- 16:10frontoparrietal and singular insular
- 16:12networks during flexible item selection
- 16:14and task modulated connectivity.
- 16:15The inferior frontal junction seems
- 16:17to be particularly important for
- 16:19this type of flexible behavior.
- 16:21And I won't,
- 16:22I'll show you at the end of the
- 16:23talk some of the work we're doing
- 16:25now to bring this task to kids with
- 16:27autism and look at the brains of
- 16:28children between the ages of 8:00
- 16:30and 12:00 as they're doing this
- 16:32type of of flexible behavior task.
- 16:34And we, well, I'll show you,
- 16:36I don't remember if I included
- 16:38this slide but I'll I'll come back
- 16:40to this towards the end.
- 16:41So as we kind of develop these tasks,
- 16:44try to use them in clinical populations
- 16:47and developmental populations at the
- 16:49same time we try to focus on these
- 16:51big data sets that can help us look
- 16:53at sort of the adults neurotypical brain.
- 16:56And then the nice thing about some
- 16:58of these publicly shared large
- 16:59data sets is they're, you know,
- 17:01hundreds of subjects large.
- 17:02There are lots more data points
- 17:04than we typically can collect
- 17:06in the clinical population.
- 17:07So we can do a lot more with the
- 17:10the methods and the inferences and
- 17:12the replication here because for
- 17:14example in human connection project
- 17:15you actually have one hour of resting
- 17:17state F MRI data from each person.
- 17:20So a lot more,
- 17:21you know, signal in there so we can
- 17:25do things like look at moment to
- 17:26moment functional connectivity or
- 17:28dynamic functional connectivity.
- 17:29So in this process,
- 17:30instead of saying what areas are
- 17:32connected to each other on average,
- 17:34we're saying if you break down
- 17:36this data into 45 second chunks
- 17:39or 62nd windows and you look at
- 17:42reoccurring patterns of whole brain
- 17:43functional connectivity and you use
- 17:44some kind of clustering to say, OK,
- 17:46here's some different brain states.
- 17:48You can then start to quantify
- 17:50dynamic metrics like the frequency
- 17:52of occurrence of a particular
- 17:53brain state or the dwell time,
- 17:55which means how long does this
- 17:57brain state persist once it comes
- 17:59along and the state transitions,
- 18:01how much switching between
- 18:02brain states can you quantify?
- 18:04So in the human connection project is
- 18:06that this is just showing you a couple
- 18:08100 subjects do split half replication,
- 18:10various things.
- 18:11You can use independent component analysis
- 18:13to breakdown the brain into little regions.
- 18:16And then the nice thing is that
- 18:17all there's all kinds of other
- 18:19data on these same subjects,
- 18:21information about their processing speed,
- 18:23inhibition, cognitive flexibility,
- 18:24fluid intelligence,
- 18:25working memory,
- 18:26lots of scores to play around with.
- 18:29And So what we found is that typically
- 18:31the the brain enters different states,
- 18:34but it's got most of its time,
- 18:35about 36% of its time in this loosely
- 18:39connected kind of flexible state.
- 18:41And about 10% of the time it's really
- 18:43in this more cohere or really tightly
- 18:46correlated state that you see on the right.
- 18:49And it turns out that those individuals
- 18:51who do better on tests of working memory
- 18:53and tests of cognitive flexibility
- 18:55like the Wisconsin Card sort of test,
- 18:57those are the ones who are spending
- 18:58sort of or showing more frequent
- 19:00occurrence of states one and two,
- 19:01the loose connectivity states with
- 19:03more variability actually in their
- 19:06connection patterns and less time in the
- 19:08tight sort of inflexible states on the right.
- 19:11And so this is just,
- 19:13you know,
- 19:14showing us how we can get at sort of
- 19:16the basic neuroscience of of brain
- 19:18flexibility using these large data sets.
- 19:21And here we're just showing an example
- 19:22of how greater cognitive flexibility,
- 19:24which is measured outside of
- 19:25the scanner in this case,
- 19:26are associated with the propensity to
- 19:29occupy these more frequently occurring
- 19:31brain configurations characterized by
- 19:33attenuated correlations and greater
- 19:35functional connectivity variability.
- 19:37And it's just showing how we can
- 19:39think about using dynamic functional
- 19:42connectivity approaches to reveal
- 19:44relationships between brain
- 19:46dynamics and flexible cognition.
- 19:48So,
- 19:48OK,
- 19:48So what I talked about in the last
- 19:51study was really a whole brain kind of
- 19:55agnostic approach to thinking about the
- 19:58how flexibility might be implemented.
- 20:01But there's also of course,
- 20:02decades of cognitive neuroscience
- 20:04literature that that we all sort of
- 20:07rely on to to think about what are the
- 20:10more specific brain networks that might
- 20:11be involved in some of these processes.
- 20:13I already sort of mentioned
- 20:15the central executive or sort
- 20:17of lateral frontal parietal systems,
- 20:19the salience or mid single
- 20:21insular systems anchored in the
- 20:23cingulate and insular cortex,
- 20:25and the default mode network which has
- 20:27key nodes in the medial prefrontal
- 20:29and posterior parietal cortices.
- 20:31A number of years ago,
- 20:32we started to notice some interesting
- 20:34patterns of interrelationships
- 20:35among these three networks.
- 20:37They show up a lot in cognitive neuroscience.
- 20:39They show up a lot in psychiatry.
- 20:40But it turns out there's
- 20:42interrelatedness between them, right?
- 20:44So you can have usually when signals
- 20:46in the default mode network go up,
- 20:48the lateral front file goes
- 20:50down and vice versa.
- 20:51But it turns out you can often
- 20:53predict what's going to happen in
- 20:55these networks based on signals
- 20:57from the anterior insular cortex
- 20:58which we think of almost as a
- 21:00causal outflow hub like driving or
- 21:03orchestrating the changes between
- 21:04these other large scale networks.
- 21:06So it's a a model we've been playing
- 21:08around with now for a number of years.
- 21:10And if we think about the development
- 21:13of some of these networks and
- 21:15development of cognitive flexibility,
- 21:16again I mentioned this nice data
- 21:18set that includes several 100
- 21:20participants between 6 and 85
- 21:22resting to data from Rai data.
- 21:23And we can actually look at relationships
- 21:26between their brain dynamics and some
- 21:28other executive function measures.
- 21:29In this particular data set,
- 21:31the Dallas Kaplan executive
- 21:33function test was conducted.
- 21:35So participants are kind of going
- 21:37from letters to numbers in sequential
- 21:40order and we were interested here in
- 21:42the relationship between the brain
- 21:44dynamics of those three systems I
- 21:46mentioned and performance on this task.
- 21:48And so we used a different instead
- 21:50of sliding window,
- 21:51we used a Co activation pattern
- 21:53analysis approach,
- 21:54which also allows you to kind
- 21:56of cluster time frames based on
- 21:58their spatial similarity and look
- 22:00at functionally relevant patterns
- 22:02at the whole brain,
- 22:04not using a sliding window.
- 22:05But you can still get things like dwell
- 22:07time transitions between States and
- 22:09frequency of occurrence of states.
- 22:11And we found for example that there's
- 22:13some patterns like involving the
- 22:15lateral front to parietal executive
- 22:17and the medial front to parietal
- 22:19default mode that show AU shaped kind
- 22:21of trajectory across the lifespan.
- 22:23So you see kind of Co activation of
- 22:26these networks a lot more in midlife
- 22:28than in young individuals or in older age.
- 22:30And also this transitions or the
- 22:32switching between brain states
- 22:34seems to be linked with individual
- 22:36differences in the behavior on
- 22:38that executive function test.
- 22:39So in in middle age essentially you
- 22:42know between 25 and 45 or thereabouts,
- 22:45you actually you have pretty high
- 22:48flexibility performance regardless
- 22:49of what's happening in terms of
- 22:51the brain state transitions.
- 22:53But in children and in older adults,
- 22:55greater number of transitions between
- 22:57brain states is associated with better
- 22:59levels of cognitive flexibility.
- 23:01So there's like really different
- 23:02things going on at different points
- 23:04in life where at some some junctures
- 23:06it's really important to have
- 23:07a lot more of these brain state
- 23:09transitions to support flexible behaviors.
- 23:11But at other points they're not really as
- 23:15dependent on this type of brain flexibility.
- 23:19So this is an example where we used Co
- 23:22activation patterns to show that executive
- 23:25and default networks change in terms of
- 23:28their representation across the lifespan,
- 23:30in terms of their frequency of of occurrence.
- 23:33And that the brain state transitions
- 23:35between these networks seem to be related
- 23:38to cognitive flexibility in different
- 23:40ways at different stages of the lifespan.
- 23:42So if you if you don't mind now
- 23:44we'll go straight basic neuroscience
- 23:46for a little while.
- 23:47Because I think part of you know
- 23:49when you have these findings
- 23:50you see different developmental
- 23:52differences or clinical differences.
- 23:53It's tempting to, you know,
- 23:55go forth and, you know,
- 23:58make inferences and go straight
- 23:59to intervention or, you know,
- 24:01whatever the case may be.
- 24:02But I also think it's it's nice
- 24:03to to take a step back and say,
- 24:05OK, what are these brain regions
- 24:06doing in a broader context?
- 24:08What do we know about the anatomy
- 24:10and function of some of these areas
- 24:12that we seem to be implicated,
- 24:14you know, trans diagnostically
- 24:17across flexibility deficits.
- 24:19So the ancillar cortex in particular
- 24:21has kind of caught my attention.
- 24:23If you look at the, you know,
- 24:25neuroimaging literature,
- 24:26it often shows up in studies of affect,
- 24:29empathy, pain, emotion.
- 24:30You'll see often a talk of an empathy
- 24:33network that involves these brain regions.
- 24:36You'll also see a subject of awareness,
- 24:38Introception,
- 24:38somatic sensory processes.
- 24:40I I forgot to put disgust in here.
- 24:43But you know a lot of things happen
- 24:45in the insular cortex that are a lot
- 24:47to do with basic sensory processing,
- 24:49but also very high level cognitive
- 24:53processing, things like inhibition,
- 24:55attention switching and conflict
- 24:57executive function as we've been
- 24:59talking about all of those processes
- 25:01also activate the insular cortex.
- 25:02So it's it's one of those areas
- 25:05where everybody has their favorite
- 25:06thing to say about it.
- 25:08And so there,
- 25:09there tends to be a lot of literature
- 25:11that doesn't really talk to each
- 25:12other when we talk about the insular
- 25:14cortex because it's a little bit
- 25:16siloed in these different fields.
- 25:19But if you look at just a question of like,
- 25:21are there subdivisions within the insula?
- 25:23There's anywhere between 2 and 27
- 25:25depending on what study you look at.
- 25:27But it's true,
- 25:28there's a lot of subdivisions in the insula.
- 25:31If you look at just resting state
- 25:33F MRI and ask the question which
- 25:35voxels in the insular cortex
- 25:36have similar patterns of whole
- 25:38brain functional connectivity.
- 25:39So you're clustering the voxels
- 25:40based on their whole brain patterns.
- 25:42There's two studies from Bendine
- 25:44and Luke Chang that suggest you
- 25:46can at least find evidence for a
- 25:48dorsal anterior insula subdivision,
- 25:49a ventral anterior insula,
- 25:51a posterior insula.
- 25:52Like I said,
- 25:53maybe up to 27 depending on
- 25:55which Atlas you look at.
- 25:56So right now let's go with three for now.
- 26:00So if you look at these subdivisions there,
- 26:02they do seem to be some kind of
- 26:05structure to their their their
- 26:07division of of Labor in the sense.
- 26:10So in Luke Chang's meta analysis he
- 26:13found that the ventral anterior insula
- 26:15seems to be more involved in studies
- 26:17in the using the terms that are in red.
- 26:20So emotion, face,
- 26:21anxiety sort of affective types
- 26:23of terminology tend to go along
- 26:25with the ventral anterior insula
- 26:27in terms of activation.
- 26:28On the right is a meta analytic
- 26:31connectivity modeling study that
- 26:32we conducted some 10 years ago,
- 26:34which was just asking the question
- 26:36across many, many F MRI studies.
- 26:38When you see ventral anterior insula active,
- 26:40what else in the brain tends to
- 26:43coactive coactivate with that?
- 26:44It does tend to be the sort of limbic
- 26:46kinds of regions that are coactive.
- 26:49If you go ahead then to look at
- 26:51dorsal anterior insula,
- 26:51that's the one that seems to show up
- 26:54in studies with the term switching
- 26:56error processing inhibition.
- 26:58Those are the sort of higher cognitive
- 27:01executive function types of terms.
- 27:03And if you look on the right,
- 27:05that's the coactivation map of dorsal,
- 27:07anterior and slow which tends
- 27:08to coactivate with frontal,
- 27:09parietal cortices and temporal as well.
- 27:12And then if you go back a little
- 27:14bit to the posterior insula,
- 27:15that tends to be the one for pain,
- 27:17somatosensory,
- 27:18that kind of more sensory based
- 27:21kind of cognition.
- 27:22And it also seems to coactivate
- 27:25with somatosensory cortices.
- 27:26So on first glance,
- 27:28there are the patterns of
- 27:30coactivation and meta analytic types
- 27:33of analysis give us some evidence
- 27:35that the insular cortex can be
- 27:37subdivided a little bit even still,
- 27:39like all of these subdivisions are
- 27:40actually active across all of these
- 27:42cognitive domains I mentioned.
- 27:43So there's there's kind of a convergence
- 27:46and divergent at the same time.
- 27:48If you go ahead and look at
- 27:50the dynamics of the functional
- 27:51connectivity of these subdivisions,
- 27:53there's four here instead of three.
- 27:55It's independent component analysis,
- 27:56it's not too important.
- 27:58But if you again if you look
- 27:59at the dorsal anterior insula,
- 28:01it's doing something different here.
- 28:02That's the one in red in this top figure.
- 28:05And if you look at the pattern of
- 28:07the these subdivisions and how they
- 28:09connect with other parts of the brain,
- 28:11there's sometimes this is
- 28:12resting state up from rye.
- 28:13Again the there's some states in which
- 28:15all of the subdivisions are very
- 28:17similar like state three in the middle.
- 28:19But other states like state 2
- 28:21which only occur 5% of the time,
- 28:23which are much more divergent,
- 28:25which means the insular subdivisions
- 28:26are acting differently during that
- 28:28particular connectivity state.
- 28:30And the dorsal anterior insula is
- 28:32the most functionally flexible
- 28:33of all these subdivisions.
- 28:34So that's the one where if you
- 28:36look at different states,
- 28:37it's interacting with different brain region,
- 28:39has more connectivity partners
- 28:40with the rest of the cortex than
- 28:43the other subdivisions.
- 28:44And that's actually the same
- 28:46dorsal anterior insula as that
- 28:47same subdivision where we found
- 28:48that it you can use
- 28:50signals from that area to estimate
- 28:52what's going to happen at later time
- 28:53points in other parts of the brain.
- 28:55So that's that causal outflow hub that
- 28:57we identified many years ago using
- 29:00things like Granger causal analysis.
- 29:01So there's something curious
- 29:03about the dorsal anterior insula.
- 29:04I'll leave it at that.
- 29:07And again, there's some interesting
- 29:08structural connections there that you
- 29:10don't see in other parts of the insulin.
- 29:11There's some frontal and and subcortical
- 29:14projections that are detectable from
- 29:16the dorsal anterior insula that we
- 29:19don't see for the other subdivisions.
- 29:21So I I wanted to take that little segue
- 29:24into anatomy and and you know just
- 29:26the basic architecture of some brain
- 29:28areas because sometimes I think we'll
- 29:31see something like insula activation,
- 29:32but we may not look very carefully at which
- 29:35subdivision right or left what you know,
- 29:37what are we actually talking about.
- 29:38So I think being a little more precise
- 29:41on on where these things are going on
- 29:43really helps us you know hone in on
- 29:46what what the functional or dysfunction
- 29:48might be in a particular population.
- 29:50And so the ancillary cortex in adults can
- 29:53be divided into at least dorsal anterior,
- 29:55posterior and ventral anterior subdivision,
- 29:58probably more with the dorsal seeming
- 30:00to be more involved in the high level
- 30:02cognitive control or executive function.
- 30:04Things we've been talking about with the
- 30:07ventral a little bit more involved in
- 30:09the affective and emotional processing and
- 30:11the posterior more involved in somatic
- 30:13sensation and the dorsal anterior insulin.
- 30:15To me that's the special one which shows
- 30:17the most variable functional connections,
- 30:20kind of the greatest level of functional
- 30:22dynamics if you will and the greatest
- 30:25diversity in terms of the the brain
- 30:27regions it interacts with and and the
- 30:29test domains in which it's engaged and
- 30:32has some unique structural connections
- 30:34that we think might underlie some
- 30:36of that functional flexibility.
- 30:37So I was very heartened to see this
- 30:39paper from insulin and cut birth a
- 30:42while ago where you know they start
- 30:44talking about R doc and dimensional
- 30:46models and thinking about breaking
- 30:48down symptom based categories in
- 30:51psychiatry into a process where we
- 30:53where we're now all very familiar with.
- 30:55We're thinking about using genetic risk,
- 30:56brain activity,
- 30:57other markers to stratify samples
- 30:59into more data-driven categories
- 31:01which with the idea that this will
- 31:03eventually be good for us to identify
- 31:06subgroups that are more amenable to
- 31:08treatment on in one form or the other.
- 31:10The only reason I show this slide though
- 31:12is because in that figure from that paper,
- 31:14they have the insular cortex
- 31:16and I didn't plant that there.
- 31:18I had nothing to do with this,
- 31:20and I think it's probably just
- 31:21a random coincidence.
- 31:22But if I were them,
- 31:24I would also focus on the insular cortex
- 31:26in in all of my studies because it it
- 31:29does seem to be very much you know,
- 31:31whether you're interested in schizophrenia,
- 31:33anxiety, addiction,
- 31:34I mean autism,
- 31:36it doesn't matter you'll you
- 31:37can find papers that talk
- 31:39about the insula structure and function
- 31:41and connectivity that are aberrant or
- 31:43atypical or what have you in that disorder.
- 31:45So I I think part of my sort of crusade
- 31:48nowadays is to try to get people to read
- 31:51outside of their favorite disorder and
- 31:53and think about bigger picture models
- 31:55of brain function that can help us
- 31:57understand in a more domain general way.
- 31:59Like what could be the consequence of
- 32:02of something going wrong in this system
- 32:04and how would that look in early life?
- 32:05How would that look in late life?
- 32:07Which subdivision are we talking
- 32:09about and what how consequential
- 32:10is that for a particular behavior?
- 32:12So I love this figure had nothing
- 32:15to do with it.
- 32:16But I promise so now just to to get back
- 32:21to the the autism arm of of things we do.
- 32:24If I have time, OK, I do.
- 32:27I I like to think about flexibility and
- 32:29autism because I know there's so much
- 32:31great work done on social cognition and
- 32:33social communication and language and autism.
- 32:35But but I think a little bit
- 32:37less is known about flexibility,
- 32:39repetitive behaviors and their
- 32:41brain bases for various reasons.
- 32:43And now we're thinking more about this.
- 32:45And I think when you get into this
- 32:48transition to adulthood in in autism,
- 32:50you see some of these outcomes
- 32:53that are surprisingly not optimal.
- 32:55So you see things like 80% of
- 32:57individuals who are diagnosed with
- 32:59autism don't live outside the home
- 33:00or live independently as they,
- 33:02you know, become 18 and older.
- 33:04You also see these kind of
- 33:06surprisingly low employment rates.
- 33:08So you know kind of 80% unemployment
- 33:11in in young adults with autism and
- 33:13that's that's really shocking and
- 33:14and not what we would have hoped for
- 33:16especially if you look at some of
- 33:19these other early life disabilities
- 33:21where you see better outcomes.
- 33:23It's it's surprising to see these
- 33:25kinds of stats and and I think
- 33:28there are treatments,
- 33:29evidence based programs,
- 33:30school based programs that help to
- 33:32train up executive function and help
- 33:35to train cognitive flexibility.
- 33:37And I think some of that is is what
- 33:39we have to start thinking about
- 33:41like when and how to deploy these
- 33:44kinds of interventions.
- 33:45And we do have a lot of things like
- 33:47social skills trading and a lot of
- 33:50stuff that's really effective at at
- 33:52ameliorating some of those difficulties.
- 33:54But there's I think a lot more work
- 33:55to be done here in in flexibility.
- 33:57And the reason I think that
- 33:59flexibility in particular is,
- 34:01is what hinders people in that transition
- 34:03to independence is because that's
- 34:05sort of a a lot of what you do when
- 34:07you're like moving out of the home,
- 34:09getting a new job, making friends,
- 34:11social relationships,
- 34:11a lot of that is kind of moment
- 34:14to moment adjustment of behavior
- 34:16and and flexible cognition.
- 34:18Really it's not just social
- 34:20cognition but like you know,
- 34:21if the bus doesn't show up
- 34:22today and I need to get to work,
- 34:24I better scramble to find a different
- 34:26way to get to work. Otherwise,
- 34:27you know you're going to get fired.
- 34:29So. So things like that I think
- 34:32really do come into Stark.
- 34:34You know, we, we kind of noticed them more
- 34:38during these transition phases of life.
- 34:39But there's also these issues,
- 34:41not a clinician,
- 34:42but those who are will tell me that there's,
- 34:45you know, clearly heterogeneity and autism.
- 34:47So not everyone has flexibility,
- 34:48deficits, not everyone even shows,
- 34:50you know, executive function on problems,
- 34:53on these sort of parent reports,
- 34:55classic measures like the Behavioral
- 34:57Rating Inventory of Executive function.
- 34:59This is a data set from Stuart
- 35:01Mostofsky's group at Kennedy Krieger,
- 35:03and he's got kids between 8:00 and 12:00.
- 35:06Some of them are diagnosed with autism,
- 35:07some with ADHD,
- 35:08some with autism and ADHD and
- 35:10some typically developing.
- 35:12And if you do a latent profile
- 35:14analysis across these behavioral
- 35:15measures of executive function,
- 35:17you'll I like to focus here on the
- 35:19middle pie just to show you that a lot
- 35:21of kids with these diagnosis have just
- 35:23totally average executive function.
- 35:25They're not impaired.
- 35:26They're doing as well as a
- 35:28typically developing kid.
- 35:29In fact,
- 35:30the impaired kids tend to be
- 35:32those ASDADHD Comorbid kids.
- 35:33Perhaps not surprisingly,
- 35:35that double diagnosis really
- 35:37impairs or is associated with
- 35:39the impaired executive function.
- 35:41We found this kind of mixed bag
- 35:43as well in our own data set,
- 35:45typically developing kids and
- 35:46kids with autism.
- 35:47This was an honors thesis student
- 35:49from a student, Adriana Baez,
- 35:51who then went on to Med school.
- 35:53So I had a lot of, just incidentally,
- 35:54a lot of great undergrads who
- 35:56write papers as first author,
- 35:58and so that's been really exciting to see.
- 36:00But anyway,
- 36:01this was her honors thesis,
- 36:02and she showed again that a lot of kids with
- 36:06autism do have average executive functions.
- 36:08So it's there's heterogeneity there,
- 36:11as one might have expected.
- 36:12Not everyone needs the same
- 36:13kinds of interventions,
- 36:14right?
- 36:15So what if we could figure out what are
- 36:19the signatures of this heterogeneity?
- 36:20Is it something as simple as
- 36:22brain state transitions that are
- 36:24related to flexible behaviors?
- 36:26Is that something that's a marker
- 36:28of flexibility and can we see
- 36:30that in in our data sets?
- 36:32So the first thing we tested was,
- 36:33do kids or individuals who are
- 36:35diagnosed with autism show a
- 36:37reduction in the number of brain
- 36:39state transitions compared to the
- 36:41typically developing individuals?
- 36:42And so I know you're probably familiar
- 36:45with the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange.
- 36:47So this was something that Adriana
- 36:49Dimartino started a while back
- 36:51trying to get all of us autism
- 36:54researchers to share our data,
- 36:55make them publicly available.
- 36:57And this has resulted in hundreds of data
- 37:00sets being now available for download.
- 37:01So you can do things like
- 37:03independent component analysis,
- 37:05lighting,
- 37:05widow connectivity.
- 37:06And initially in this data set we
- 37:09do find some evidence for small but
- 37:11significant reduction in the number
- 37:13of brain state transitions for those
- 37:15diagnosed with autism compared to the
- 37:18typically developing individuals.
- 37:19This was another Emily Marshall in our
- 37:22lab as an undergrad honors thesis wrote.
- 37:25A paper is now a Med student doing great.
- 37:28So she looked at the dynamics of this
- 37:30salience or cingular insular network
- 37:32and found again a reduction in the
- 37:35frequency of brain states involving
- 37:37this network and its Co activation with
- 37:41others in a study she did as part of
- 37:43her honors thesis a few years back.
- 37:45So, so you know we are starting
- 37:47to find these signatures in the
- 37:49brain imaging data as well.
- 37:51And if again if you look at a task
- 37:53that was the last one was resting
- 37:54state but this is a task where you've
- 37:56got kids doing a a set shifting task,
- 37:58it's actually pretty easy.
- 38:00This was I think BJ Casey first came
- 38:02up with this paradigm but it's you
- 38:04know you see circles and squares and
- 38:06you have to pick the odd one out.
- 38:07Sometimes it's the odd one out
- 38:09is different based on shapes,
- 38:10sometimes it's color and in mixed
- 38:12blocks it could be shape or color.
- 38:13So that's the set shifting kind
- 38:16of part of it.
- 38:17It's a cognitive flexibility test.
- 38:20It's relatively easy to do and in fact
- 38:21all of our kids can do it easily.
- 38:23So behaviorally this isn't too challenging.
- 38:26But what we find is that if you look
- 38:28at Co activation patterns between the
- 38:30executive and salience and default
- 38:32three kind of networks I've been focusing on,
- 38:34kids with autism can do the task
- 38:36and their brains are doing the task.
- 38:38Similarly to typically developing
- 38:39kids as they go on and on and get
- 38:41into the fourth run of the task
- 38:43they actually have to engage more of
- 38:44this front of pride all there's more
- 38:47frequent patterns of that network occurring.
- 38:49So it's like they they can do things
- 38:51but the strategies might be different
- 38:52or the brain networks involved might
- 38:54change over time in ways that we can't
- 38:56see it with a whole brain activation
- 38:58analysis and we actually can't even see
- 39:00with a functional connectivity analysis.
- 39:02But we can see with these like
- 39:04smaller wind approaches that look at
- 39:06dynamics and don't look at averages
- 39:09but look at like changes across time.
- 39:11So that's something we've we found
- 39:13and are sort of following up on.
- 39:15So this is kind of a full circle about
- 39:19thinking about dynamic functional
- 39:20connectivity approaches to reveal a
- 39:23typical patterns of brain dynamics in
- 39:25prevalent neurodevelopmental conditions
- 39:26characterized by cognitive inflexibility
- 39:28like autism as I mentioned here.
- 39:31And in the extent to which these
- 39:33individual differences in brain dynamics
- 39:35underlie individual differences in
- 39:37flexible behaviors is is something
- 39:39we're we're actively investigating as
- 39:40well as as I'll mention in a moment
- 39:43other moderators or mediators of these
- 39:46relationships between brain and behavior.
- 39:48So this whole time before 2021 we
- 39:52were conducting this work in Miami,
- 39:54which has a huge bilingual population.
- 39:57So most of our participants actually
- 39:59were Spanish,
- 40:00English, bilingual and so one one thing
- 40:02we did which was completely unplanned.
- 40:05Was that we were able to test the
- 40:08relationship between bilingual
- 40:08exposure and executive function
- 40:10in our children in the study.
- 40:12So the RO one was focused
- 40:13on cognitive flexibility.
- 40:14Not just nothing to do with language,
- 40:16nothing to do with bilingualism,
- 40:18but just the way the the sampling occurred.
- 40:20We had a lot of this
- 40:22population in our sample.
- 40:23So it turns out there's a lot of controversy
- 40:26about whether or not raising a child
- 40:29with developmental language delays,
- 40:30whether raising them in a bilingual
- 40:32home will result in slowing down of
- 40:34language and cognitive development.
- 40:36But there's been a lot more work
- 40:39recently suggesting not only are there
- 40:42no negative consequences of either
- 40:44in cognitive or language development
- 40:45for being raised in a bilingual home,
- 40:47but there might even be mitigation
- 40:49of some flexibility deficits.
- 40:50And in fact,
- 40:51some folks are starting to report fewer
- 40:54executive function problems in dual
- 40:56language households or autism kids with
- 40:58autism raised in dual language household.
- 41:00So this is to me a really exciting
- 41:03kind of untapped potential to explore.
- 41:05It's almost like a natural intervention.
- 41:08So in the earlier days,
- 41:10I think social or sorry,
- 41:12communication disorders,
- 41:13clinicians would say, OK,
- 41:15if your child is having language delay,
- 41:17don't expose them to multiple languages,
- 41:18just stick with one.
- 41:20We don't want to confuse them.
- 41:21But it turns out that wasn't the best wisdom.
- 41:24I mean, there's really no negative effect.
- 41:26There might even be these boosts in executive
- 41:28function that we we can encourage with this.
- 41:30Not to mention all the other
- 41:32benefits of bilingualism.
- 41:33Right.
- 41:33So we found, you know,
- 41:34in our initial study with Celia Romero,
- 41:37she's just about to hopefully have this
- 41:40paper accepted just initial evidence.
- 41:42When you get to typically developing kids,
- 41:44there's almost a ceiling effect if
- 41:45they're good at executive function.
- 41:46The bilingualism doesn't really change that.
- 41:50It sort of, you know,
- 41:50doesn't affect anything at all.
- 41:53But if you look here,
- 41:53it's just one scale of the behavior
- 41:55rating inventory of executive function.
- 41:57If you look at it,
- 41:57the kids with autism being from
- 42:00a bilingual home was associated
- 42:02with better inhibitory control
- 42:04than being from a monolingual home
- 42:07in in that particular population.
- 42:09So there may be some boosts or
- 42:11protective effects or what have you
- 42:13in certain conditions that we don't
- 42:14see in a typically developing case
- 42:16because they may already be at ceiling
- 42:18or they may not be affected in such a way.
- 42:21So this is something we finally
- 42:23got an R21 to do and believe me,
- 42:25nobody wanted to fund this for some reason.
- 42:28But we're finally starting to
- 42:29collect data to really explore
- 42:31the links between bilingualism,
- 42:33executive function and brain
- 42:34development in 8 to 12 year old
- 42:36children with autism now at UCLA.
- 42:38And we put in an RO one to look
- 42:40at this longitudinally.
- 42:42So we're doing the data collection
- 42:43on the first set
- 42:44and then we want to follow them
- 42:45up across that adolescent phase,
- 42:47keep keep them going into
- 42:48young adulthood if possible.
- 42:49But we want to see how, you know,
- 42:52the initial bilingual exposure kind
- 42:54of plays out across this interesting
- 42:56period of adolescence when things are
- 42:59changing executive function, you know,
- 43:01deficits might be becoming more prevalent.
- 43:03We want to see if there
- 43:04are protective effect.
- 43:05Is there a bilingual advantage?
- 43:06Is there a boost?
- 43:08Nobody knows.
- 43:08And there's like no studies at all
- 43:10about bilingualism and the influence
- 43:12on brain development and autism.
- 43:14There's nothing that exists.
- 43:16So that's something we're really,
- 43:19as far as data collection goes,
- 43:20that's where we're going
- 43:21in the next few years.
- 43:23The last thing I wanted to
- 43:25mention was this is like,
- 43:26and now for something completely different.
- 43:29So we have so many of you know I've
- 43:31I've become super involved with the
- 43:33adolescent brain cognitive development study.
- 43:35So I'm the one of the associate
- 43:37directors now for justice,
- 43:39equity, diversity,
- 43:40inclusion for the entire consortium.
- 43:42In addition to the 20 hours of
- 43:44meetings a week that entails also
- 43:46a lot of knowledge about the ABCD
- 43:48data set that now we can use as a
- 43:51lab to to further our own goals.
- 43:52So this is the study of, you know,
- 43:5410,000 youth, Yale as a site,
- 43:56UCLA as a site.
- 43:58They're started in 2016 and now the
- 44:00kids were 9 and 10 at the first
- 44:02wave and now they're about 16/17/18.
- 44:05So it's 10 year longitudinal
- 44:08study imaging neurocog,
- 44:10substance abuse substance use data.
- 44:13So and I won't.
- 44:14I won't try to embarrass myself by
- 44:15pretending to know anything about addiction.
- 44:17This crowd knows about addiction.
- 44:19I don't.
- 44:19But what I do want to do is
- 44:21think about salience,
- 44:22network dynamics and how they might be
- 44:25related to substance use initiation.
- 44:27And I swear this wasn't my idea.
- 44:29A program officer asked me to do this study.
- 44:31So we're trying to do is is look
- 44:33at precursors of substance use
- 44:35initiation using this ABCD data set.
- 44:38Cause at 9:00 and 10:00 the
- 44:39kids aren't really doing much.
- 44:40They might,
- 44:41a few of them report sipping alcohol,
- 44:42but as you follow them up over
- 44:44time you can see, you know,
- 44:46they start to do things like try out smoking,
- 44:48try out this and that, and things change.
- 44:50And that's what this study has meant to
- 44:52track like mental health trajectories
- 44:53and substance use initiation trajectories.
- 44:55And what we want to do is use
- 44:57predictive modeling.
- 44:58Of course,
- 44:58connectome based predictive
- 44:59modeling was invented here at Yale,
- 45:01you all know about it.
- 45:02But we want to use dynamic connectome
- 45:04based predictive modeling to try to
- 45:07see if anything about the salience
- 45:08network and its initial state
- 45:10predicts vulnerability to later
- 45:12substance use initiation using the
- 45:14machine learning and some of the
- 45:17connectivity approaches that I mentioned.
- 45:19And we think this will be interesting
- 45:21because as far as I know about addiction,
- 45:23most of a lot of the studies
- 45:24that are done
- 45:25in adults aren't able to really tease
- 45:27apart whether the brain difference
- 45:28is seen are the consequence or
- 45:30the cause of the of the addiction.
- 45:33So you see kind of what happens
- 45:35after years of of, you know,
- 45:37being a user of some substance,
- 45:38but we don't know necessarily if
- 45:41there's like some vulnerability
- 45:42in some circuits or things like
- 45:44that that pre preceded the onset.
- 45:46And so the adolescent brain cognitive
- 45:48development study actually allows
- 45:49you to do that 'cause you'll,
- 45:50you might, you know,
- 45:51be able to predict what some of
- 45:53these vulnerabilities are related
- 45:55to inhibition and and you know,
- 45:57the networks that are involved in in
- 45:59flexibility and things like that.
- 46:00So this is something we're trying
- 46:02to get funding to do as well.
- 46:04Just pure data analysis of the ABCD
- 46:07study and we'll see how that goes.
- 46:10But basically as I start out talking
- 46:12about why we're interested in flexibility
- 46:15deficits and flexible behaviors can
- 46:18be impact day-to-day activities,
- 46:20forming and maintaining relationships,
- 46:22employment and independent living.
- 46:24So we think that understanding the neural
- 46:27mechanisms underlying flexibility deficits,
- 46:28you know across autism is our big focus.
- 46:30But across lots of clinical
- 46:32conditions we think can be important
- 46:35for tailoring therapies,
- 46:36predicting responses,
- 46:37thinking about risk and resilience for
- 46:40for various things like addiction.
- 46:43And so eventually,
- 46:44you know our kids can can focus
- 46:47more about their interactions in
- 46:49the world and less less on the
- 46:51specific repetitive behaviors and
- 46:53routines that might hold them back.
- 46:55And so this is the lab at UCLA who does
- 46:58all the work and my collaboration at
- 47:00work on the right some of the funding
- 47:02and the people who have donated time data,
- 47:06code of resources,
- 47:08mentorship and collaboration
- 47:09and friendship over the years.
- 47:11And this is the not where I do my work,
- 47:14but this is, this is UCLA.
- 47:16And I'm really happy to take any
- 47:18questions and have discussions.
- 47:19Thanks again.