Skip to Main Content

Yale Psychiatry Grand Rounds: "Brain Dynamics and Flexible Behaviors"

May 17, 2024
  • 00:00This really kind introduction and for
  • 00:02the invitation to visit here it's I will
  • 00:05say it's it's always fun to visit Yale.
  • 00:08The the first time I came here
  • 00:09was in 2012 and then I was here
  • 00:12again in 2018 and now it's 2024.
  • 00:15So I hope to see you all again in
  • 00:172030 because it seems like every six
  • 00:19years I get the opportunity to come
  • 00:21and and and meet and just benefit from
  • 00:24these wonderful environment in the
  • 00:27outstanding discussions and just feel
  • 00:29the collegiality every time I'm here.
  • 00:31So it's really nice to be back.
  • 00:34Thank you again, Denise,
  • 00:35for this wonderful introduction.
  • 00:36Thank you, John and and everyone
  • 00:38else for for the warm welcome.
  • 00:40Now I suppose I need to figure out
  • 00:42where the slide advancement is.
  • 00:43So. Oh, here we go. OK All right.
  • 00:47So it's just it's an honor to be
  • 00:51here following up this introduction
  • 00:53of of RIBICOFF.
  • 00:55So I'll talk to you today about our
  • 00:58work on brain dynamics and flexible
  • 01:01behaviors and I think it's it's of
  • 01:03interest for our lab but I think
  • 01:05for a lot of people in psychiatry
  • 01:07to think about flexibility.
  • 01:09We are we focus a lot on children
  • 01:11with autism in our own research and in
  • 01:14this particular clinical condition we
  • 01:16often see this insistence on sameness,
  • 01:18behavioral inflexibility.
  • 01:19For example,
  • 01:20a child might want to wear a particular
  • 01:23pair of socks or eat as only yellow
  • 01:25foods or you know these kinds of
  • 01:28repetitive behaviors that sometimes can,
  • 01:30you know this insistence on sameness
  • 01:32can sometimes cause challenges
  • 01:34for caregivers and and difficulty
  • 01:36in in day-to-day life activities.
  • 01:39And so I used to kind of introduce
  • 01:40my talk saying you know flexibility
  • 01:42is important.
  • 01:43But during the pandemic I think
  • 01:44we all had a a big dose of what
  • 01:47it means to be flexible.
  • 01:48So we went from our everyday lives and and
  • 01:51routines to something completely different.
  • 01:53We started giving talks over zoom instead of
  • 01:57giving them at at lecture podiums like this.
  • 02:00So I'm myself readjusting
  • 02:02to being standing up again.
  • 02:06So.
  • 02:06So we've all sort of experienced this
  • 02:08like how changes and and be having
  • 02:11difficulties with flexible behaviors can
  • 02:12kind of impact our our day-to-day lives.
  • 02:15So we try to focus on the lab on
  • 02:17thinking about the neural basis of
  • 02:19flexibility and we do kind of take a
  • 02:22developmental look at these questions.
  • 02:23So we think about how brain networks
  • 02:26develop and mature in ways that can
  • 02:28support increasingly sophisticated
  • 02:30types of cognition like flexibility
  • 02:32across the lifespan.
  • 02:33So we think about how brain
  • 02:35networks develop from childhood,
  • 02:36adolescence into adulthood,
  • 02:38how that underlies cognitive development.
  • 02:40And you know basically how can compromised
  • 02:43connectivity within and between large
  • 02:46scale brain networks be related to
  • 02:48developmental neuropathologies.
  • 02:49And we try to think about how we
  • 02:53can use this basic information
  • 02:55about brain structure and function
  • 02:58to inform diagnosis and design
  • 03:00interventions ultimately.
  • 03:01And so I'm really excited to be here in
  • 03:04the in the Ribicoff series because
  • 03:06we do try to do in our lab a sort of
  • 03:10combination of basic neuroscience
  • 03:12developmental work and clinical clinically
  • 03:14translatable kind of research in the
  • 03:17same umbrella or under the same lab.
  • 03:20So we use structural and
  • 03:22functional neuroimaging.
  • 03:23So that includes task functional
  • 03:25magnetic resonance imaging,
  • 03:27resting state fMRI,
  • 03:28diffusion weighted imaging to look
  • 03:30at structural connections and also
  • 03:32some causal modeling approaches
  • 03:34to look at relationships among and
  • 03:36between brain regions as they relate
  • 03:39to things like executive function,
  • 03:40flexible behaviors.
  • 03:41And although we we love neuroimaging,
  • 03:44we're a network neuroscience lab,
  • 03:46we loved all this sort of cutting
  • 03:47edge tools that are now available.
  • 03:49I think it's important to remember
  • 03:51how much of this work just dates back
  • 03:54to before we had these fancy tools.
  • 03:57So the foundations of what we now
  • 03:59call network neuroscience were
  • 04:00planted a long time ago.
  • 04:02You know before MRI was really
  • 04:04very much in vogue.
  • 04:05I I love some of these papers from
  • 04:08Marcel Maslam who is who has you know
  • 04:10is a is a neurologist who were and
  • 04:13a neuropsychologist who had these
  • 04:16great ideas about brain function.
  • 04:18That that really came from looking
  • 04:20at patients and lesions and figuring
  • 04:22out what the deficits were in
  • 04:25in patients with focal lesions.
  • 04:27And the interesting conclusions
  • 04:28that are nicely summarized in one
  • 04:31of his Seminole papers.
  • 04:32Or that cognition is served by
  • 04:35interconnected neural networks and
  • 04:37any complex behaviour is mapped at
  • 04:39the level of multifocal neural systems
  • 04:41rather than specific anatomical sites.
  • 04:43And I, I love these these papers,
  • 04:46They're kind of just a history of how
  • 04:48the brain works and they really hold up.
  • 04:50I think to this day,
  • 04:51if you read this,
  • 04:51you'll say, hey everything.
  • 04:52And he was right.
  • 04:54He was right about everything.
  • 04:56And I think this is interesting because
  • 04:58there was the trend at one point to say,
  • 05:00well, OK,
  • 05:00we have a lesion to this brain
  • 05:01area and that causes this deficit,
  • 05:03that brain,
  • 05:03you know,
  • 05:04there must be a one to one mapping
  • 05:06between that function and that structure.
  • 05:07But even you can use the same data to
  • 05:09make the opposite conclusion is that,
  • 05:11you know,
  • 05:12brain regions can be relatively
  • 05:14specialized and not sort of
  • 05:16specifically tied to to one function.
  • 05:18And so I think this is some
  • 05:21really nice foundational work.
  • 05:22There's also one of my favorite papers
  • 05:25is from Randy McIntosh in 2004 where
  • 05:27he introduced the concept of neural context.
  • 05:30The idea that relevant functional
  • 05:32relevance of any given brain area
  • 05:34depends on the status of other
  • 05:36connected areas to that brain region.
  • 05:38So kind of not just focusing on the one,
  • 05:41one or two areas at a time,
  • 05:42but the whole kind of context
  • 05:44in which brain function is,
  • 05:45is occurring.
  • 05:47And we also think a lot
  • 05:48nowadays about the time domain,
  • 05:50so about how networks need to be
  • 05:52understood in terms of interactions
  • 05:54as they unfold temporally between
  • 05:56between multiple brain regions
  • 05:57as they unfold temporarily.
  • 05:59A nice paper from Luis Pasoa 10 years
  • 06:02ago really drives this point home.
  • 06:05So there are some things that many
  • 06:07of you are very familiar with.
  • 06:09And so back in 1995,
  • 06:12Broad Bizwell published a paper
  • 06:14showing that you can find these
  • 06:16coherent spontaneous fluctuations
  • 06:17in different parts of the brain.
  • 06:19You focused on the motor cortex.
  • 06:21But since that 1995 paper there have been,
  • 06:24you know, thousands of studies showing
  • 06:26that at this very low frequency the brain
  • 06:29seems to recapitulate in the resting state.
  • 06:32All of the the brain networks that we
  • 06:34can see engaged in tasks like memory,
  • 06:36attention, vision, motor processing.
  • 06:38At these point O1 to .1 Hertz
  • 06:42is low frequency.
  • 06:43We can see these oscillations
  • 06:45spontaneously occurring.
  • 06:46And this is just a ongoing,
  • 06:49you know,
  • 06:49fMRI of somebody doing nothing
  • 06:50at all in the scanner,
  • 06:51just kind of asked to just lay still.
  • 06:54But it's not sort of random
  • 06:56spontaneous activity,
  • 06:57but really coherent in these systems
  • 06:59that we've been studying for many years.
  • 07:01If you wait around a while,
  • 07:02you'll see a language network or a vision,
  • 07:04a visual cortices.
  • 07:06So these kind of spontaneous fluctuations,
  • 07:09many labs have been exploiting now for
  • 07:11a number of years to try to understand
  • 07:13the functional organization of the brain.
  • 07:17And as I,
  • 07:17as I mentioned before,
  • 07:18we all know what flexibility is
  • 07:20and we've had to be very flexible
  • 07:22across this pandemic period.
  • 07:24During that period,
  • 07:24I had the time to sit down and
  • 07:27write a bunch of invited reviews.
  • 07:29So that was my my COVID,
  • 07:31my my pandemic was review writing.
  • 07:33But it gave me time to really think about
  • 07:35what we mean when we study flexibility,
  • 07:37what we're what we're using
  • 07:38that term to refer to.
  • 07:40And in in human sort of cognitive
  • 07:43neuroscience and psychology we tend
  • 07:44to use things like the Wisconsin
  • 07:46cards sort tasks and we ask people
  • 07:49to make categorizations and then you
  • 07:51know switch the criteria and and we
  • 07:53use that as an index of flexibility.
  • 07:55But I also think we have all these
  • 07:57parallels in behavioral neuroscience.
  • 08:00In reversal learning paradigms for
  • 08:01example you're just asking animals
  • 08:03to make different kinds of stimulus
  • 08:05outcome mappings and we we call
  • 08:07that behavioral flexibility and
  • 08:08I think mainly because we can't
  • 08:10guess what animals are thinking.
  • 08:12So we have them do a behavior and
  • 08:14we see how how they flexibly behave.
  • 08:17When we look at developmental psychology,
  • 08:19we often come up with things
  • 08:21that kids can do like,
  • 08:23you know,
  • 08:23tell me you know which color is this fish
  • 08:25or which direction is the fish pointing.
  • 08:26But we do things like task switching
  • 08:28to engage the development of
  • 08:30flexible behaviors in in children.
  • 08:32But all of these kinds of paradigms tend
  • 08:36to engage frontal parietal networks.
  • 08:38They tend to engage singular insular
  • 08:41cortices and frontostriatal systems and
  • 08:43and many of you have been, you know,
  • 08:47working in this field for years.
  • 08:48So I'm not telling you anything new.
  • 08:50What I do think is interesting
  • 08:52about flexibility is that
  • 08:53you can see difficulties,
  • 08:54oops, across many different
  • 08:56kinds of clinical conditions.
  • 08:58So we focus a lot on autism and early
  • 09:00life conditions like ADHD where you get
  • 09:02things like and autism specifically,
  • 09:04you get restricted and repetitive behaviors.
  • 09:06But then you have things
  • 09:07like anxiety and depression,
  • 09:08which come along more during
  • 09:10adolescence that are associated
  • 09:11with repetitive negative thinking.
  • 09:13Worry in the case of anxiety or
  • 09:15rumination in the case of depression
  • 09:17are kind of inflexible thought patterns.
  • 09:19So it's another way of thinking about
  • 09:22flexibility and and difficulties
  • 09:24with flexibility.
  • 09:24And of course,
  • 09:26OCD is another great example of
  • 09:28a case where you really have
  • 09:30difficulty coming out of a routine.
  • 09:32And then even later in life,
  • 09:33things like Alzheimer's and
  • 09:35and Parkinson's are associated
  • 09:36with cognitive rigidity.
  • 09:38So, you know,
  • 09:38these are maybe not all the same thing
  • 09:40we're seeing at these different stages,
  • 09:42but there's something about getting
  • 09:44stuck in a particular behavioral
  • 09:46pattern or thought pattern that we
  • 09:48can see perhaps as a, you know,
  • 09:50transdiagnostic, you know, difficulty.
  • 09:52And so for us, we think,
  • 09:55well, let's try to figure out,
  • 09:56you know,
  • 09:56what in the brain is allowing
  • 09:58flexibility in the first place.
  • 10:00And so a lot of the work that
  • 10:02I won't talk about today is on
  • 10:03thinking about development of brain
  • 10:05networks involved in flexibility.
  • 10:06So we'll do things like look at MRI
  • 10:09data collected from participants
  • 10:10between the age of 6 and 85 S really
  • 10:13kind of lifespan data sets and
  • 10:15look at changes in brain signals
  • 10:17as they associate with, you know,
  • 10:19like flexibility of brain
  • 10:20networks across the lifespan.
  • 10:22We'll look at things like differences
  • 10:24in children versus adults in the
  • 10:26strength of certain circuits or the
  • 10:28effective connections between brain
  • 10:29regions and how they change with development.
  • 10:31And we'll get how network interactions
  • 10:33again change across the lifespan,
  • 10:35for example,
  • 10:36between age of 6 and 85.
  • 10:38There's a nice Nathan Klein
  • 10:40Institute data set that that
  • 10:42provides a lot of these data and
  • 10:44they're publicly available now.
  • 10:45So lots of things are are you know
  • 10:48analysis are available and we're
  • 10:50we've we've shown kind of how these
  • 10:52differences in brain organization
  • 10:54across the lifespan differentially
  • 10:56relate to executive function.
  • 10:58So like some of these brain variability
  • 11:01metrics you know are associated
  • 11:03with better executive function and
  • 11:05adolescence but worse executive
  • 11:06function and old age for example.
  • 11:08And I know you can't see that slide,
  • 11:10so I apologize.
  • 11:11I'll make it bigger next time, I promise.
  • 11:13But what we do do in this scanner
  • 11:16is sometimes we just test.
  • 11:18You've tried to develop a paradigm
  • 11:19that can be used across children,
  • 11:21across adults, across clinical populations.
  • 11:24And we took this one paradigm from the
  • 11:27developmental psychology literature
  • 11:28called the flexible item selection task.
  • 11:30And what you do here is you ask
  • 11:32somebody to pick two things that go
  • 11:34together and they might say, well,
  • 11:35the 2nd and the 4th card are both blue,
  • 11:38so they go together on color.
  • 11:40And then you might say, OK,
  • 11:41now pick another two things that go
  • 11:42together and they might say, well,
  • 11:44the 3rd and the 4th are both rabbits.
  • 11:46I think that's right. And so it's OK.
  • 11:48Pick another set of things
  • 11:49that go together and say, well,
  • 11:50the 1st and the 4th, they both have one
  • 11:52item so that they're both showing one.
  • 11:54So that's something that goes together.
  • 11:56And this requires, you know,
  • 11:57working memory, inhibition,
  • 11:59all of these processes, attention,
  • 12:00but also of course flexibility.
  • 12:02The further you get along these dimensions,
  • 12:04the harder it is to come up
  • 12:06with things that go together.
  • 12:07So we tried to put this in
  • 12:09the scanner and we said, OK,
  • 12:10here's just a control version of it,
  • 12:12just hit the two buttons that
  • 12:13are highlighted.
  • 12:14So OK, just follow along.
  • 12:16So we spent some years validating this task,
  • 12:20having adults do it,
  • 12:21putting people in the scanners,
  • 12:22looking at their accuracy
  • 12:24inside of the scanner,
  • 12:26outside the scanner,
  • 12:27all the typical validation stuff come by,
  • 12:30created an efficiency metric to look at
  • 12:32how good people are at this task and
  • 12:34how much better they get at it over time.
  • 12:36So Dejani did this work in our lab
  • 12:39and found as one might expect really
  • 12:42robust brain activation and flexibility
  • 12:45trials in lateral prefrontal cortex,
  • 12:48parietal cortices,
  • 12:49cerebellum and basal ganglia and the
  • 12:52anterior cingulate and the anterior
  • 12:55ansula favorite brain region of mine
  • 12:58that will come up again and really,
  • 13:01really strong and robust
  • 13:02activation across these regions.
  • 13:04So,
  • 13:04so we know as as from lots of
  • 13:06literature that these are all
  • 13:08very much involved in in flexible
  • 13:09thinking and flexible behaviors.
  • 13:11What I think is interesting to
  • 13:13note though is for those fans
  • 13:15of neurosynth and meta analysis.
  • 13:17If you look at if you put a term
  • 13:19like shifting or working memory
  • 13:20for example into the neurosynth to
  • 13:22look at automated across studies,
  • 13:24what are the brain regions that are
  • 13:26involved in shifting or I'm sorry,
  • 13:28flexibility, you'll see these regions,
  • 13:30right, the the same ones I just mentioned,
  • 13:32frontal parietal cingulance or
  • 13:33if you go into neurosynth,
  • 13:35then you type in updating or
  • 13:37you know working memory,
  • 13:38whatever phrase you want to use,
  • 13:40you'll see similar kinds of activation.
  • 13:43And then if you type in inhibition
  • 13:44again you'll see a lot of these
  • 13:46overlapping brain regions.
  • 13:47So they're really kind of broadly
  • 13:50involved in these different
  • 13:51components of executive function.
  • 13:53It's hard to,
  • 13:54I guess the point is it's hard to
  • 13:57pull out of flexibility specific
  • 13:59activations when almost all the
  • 14:01tasks that try to tap flexible
  • 14:03thinking also involve attention,
  • 14:04working memory, inhibition and you know,
  • 14:06everything else.
  • 14:07So it's it's all not as clean as,
  • 14:10as you know,
  • 14:11we like to pretend,
  • 14:12but you can do things like there's
  • 14:14connectivity modelling approaches that
  • 14:16let you get at some of these questions.
  • 14:18One of them we've worked on
  • 14:20here with Katie Gates at UNC,
  • 14:22it's called group iterative
  • 14:24multiple model estimation or Gimme.
  • 14:26It's kind of a an iterative search
  • 14:28algorithm that tries to fit a
  • 14:30structural equation model to describe
  • 14:32an individual connectome using user
  • 14:34specified regions of interest.
  • 14:36So if you take for example of all
  • 14:38of these nodes that are activated
  • 14:40in the flexible item selection task,
  • 14:42you can look at OK,
  • 14:43which sort of nodes kind of are most
  • 14:46directly activated by this paradigm and
  • 14:48which are sort of secondarily activated.
  • 14:51So here we found that inferior
  • 14:53frontal junction is sort of directly
  • 14:56activated by flexible thinking
  • 14:57or flexible item selection.
  • 14:59And then there's information flow
  • 15:01to other regions including the
  • 15:03dorsilateral prefrontal cortex,
  • 15:05anterior cingulate and others.
  • 15:07And so there's kind of some regions
  • 15:10that are more influential than others
  • 15:12in this in this type of flexibility.
  • 15:15So there's kind of ways of of teasing
  • 15:18apart some of these activations and
  • 15:20looking at more specific information flow.
  • 15:22There's also a great deal of
  • 15:24individual variability,
  • 15:25like even though all subjects tend to
  • 15:28activate these brain nodes during this test,
  • 15:30there's group level paths or
  • 15:32connections that are consistent,
  • 15:34but a lot of sub sub sub group level
  • 15:38paths meaning like some subjects engage
  • 15:40these connections and others don't.
  • 15:42So there's a lot of kind of more nuance
  • 15:46to these blobs and activation patterns
  • 15:49than we might initially realize.
  • 15:51So we think about,
  • 15:53for us at least,
  • 15:54how cognitive flexibility involves
  • 15:56the coordination among multiple
  • 15:57brain regions that are all known to
  • 15:59play a role in executive function,
  • 16:01adults and children.
  • 16:02I didn't show the maps from children,
  • 16:04but children between the ages of
  • 16:058 to 12 also show these kind of
  • 16:08similar activations of lateral
  • 16:10frontoparrietal and singular insular
  • 16:12networks during flexible item selection
  • 16:14and task modulated connectivity.
  • 16:15The inferior frontal junction seems
  • 16:17to be particularly important for
  • 16:19this type of flexible behavior.
  • 16:21And I won't,
  • 16:22I'll show you at the end of the
  • 16:23talk some of the work we're doing
  • 16:25now to bring this task to kids with
  • 16:27autism and look at the brains of
  • 16:28children between the ages of 8:00
  • 16:30and 12:00 as they're doing this
  • 16:32type of of flexible behavior task.
  • 16:34And we, well, I'll show you,
  • 16:36I don't remember if I included
  • 16:38this slide but I'll I'll come back
  • 16:40to this towards the end.
  • 16:41So as we kind of develop these tasks,
  • 16:44try to use them in clinical populations
  • 16:47and developmental populations at the
  • 16:49same time we try to focus on these
  • 16:51big data sets that can help us look
  • 16:53at sort of the adults neurotypical brain.
  • 16:56And then the nice thing about some
  • 16:58of these publicly shared large
  • 16:59data sets is they're, you know,
  • 17:01hundreds of subjects large.
  • 17:02There are lots more data points
  • 17:04than we typically can collect
  • 17:06in the clinical population.
  • 17:07So we can do a lot more with the
  • 17:10the methods and the inferences and
  • 17:12the replication here because for
  • 17:14example in human connection project
  • 17:15you actually have one hour of resting
  • 17:17state F MRI data from each person.
  • 17:20So a lot more,
  • 17:21you know, signal in there so we can
  • 17:25do things like look at moment to
  • 17:26moment functional connectivity or
  • 17:28dynamic functional connectivity.
  • 17:29So in this process,
  • 17:30instead of saying what areas are
  • 17:32connected to each other on average,
  • 17:34we're saying if you break down
  • 17:36this data into 45 second chunks
  • 17:39or 62nd windows and you look at
  • 17:42reoccurring patterns of whole brain
  • 17:43functional connectivity and you use
  • 17:44some kind of clustering to say, OK,
  • 17:46here's some different brain states.
  • 17:48You can then start to quantify
  • 17:50dynamic metrics like the frequency
  • 17:52of occurrence of a particular
  • 17:53brain state or the dwell time,
  • 17:55which means how long does this
  • 17:57brain state persist once it comes
  • 17:59along and the state transitions,
  • 18:01how much switching between
  • 18:02brain states can you quantify?
  • 18:04So in the human connection project is
  • 18:06that this is just showing you a couple
  • 18:08100 subjects do split half replication,
  • 18:10various things.
  • 18:11You can use independent component analysis
  • 18:13to breakdown the brain into little regions.
  • 18:16And then the nice thing is that
  • 18:17all there's all kinds of other
  • 18:19data on these same subjects,
  • 18:21information about their processing speed,
  • 18:23inhibition, cognitive flexibility,
  • 18:24fluid intelligence,
  • 18:25working memory,
  • 18:26lots of scores to play around with.
  • 18:29And So what we found is that typically
  • 18:31the the brain enters different states,
  • 18:34but it's got most of its time,
  • 18:35about 36% of its time in this loosely
  • 18:39connected kind of flexible state.
  • 18:41And about 10% of the time it's really
  • 18:43in this more cohere or really tightly
  • 18:46correlated state that you see on the right.
  • 18:49And it turns out that those individuals
  • 18:51who do better on tests of working memory
  • 18:53and tests of cognitive flexibility
  • 18:55like the Wisconsin Card sort of test,
  • 18:57those are the ones who are spending
  • 18:58sort of or showing more frequent
  • 19:00occurrence of states one and two,
  • 19:01the loose connectivity states with
  • 19:03more variability actually in their
  • 19:06connection patterns and less time in the
  • 19:08tight sort of inflexible states on the right.
  • 19:11And so this is just,
  • 19:13you know,
  • 19:14showing us how we can get at sort of
  • 19:16the basic neuroscience of of brain
  • 19:18flexibility using these large data sets.
  • 19:21And here we're just showing an example
  • 19:22of how greater cognitive flexibility,
  • 19:24which is measured outside of
  • 19:25the scanner in this case,
  • 19:26are associated with the propensity to
  • 19:29occupy these more frequently occurring
  • 19:31brain configurations characterized by
  • 19:33attenuated correlations and greater
  • 19:35functional connectivity variability.
  • 19:37And it's just showing how we can
  • 19:39think about using dynamic functional
  • 19:42connectivity approaches to reveal
  • 19:44relationships between brain
  • 19:46dynamics and flexible cognition.
  • 19:48So,
  • 19:48OK,
  • 19:48So what I talked about in the last
  • 19:51study was really a whole brain kind of
  • 19:55agnostic approach to thinking about the
  • 19:58how flexibility might be implemented.
  • 20:01But there's also of course,
  • 20:02decades of cognitive neuroscience
  • 20:04literature that that we all sort of
  • 20:07rely on to to think about what are the
  • 20:10more specific brain networks that might
  • 20:11be involved in some of these processes.
  • 20:13I already sort of mentioned
  • 20:15the central executive or sort
  • 20:17of lateral frontal parietal systems,
  • 20:19the salience or mid single
  • 20:21insular systems anchored in the
  • 20:23cingulate and insular cortex,
  • 20:25and the default mode network which has
  • 20:27key nodes in the medial prefrontal
  • 20:29and posterior parietal cortices.
  • 20:31A number of years ago,
  • 20:32we started to notice some interesting
  • 20:34patterns of interrelationships
  • 20:35among these three networks.
  • 20:37They show up a lot in cognitive neuroscience.
  • 20:39They show up a lot in psychiatry.
  • 20:40But it turns out there's
  • 20:42interrelatedness between them, right?
  • 20:44So you can have usually when signals
  • 20:46in the default mode network go up,
  • 20:48the lateral front file goes
  • 20:50down and vice versa.
  • 20:51But it turns out you can often
  • 20:53predict what's going to happen in
  • 20:55these networks based on signals
  • 20:57from the anterior insular cortex
  • 20:58which we think of almost as a
  • 21:00causal outflow hub like driving or
  • 21:03orchestrating the changes between
  • 21:04these other large scale networks.
  • 21:06So it's a a model we've been playing
  • 21:08around with now for a number of years.
  • 21:10And if we think about the development
  • 21:13of some of these networks and
  • 21:15development of cognitive flexibility,
  • 21:16again I mentioned this nice data
  • 21:18set that includes several 100
  • 21:20participants between 6 and 85
  • 21:22resting to data from Rai data.
  • 21:23And we can actually look at relationships
  • 21:26between their brain dynamics and some
  • 21:28other executive function measures.
  • 21:29In this particular data set,
  • 21:31the Dallas Kaplan executive
  • 21:33function test was conducted.
  • 21:35So participants are kind of going
  • 21:37from letters to numbers in sequential
  • 21:40order and we were interested here in
  • 21:42the relationship between the brain
  • 21:44dynamics of those three systems I
  • 21:46mentioned and performance on this task.
  • 21:48And so we used a different instead
  • 21:50of sliding window,
  • 21:51we used a Co activation pattern
  • 21:53analysis approach,
  • 21:54which also allows you to kind
  • 21:56of cluster time frames based on
  • 21:58their spatial similarity and look
  • 22:00at functionally relevant patterns
  • 22:02at the whole brain,
  • 22:04not using a sliding window.
  • 22:05But you can still get things like dwell
  • 22:07time transitions between States and
  • 22:09frequency of occurrence of states.
  • 22:11And we found for example that there's
  • 22:13some patterns like involving the
  • 22:15lateral front to parietal executive
  • 22:17and the medial front to parietal
  • 22:19default mode that show AU shaped kind
  • 22:21of trajectory across the lifespan.
  • 22:23So you see kind of Co activation of
  • 22:26these networks a lot more in midlife
  • 22:28than in young individuals or in older age.
  • 22:30And also this transitions or the
  • 22:32switching between brain states
  • 22:34seems to be linked with individual
  • 22:36differences in the behavior on
  • 22:38that executive function test.
  • 22:39So in in middle age essentially you
  • 22:42know between 25 and 45 or thereabouts,
  • 22:45you actually you have pretty high
  • 22:48flexibility performance regardless
  • 22:49of what's happening in terms of
  • 22:51the brain state transitions.
  • 22:53But in children and in older adults,
  • 22:55greater number of transitions between
  • 22:57brain states is associated with better
  • 22:59levels of cognitive flexibility.
  • 23:01So there's like really different
  • 23:02things going on at different points
  • 23:04in life where at some some junctures
  • 23:06it's really important to have
  • 23:07a lot more of these brain state
  • 23:09transitions to support flexible behaviors.
  • 23:11But at other points they're not really as
  • 23:15dependent on this type of brain flexibility.
  • 23:19So this is an example where we used Co
  • 23:22activation patterns to show that executive
  • 23:25and default networks change in terms of
  • 23:28their representation across the lifespan,
  • 23:30in terms of their frequency of of occurrence.
  • 23:33And that the brain state transitions
  • 23:35between these networks seem to be related
  • 23:38to cognitive flexibility in different
  • 23:40ways at different stages of the lifespan.
  • 23:42So if you if you don't mind now
  • 23:44we'll go straight basic neuroscience
  • 23:46for a little while.
  • 23:47Because I think part of you know
  • 23:49when you have these findings
  • 23:50you see different developmental
  • 23:52differences or clinical differences.
  • 23:53It's tempting to, you know,
  • 23:55go forth and, you know,
  • 23:58make inferences and go straight
  • 23:59to intervention or, you know,
  • 24:01whatever the case may be.
  • 24:02But I also think it's it's nice
  • 24:03to to take a step back and say,
  • 24:05OK, what are these brain regions
  • 24:06doing in a broader context?
  • 24:08What do we know about the anatomy
  • 24:10and function of some of these areas
  • 24:12that we seem to be implicated,
  • 24:14you know, trans diagnostically
  • 24:17across flexibility deficits.
  • 24:19So the ancillar cortex in particular
  • 24:21has kind of caught my attention.
  • 24:23If you look at the, you know,
  • 24:25neuroimaging literature,
  • 24:26it often shows up in studies of affect,
  • 24:29empathy, pain, emotion.
  • 24:30You'll see often a talk of an empathy
  • 24:33network that involves these brain regions.
  • 24:36You'll also see a subject of awareness,
  • 24:38Introception,
  • 24:38somatic sensory processes.
  • 24:40I I forgot to put disgust in here.
  • 24:43But you know a lot of things happen
  • 24:45in the insular cortex that are a lot
  • 24:47to do with basic sensory processing,
  • 24:49but also very high level cognitive
  • 24:53processing, things like inhibition,
  • 24:55attention switching and conflict
  • 24:57executive function as we've been
  • 24:59talking about all of those processes
  • 25:01also activate the insular cortex.
  • 25:02So it's it's one of those areas
  • 25:05where everybody has their favorite
  • 25:06thing to say about it.
  • 25:08And so there,
  • 25:09there tends to be a lot of literature
  • 25:11that doesn't really talk to each
  • 25:12other when we talk about the insular
  • 25:14cortex because it's a little bit
  • 25:16siloed in these different fields.
  • 25:19But if you look at just a question of like,
  • 25:21are there subdivisions within the insula?
  • 25:23There's anywhere between 2 and 27
  • 25:25depending on what study you look at.
  • 25:27But it's true,
  • 25:28there's a lot of subdivisions in the insula.
  • 25:31If you look at just resting state
  • 25:33F MRI and ask the question which
  • 25:35voxels in the insular cortex
  • 25:36have similar patterns of whole
  • 25:38brain functional connectivity.
  • 25:39So you're clustering the voxels
  • 25:40based on their whole brain patterns.
  • 25:42There's two studies from Bendine
  • 25:44and Luke Chang that suggest you
  • 25:46can at least find evidence for a
  • 25:48dorsal anterior insula subdivision,
  • 25:49a ventral anterior insula,
  • 25:51a posterior insula.
  • 25:52Like I said,
  • 25:53maybe up to 27 depending on
  • 25:55which Atlas you look at.
  • 25:56So right now let's go with three for now.
  • 26:00So if you look at these subdivisions there,
  • 26:02they do seem to be some kind of
  • 26:05structure to their their their
  • 26:07division of of Labor in the sense.
  • 26:10So in Luke Chang's meta analysis he
  • 26:13found that the ventral anterior insula
  • 26:15seems to be more involved in studies
  • 26:17in the using the terms that are in red.
  • 26:20So emotion, face,
  • 26:21anxiety sort of affective types
  • 26:23of terminology tend to go along
  • 26:25with the ventral anterior insula
  • 26:27in terms of activation.
  • 26:28On the right is a meta analytic
  • 26:31connectivity modeling study that
  • 26:32we conducted some 10 years ago,
  • 26:34which was just asking the question
  • 26:36across many, many F MRI studies.
  • 26:38When you see ventral anterior insula active,
  • 26:40what else in the brain tends to
  • 26:43coactive coactivate with that?
  • 26:44It does tend to be the sort of limbic
  • 26:46kinds of regions that are coactive.
  • 26:49If you go ahead then to look at
  • 26:51dorsal anterior insula,
  • 26:51that's the one that seems to show up
  • 26:54in studies with the term switching
  • 26:56error processing inhibition.
  • 26:58Those are the sort of higher cognitive
  • 27:01executive function types of terms.
  • 27:03And if you look on the right,
  • 27:05that's the coactivation map of dorsal,
  • 27:07anterior and slow which tends
  • 27:08to coactivate with frontal,
  • 27:09parietal cortices and temporal as well.
  • 27:12And then if you go back a little
  • 27:14bit to the posterior insula,
  • 27:15that tends to be the one for pain,
  • 27:17somatosensory,
  • 27:18that kind of more sensory based
  • 27:21kind of cognition.
  • 27:22And it also seems to coactivate
  • 27:25with somatosensory cortices.
  • 27:26So on first glance,
  • 27:28there are the patterns of
  • 27:30coactivation and meta analytic types
  • 27:33of analysis give us some evidence
  • 27:35that the insular cortex can be
  • 27:37subdivided a little bit even still,
  • 27:39like all of these subdivisions are
  • 27:40actually active across all of these
  • 27:42cognitive domains I mentioned.
  • 27:43So there's there's kind of a convergence
  • 27:46and divergent at the same time.
  • 27:48If you go ahead and look at
  • 27:50the dynamics of the functional
  • 27:51connectivity of these subdivisions,
  • 27:53there's four here instead of three.
  • 27:55It's independent component analysis,
  • 27:56it's not too important.
  • 27:58But if you again if you look
  • 27:59at the dorsal anterior insula,
  • 28:01it's doing something different here.
  • 28:02That's the one in red in this top figure.
  • 28:05And if you look at the pattern of
  • 28:07the these subdivisions and how they
  • 28:09connect with other parts of the brain,
  • 28:11there's sometimes this is
  • 28:12resting state up from rye.
  • 28:13Again the there's some states in which
  • 28:15all of the subdivisions are very
  • 28:17similar like state three in the middle.
  • 28:19But other states like state 2
  • 28:21which only occur 5% of the time,
  • 28:23which are much more divergent,
  • 28:25which means the insular subdivisions
  • 28:26are acting differently during that
  • 28:28particular connectivity state.
  • 28:30And the dorsal anterior insula is
  • 28:32the most functionally flexible
  • 28:33of all these subdivisions.
  • 28:34So that's the one where if you
  • 28:36look at different states,
  • 28:37it's interacting with different brain region,
  • 28:39has more connectivity partners
  • 28:40with the rest of the cortex than
  • 28:43the other subdivisions.
  • 28:44And that's actually the same
  • 28:46dorsal anterior insula as that
  • 28:47same subdivision where we found
  • 28:48that it you can use
  • 28:50signals from that area to estimate
  • 28:52what's going to happen at later time
  • 28:53points in other parts of the brain.
  • 28:55So that's that causal outflow hub that
  • 28:57we identified many years ago using
  • 29:00things like Granger causal analysis.
  • 29:01So there's something curious
  • 29:03about the dorsal anterior insula.
  • 29:04I'll leave it at that.
  • 29:07And again, there's some interesting
  • 29:08structural connections there that you
  • 29:10don't see in other parts of the insulin.
  • 29:11There's some frontal and and subcortical
  • 29:14projections that are detectable from
  • 29:16the dorsal anterior insula that we
  • 29:19don't see for the other subdivisions.
  • 29:21So I I wanted to take that little segue
  • 29:24into anatomy and and you know just
  • 29:26the basic architecture of some brain
  • 29:28areas because sometimes I think we'll
  • 29:31see something like insula activation,
  • 29:32but we may not look very carefully at which
  • 29:35subdivision right or left what you know,
  • 29:37what are we actually talking about.
  • 29:38So I think being a little more precise
  • 29:41on on where these things are going on
  • 29:43really helps us you know hone in on
  • 29:46what what the functional or dysfunction
  • 29:48might be in a particular population.
  • 29:50And so the ancillary cortex in adults can
  • 29:53be divided into at least dorsal anterior,
  • 29:55posterior and ventral anterior subdivision,
  • 29:58probably more with the dorsal seeming
  • 30:00to be more involved in the high level
  • 30:02cognitive control or executive function.
  • 30:04Things we've been talking about with the
  • 30:07ventral a little bit more involved in
  • 30:09the affective and emotional processing and
  • 30:11the posterior more involved in somatic
  • 30:13sensation and the dorsal anterior insulin.
  • 30:15To me that's the special one which shows
  • 30:17the most variable functional connections,
  • 30:20kind of the greatest level of functional
  • 30:22dynamics if you will and the greatest
  • 30:25diversity in terms of the the brain
  • 30:27regions it interacts with and and the
  • 30:29test domains in which it's engaged and
  • 30:32has some unique structural connections
  • 30:34that we think might underlie some
  • 30:36of that functional flexibility.
  • 30:37So I was very heartened to see this
  • 30:39paper from insulin and cut birth a
  • 30:42while ago where you know they start
  • 30:44talking about R doc and dimensional
  • 30:46models and thinking about breaking
  • 30:48down symptom based categories in
  • 30:51psychiatry into a process where we
  • 30:53where we're now all very familiar with.
  • 30:55We're thinking about using genetic risk,
  • 30:56brain activity,
  • 30:57other markers to stratify samples
  • 30:59into more data-driven categories
  • 31:01which with the idea that this will
  • 31:03eventually be good for us to identify
  • 31:06subgroups that are more amenable to
  • 31:08treatment on in one form or the other.
  • 31:10The only reason I show this slide though
  • 31:12is because in that figure from that paper,
  • 31:14they have the insular cortex
  • 31:16and I didn't plant that there.
  • 31:18I had nothing to do with this,
  • 31:20and I think it's probably just
  • 31:21a random coincidence.
  • 31:22But if I were them,
  • 31:24I would also focus on the insular cortex
  • 31:26in in all of my studies because it it
  • 31:29does seem to be very much you know,
  • 31:31whether you're interested in schizophrenia,
  • 31:33anxiety, addiction,
  • 31:34I mean autism,
  • 31:36it doesn't matter you'll you
  • 31:37can find papers that talk
  • 31:39about the insula structure and function
  • 31:41and connectivity that are aberrant or
  • 31:43atypical or what have you in that disorder.
  • 31:45So I I think part of my sort of crusade
  • 31:48nowadays is to try to get people to read
  • 31:51outside of their favorite disorder and
  • 31:53and think about bigger picture models
  • 31:55of brain function that can help us
  • 31:57understand in a more domain general way.
  • 31:59Like what could be the consequence of
  • 32:02of something going wrong in this system
  • 32:04and how would that look in early life?
  • 32:05How would that look in late life?
  • 32:07Which subdivision are we talking
  • 32:09about and what how consequential
  • 32:10is that for a particular behavior?
  • 32:12So I love this figure had nothing
  • 32:15to do with it.
  • 32:16But I promise so now just to to get back
  • 32:21to the the autism arm of of things we do.
  • 32:24If I have time, OK, I do.
  • 32:27I I like to think about flexibility and
  • 32:29autism because I know there's so much
  • 32:31great work done on social cognition and
  • 32:33social communication and language and autism.
  • 32:35But but I think a little bit
  • 32:37less is known about flexibility,
  • 32:39repetitive behaviors and their
  • 32:41brain bases for various reasons.
  • 32:43And now we're thinking more about this.
  • 32:45And I think when you get into this
  • 32:48transition to adulthood in in autism,
  • 32:50you see some of these outcomes
  • 32:53that are surprisingly not optimal.
  • 32:55So you see things like 80% of
  • 32:57individuals who are diagnosed with
  • 32:59autism don't live outside the home
  • 33:00or live independently as they,
  • 33:02you know, become 18 and older.
  • 33:04You also see these kind of
  • 33:06surprisingly low employment rates.
  • 33:08So you know kind of 80% unemployment
  • 33:11in in young adults with autism and
  • 33:13that's that's really shocking and
  • 33:14and not what we would have hoped for
  • 33:16especially if you look at some of
  • 33:19these other early life disabilities
  • 33:21where you see better outcomes.
  • 33:23It's it's surprising to see these
  • 33:25kinds of stats and and I think
  • 33:28there are treatments,
  • 33:29evidence based programs,
  • 33:30school based programs that help to
  • 33:32train up executive function and help
  • 33:35to train cognitive flexibility.
  • 33:37And I think some of that is is what
  • 33:39we have to start thinking about
  • 33:41like when and how to deploy these
  • 33:44kinds of interventions.
  • 33:45And we do have a lot of things like
  • 33:47social skills trading and a lot of
  • 33:50stuff that's really effective at at
  • 33:52ameliorating some of those difficulties.
  • 33:54But there's I think a lot more work
  • 33:55to be done here in in flexibility.
  • 33:57And the reason I think that
  • 33:59flexibility in particular is,
  • 34:01is what hinders people in that transition
  • 34:03to independence is because that's
  • 34:05sort of a a lot of what you do when
  • 34:07you're like moving out of the home,
  • 34:09getting a new job, making friends,
  • 34:11social relationships,
  • 34:11a lot of that is kind of moment
  • 34:14to moment adjustment of behavior
  • 34:16and and flexible cognition.
  • 34:18Really it's not just social
  • 34:20cognition but like you know,
  • 34:21if the bus doesn't show up
  • 34:22today and I need to get to work,
  • 34:24I better scramble to find a different
  • 34:26way to get to work. Otherwise,
  • 34:27you know you're going to get fired.
  • 34:29So. So things like that I think
  • 34:32really do come into Stark.
  • 34:34You know, we, we kind of noticed them more
  • 34:38during these transition phases of life.
  • 34:39But there's also these issues,
  • 34:41not a clinician,
  • 34:42but those who are will tell me that there's,
  • 34:45you know, clearly heterogeneity and autism.
  • 34:47So not everyone has flexibility,
  • 34:48deficits, not everyone even shows,
  • 34:50you know, executive function on problems,
  • 34:53on these sort of parent reports,
  • 34:55classic measures like the Behavioral
  • 34:57Rating Inventory of Executive function.
  • 34:59This is a data set from Stuart
  • 35:01Mostofsky's group at Kennedy Krieger,
  • 35:03and he's got kids between 8:00 and 12:00.
  • 35:06Some of them are diagnosed with autism,
  • 35:07some with ADHD,
  • 35:08some with autism and ADHD and
  • 35:10some typically developing.
  • 35:12And if you do a latent profile
  • 35:14analysis across these behavioral
  • 35:15measures of executive function,
  • 35:17you'll I like to focus here on the
  • 35:19middle pie just to show you that a lot
  • 35:21of kids with these diagnosis have just
  • 35:23totally average executive function.
  • 35:25They're not impaired.
  • 35:26They're doing as well as a
  • 35:28typically developing kid.
  • 35:29In fact,
  • 35:30the impaired kids tend to be
  • 35:32those ASDADHD Comorbid kids.
  • 35:33Perhaps not surprisingly,
  • 35:35that double diagnosis really
  • 35:37impairs or is associated with
  • 35:39the impaired executive function.
  • 35:41We found this kind of mixed bag
  • 35:43as well in our own data set,
  • 35:45typically developing kids and
  • 35:46kids with autism.
  • 35:47This was an honors thesis student
  • 35:49from a student, Adriana Baez,
  • 35:51who then went on to Med school.
  • 35:53So I had a lot of, just incidentally,
  • 35:54a lot of great undergrads who
  • 35:56write papers as first author,
  • 35:58and so that's been really exciting to see.
  • 36:00But anyway,
  • 36:01this was her honors thesis,
  • 36:02and she showed again that a lot of kids with
  • 36:06autism do have average executive functions.
  • 36:08So it's there's heterogeneity there,
  • 36:11as one might have expected.
  • 36:12Not everyone needs the same
  • 36:13kinds of interventions,
  • 36:14right?
  • 36:15So what if we could figure out what are
  • 36:19the signatures of this heterogeneity?
  • 36:20Is it something as simple as
  • 36:22brain state transitions that are
  • 36:24related to flexible behaviors?
  • 36:26Is that something that's a marker
  • 36:28of flexibility and can we see
  • 36:30that in in our data sets?
  • 36:32So the first thing we tested was,
  • 36:33do kids or individuals who are
  • 36:35diagnosed with autism show a
  • 36:37reduction in the number of brain
  • 36:39state transitions compared to the
  • 36:41typically developing individuals?
  • 36:42And so I know you're probably familiar
  • 36:45with the Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange.
  • 36:47So this was something that Adriana
  • 36:49Dimartino started a while back
  • 36:51trying to get all of us autism
  • 36:54researchers to share our data,
  • 36:55make them publicly available.
  • 36:57And this has resulted in hundreds of data
  • 37:00sets being now available for download.
  • 37:01So you can do things like
  • 37:03independent component analysis,
  • 37:05lighting,
  • 37:05widow connectivity.
  • 37:06And initially in this data set we
  • 37:09do find some evidence for small but
  • 37:11significant reduction in the number
  • 37:13of brain state transitions for those
  • 37:15diagnosed with autism compared to the
  • 37:18typically developing individuals.
  • 37:19This was another Emily Marshall in our
  • 37:22lab as an undergrad honors thesis wrote.
  • 37:25A paper is now a Med student doing great.
  • 37:28So she looked at the dynamics of this
  • 37:30salience or cingular insular network
  • 37:32and found again a reduction in the
  • 37:35frequency of brain states involving
  • 37:37this network and its Co activation with
  • 37:41others in a study she did as part of
  • 37:43her honors thesis a few years back.
  • 37:45So, so you know we are starting
  • 37:47to find these signatures in the
  • 37:49brain imaging data as well.
  • 37:51And if again if you look at a task
  • 37:53that was the last one was resting
  • 37:54state but this is a task where you've
  • 37:56got kids doing a a set shifting task,
  • 37:58it's actually pretty easy.
  • 38:00This was I think BJ Casey first came
  • 38:02up with this paradigm but it's you
  • 38:04know you see circles and squares and
  • 38:06you have to pick the odd one out.
  • 38:07Sometimes it's the odd one out
  • 38:09is different based on shapes,
  • 38:10sometimes it's color and in mixed
  • 38:12blocks it could be shape or color.
  • 38:13So that's the set shifting kind
  • 38:16of part of it.
  • 38:17It's a cognitive flexibility test.
  • 38:20It's relatively easy to do and in fact
  • 38:21all of our kids can do it easily.
  • 38:23So behaviorally this isn't too challenging.
  • 38:26But what we find is that if you look
  • 38:28at Co activation patterns between the
  • 38:30executive and salience and default
  • 38:32three kind of networks I've been focusing on,
  • 38:34kids with autism can do the task
  • 38:36and their brains are doing the task.
  • 38:38Similarly to typically developing
  • 38:39kids as they go on and on and get
  • 38:41into the fourth run of the task
  • 38:43they actually have to engage more of
  • 38:44this front of pride all there's more
  • 38:47frequent patterns of that network occurring.
  • 38:49So it's like they they can do things
  • 38:51but the strategies might be different
  • 38:52or the brain networks involved might
  • 38:54change over time in ways that we can't
  • 38:56see it with a whole brain activation
  • 38:58analysis and we actually can't even see
  • 39:00with a functional connectivity analysis.
  • 39:02But we can see with these like
  • 39:04smaller wind approaches that look at
  • 39:06dynamics and don't look at averages
  • 39:09but look at like changes across time.
  • 39:11So that's something we've we found
  • 39:13and are sort of following up on.
  • 39:15So this is kind of a full circle about
  • 39:19thinking about dynamic functional
  • 39:20connectivity approaches to reveal a
  • 39:23typical patterns of brain dynamics in
  • 39:25prevalent neurodevelopmental conditions
  • 39:26characterized by cognitive inflexibility
  • 39:28like autism as I mentioned here.
  • 39:31And in the extent to which these
  • 39:33individual differences in brain dynamics
  • 39:35underlie individual differences in
  • 39:37flexible behaviors is is something
  • 39:39we're we're actively investigating as
  • 39:40well as as I'll mention in a moment
  • 39:43other moderators or mediators of these
  • 39:46relationships between brain and behavior.
  • 39:48So this whole time before 2021 we
  • 39:52were conducting this work in Miami,
  • 39:54which has a huge bilingual population.
  • 39:57So most of our participants actually
  • 39:59were Spanish,
  • 40:00English, bilingual and so one one thing
  • 40:02we did which was completely unplanned.
  • 40:05Was that we were able to test the
  • 40:08relationship between bilingual
  • 40:08exposure and executive function
  • 40:10in our children in the study.
  • 40:12So the RO one was focused
  • 40:13on cognitive flexibility.
  • 40:14Not just nothing to do with language,
  • 40:16nothing to do with bilingualism,
  • 40:18but just the way the the sampling occurred.
  • 40:20We had a lot of this
  • 40:22population in our sample.
  • 40:23So it turns out there's a lot of controversy
  • 40:26about whether or not raising a child
  • 40:29with developmental language delays,
  • 40:30whether raising them in a bilingual
  • 40:32home will result in slowing down of
  • 40:34language and cognitive development.
  • 40:36But there's been a lot more work
  • 40:39recently suggesting not only are there
  • 40:42no negative consequences of either
  • 40:44in cognitive or language development
  • 40:45for being raised in a bilingual home,
  • 40:47but there might even be mitigation
  • 40:49of some flexibility deficits.
  • 40:50And in fact,
  • 40:51some folks are starting to report fewer
  • 40:54executive function problems in dual
  • 40:56language households or autism kids with
  • 40:58autism raised in dual language household.
  • 41:00So this is to me a really exciting
  • 41:03kind of untapped potential to explore.
  • 41:05It's almost like a natural intervention.
  • 41:08So in the earlier days,
  • 41:10I think social or sorry,
  • 41:12communication disorders,
  • 41:13clinicians would say, OK,
  • 41:15if your child is having language delay,
  • 41:17don't expose them to multiple languages,
  • 41:18just stick with one.
  • 41:20We don't want to confuse them.
  • 41:21But it turns out that wasn't the best wisdom.
  • 41:24I mean, there's really no negative effect.
  • 41:26There might even be these boosts in executive
  • 41:28function that we we can encourage with this.
  • 41:30Not to mention all the other
  • 41:32benefits of bilingualism.
  • 41:33Right.
  • 41:33So we found, you know,
  • 41:34in our initial study with Celia Romero,
  • 41:37she's just about to hopefully have this
  • 41:40paper accepted just initial evidence.
  • 41:42When you get to typically developing kids,
  • 41:44there's almost a ceiling effect if
  • 41:45they're good at executive function.
  • 41:46The bilingualism doesn't really change that.
  • 41:50It sort of, you know,
  • 41:50doesn't affect anything at all.
  • 41:53But if you look here,
  • 41:53it's just one scale of the behavior
  • 41:55rating inventory of executive function.
  • 41:57If you look at it,
  • 41:57the kids with autism being from
  • 42:00a bilingual home was associated
  • 42:02with better inhibitory control
  • 42:04than being from a monolingual home
  • 42:07in in that particular population.
  • 42:09So there may be some boosts or
  • 42:11protective effects or what have you
  • 42:13in certain conditions that we don't
  • 42:14see in a typically developing case
  • 42:16because they may already be at ceiling
  • 42:18or they may not be affected in such a way.
  • 42:21So this is something we finally
  • 42:23got an R21 to do and believe me,
  • 42:25nobody wanted to fund this for some reason.
  • 42:28But we're finally starting to
  • 42:29collect data to really explore
  • 42:31the links between bilingualism,
  • 42:33executive function and brain
  • 42:34development in 8 to 12 year old
  • 42:36children with autism now at UCLA.
  • 42:38And we put in an RO one to look
  • 42:40at this longitudinally.
  • 42:42So we're doing the data collection
  • 42:43on the first set
  • 42:44and then we want to follow them
  • 42:45up across that adolescent phase,
  • 42:47keep keep them going into
  • 42:48young adulthood if possible.
  • 42:49But we want to see how, you know,
  • 42:52the initial bilingual exposure kind
  • 42:54of plays out across this interesting
  • 42:56period of adolescence when things are
  • 42:59changing executive function, you know,
  • 43:01deficits might be becoming more prevalent.
  • 43:03We want to see if there
  • 43:04are protective effect.
  • 43:05Is there a bilingual advantage?
  • 43:06Is there a boost?
  • 43:08Nobody knows.
  • 43:08And there's like no studies at all
  • 43:10about bilingualism and the influence
  • 43:12on brain development and autism.
  • 43:14There's nothing that exists.
  • 43:16So that's something we're really,
  • 43:19as far as data collection goes,
  • 43:20that's where we're going
  • 43:21in the next few years.
  • 43:23The last thing I wanted to
  • 43:25mention was this is like,
  • 43:26and now for something completely different.
  • 43:29So we have so many of you know I've
  • 43:31I've become super involved with the
  • 43:33adolescent brain cognitive development study.
  • 43:35So I'm the one of the associate
  • 43:37directors now for justice,
  • 43:39equity, diversity,
  • 43:40inclusion for the entire consortium.
  • 43:42In addition to the 20 hours of
  • 43:44meetings a week that entails also
  • 43:46a lot of knowledge about the ABCD
  • 43:48data set that now we can use as a
  • 43:51lab to to further our own goals.
  • 43:52So this is the study of, you know,
  • 43:5410,000 youth, Yale as a site,
  • 43:56UCLA as a site.
  • 43:58They're started in 2016 and now the
  • 44:00kids were 9 and 10 at the first
  • 44:02wave and now they're about 16/17/18.
  • 44:05So it's 10 year longitudinal
  • 44:08study imaging neurocog,
  • 44:10substance abuse substance use data.
  • 44:13So and I won't.
  • 44:14I won't try to embarrass myself by
  • 44:15pretending to know anything about addiction.
  • 44:17This crowd knows about addiction.
  • 44:19I don't.
  • 44:19But what I do want to do is
  • 44:21think about salience,
  • 44:22network dynamics and how they might be
  • 44:25related to substance use initiation.
  • 44:27And I swear this wasn't my idea.
  • 44:29A program officer asked me to do this study.
  • 44:31So we're trying to do is is look
  • 44:33at precursors of substance use
  • 44:35initiation using this ABCD data set.
  • 44:38Cause at 9:00 and 10:00 the
  • 44:39kids aren't really doing much.
  • 44:40They might,
  • 44:41a few of them report sipping alcohol,
  • 44:42but as you follow them up over
  • 44:44time you can see, you know,
  • 44:46they start to do things like try out smoking,
  • 44:48try out this and that, and things change.
  • 44:50And that's what this study has meant to
  • 44:52track like mental health trajectories
  • 44:53and substance use initiation trajectories.
  • 44:55And what we want to do is use
  • 44:57predictive modeling.
  • 44:58Of course,
  • 44:58connectome based predictive
  • 44:59modeling was invented here at Yale,
  • 45:01you all know about it.
  • 45:02But we want to use dynamic connectome
  • 45:04based predictive modeling to try to
  • 45:07see if anything about the salience
  • 45:08network and its initial state
  • 45:10predicts vulnerability to later
  • 45:12substance use initiation using the
  • 45:14machine learning and some of the
  • 45:17connectivity approaches that I mentioned.
  • 45:19And we think this will be interesting
  • 45:21because as far as I know about addiction,
  • 45:23most of a lot of the studies
  • 45:24that are done
  • 45:25in adults aren't able to really tease
  • 45:27apart whether the brain difference
  • 45:28is seen are the consequence or
  • 45:30the cause of the of the addiction.
  • 45:33So you see kind of what happens
  • 45:35after years of of, you know,
  • 45:37being a user of some substance,
  • 45:38but we don't know necessarily if
  • 45:41there's like some vulnerability
  • 45:42in some circuits or things like
  • 45:44that that pre preceded the onset.
  • 45:46And so the adolescent brain cognitive
  • 45:48development study actually allows
  • 45:49you to do that 'cause you'll,
  • 45:50you might, you know,
  • 45:51be able to predict what some of
  • 45:53these vulnerabilities are related
  • 45:55to inhibition and and you know,
  • 45:57the networks that are involved in in
  • 45:59flexibility and things like that.
  • 46:00So this is something we're trying
  • 46:02to get funding to do as well.
  • 46:04Just pure data analysis of the ABCD
  • 46:07study and we'll see how that goes.
  • 46:10But basically as I start out talking
  • 46:12about why we're interested in flexibility
  • 46:15deficits and flexible behaviors can
  • 46:18be impact day-to-day activities,
  • 46:20forming and maintaining relationships,
  • 46:22employment and independent living.
  • 46:24So we think that understanding the neural
  • 46:27mechanisms underlying flexibility deficits,
  • 46:28you know across autism is our big focus.
  • 46:30But across lots of clinical
  • 46:32conditions we think can be important
  • 46:35for tailoring therapies,
  • 46:36predicting responses,
  • 46:37thinking about risk and resilience for
  • 46:40for various things like addiction.
  • 46:43And so eventually,
  • 46:44you know our kids can can focus
  • 46:47more about their interactions in
  • 46:49the world and less less on the
  • 46:51specific repetitive behaviors and
  • 46:53routines that might hold them back.
  • 46:55And so this is the lab at UCLA who does
  • 46:58all the work and my collaboration at
  • 47:00work on the right some of the funding
  • 47:02and the people who have donated time data,
  • 47:06code of resources,
  • 47:08mentorship and collaboration
  • 47:09and friendship over the years.
  • 47:11And this is the not where I do my work,
  • 47:14but this is, this is UCLA.
  • 47:16And I'm really happy to take any
  • 47:18questions and have discussions.
  • 47:19Thanks again.