Skip to Main Content

"Network Physiology and Pathology of Sleep" Robert Thomas, MD (09/14/2022)

November 22, 2022
  • 00:00Okeydoke. Let's see.
  • 00:04Good afternoon, everybody.
  • 00:06Can you guys hear me? Oh, excellent.
  • 00:09Nice to see all the familiar faces.
  • 00:13Welcome back from the spring break
  • 00:15and my name is Andrea Zinchuk and
  • 00:18I wanted to welcome all of you
  • 00:21to our Joint Sleep Conference,
  • 00:23which is a seminar that's held
  • 00:25between the Yale Beth Israel,
  • 00:27Brigham Women's MGH, BMC Tough sleep centers.
  • 00:29And so we have grown over the last couple
  • 00:32of years and they will continue to grow and.
  • 00:35Excited to talk to you guys today about our.
  • 00:40Great Speaker Doctor Robert Thomas.
  • 00:41But before I do that, I just want
  • 00:43to make a couple of announcements.
  • 00:441st, Please take a moment to
  • 00:46make sure that you're muted.
  • 00:48And to receive CME credit,
  • 00:50please see the chat room for instructions.
  • 00:52And if you're not already
  • 00:54registered with the CME,
  • 00:55you will need to do that first.
  • 00:57That there will be a code in there
  • 01:00that you can text to the phone
  • 01:02number for CME to get credit.
  • 01:04And so the recording of the session
  • 01:06will usually be available online
  • 01:08within two weeks and there will be a
  • 01:10link provided in the chat for that.
  • 01:11And if you have questions during the talk,
  • 01:13please make sure to make use of the
  • 01:17chat chat room throughout the hour.
  • 01:18I will moderate it as well.
  • 01:21And so it is a without further ado,
  • 01:24it is a great pleasure for me
  • 01:26to introduce our next speaker,
  • 01:27which is Doctor Robert Thomas from
  • 01:30Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center.
  • 01:33And Harvard School of Medicine.
  • 01:36And it's a real pleasure to have
  • 01:40Robert speak here.
  • 01:41Because Robert is turns out to be
  • 01:43a mentor of mine and he is the one
  • 01:45who got me interested in sleep,
  • 01:46medicine and.
  • 01:47Kept encouraging me to stay
  • 01:50and stick with research,
  • 01:51which has been a wonderful endeavor
  • 01:54for me and I've collaborated with
  • 01:56Robert over the last ten years or so.
  • 01:58And so Robert is one of the foremost
  • 02:01experts in sleep to sort of breathing
  • 02:03in the United States and worldwide.
  • 02:06And he had coined the term
  • 02:09treatment emergent central sleep
  • 02:11apnea or complex sleep apnea and
  • 02:13describe it first in the mid 2000s.
  • 02:16He has published.
  • 02:17You know nearly 100 articles and
  • 02:20many books and is editor of the.
  • 02:23Landmarks book by Doctor Krieger and others
  • 02:26principles and practice of Sleep Medicine.
  • 02:30He's edited sleep disorder breathing
  • 02:32section and is a recipient of
  • 02:34many grants over the past several
  • 02:36years from various foundations,
  • 02:38including the NIH.
  • 02:40And so without further ado,
  • 02:43I'm going to let Robert take
  • 02:44over since he has some very
  • 02:46fascinating things to share with
  • 02:47us on his thoughts about network
  • 02:50Physiology and pathology of sleep.
  • 02:52So thank you again everyone for attending.
  • 02:54Welcome Robert and looking
  • 02:56forward to this wonderful talk.
  • 02:59Thank you, Andre.
  • 03:00And I'm of course very happy to be
  • 03:03here to share with you some of my.
  • 03:06More recent thinking and learning.
  • 03:10In the area of sleep
  • 03:13Physiology and pathology.
  • 03:14So it turns out that anybody
  • 03:17who deals with sleep signs,
  • 03:20Sleep Medicine, sleep pathology,
  • 03:23sleep treatments,
  • 03:24we are actually network scientists,
  • 03:28physiologists, therapists,
  • 03:29whether we like it or not.
  • 03:33Of all these specialties of
  • 03:36specialties of healthcare.
  • 03:39Sleep is the closest which comes to almost
  • 03:42automatically being a network science.
  • 03:45Much of network science and
  • 03:46network medicine has focused on
  • 03:48things like metabolic networks,
  • 03:50gene networks, epigenetic networks,
  • 03:52seemingly things which are far
  • 03:55away from touching the patient.
  • 03:57But it turns out that we have been
  • 04:00touching the network of sleep.
  • 04:02All along. When we look at sleep.
  • 04:05From the network point of view,
  • 04:08it becomes actually quite
  • 04:10interesting and fascinating,
  • 04:11and I hope I can share
  • 04:14that excitement with you.
  • 04:15And let me move my slide.
  • 04:17Here we go.
  • 04:19So my main disclosure really for this is.
  • 04:23I'll be showing you a few slides,
  • 04:24uh,
  • 04:25using technology I was involved
  • 04:27in developing cardio pulmonary
  • 04:29coupling to track.
  • 04:30In an ambulatory way,
  • 04:32the network biology of sleep.
  • 04:34But other than that, much of this is just.
  • 04:38Good stuff. OK.
  • 04:41So I will start off by saying that
  • 04:43sleep is a unique network state.
  • 04:46Just think of the.
  • 04:48Number of different functions which have to.
  • 04:52Function.
  • 04:54Together with some degree of
  • 04:56tolerance and harmony of each other.
  • 04:58To give us what is sleep?
  • 05:01Network activity may be intrinsic
  • 05:04to a subsystem.
  • 05:05Integrated or communicator?
  • 05:07In the context of sleep.
  • 05:10So as an example,
  • 05:11the respiratory network is
  • 05:13a subsystem of sleep.
  • 05:15Integrated networks are seen when you
  • 05:17have say sinus arrhythmia where you
  • 05:20have cardio pulmonary integration.
  • 05:22There can be wide scale
  • 05:24communication with spindles.
  • 05:26Long range communication from
  • 05:28the cortex to the brainstem.
  • 05:30The non R.E.M slow oscillation
  • 05:32which permeates.
  • 05:33Pretty much the whole brain.
  • 05:36These are all forms of
  • 05:38network and coupling activity.
  • 05:40Now couple Physiologies are,
  • 05:42of course,
  • 05:43networked.
  • 05:43So one could to some extent
  • 05:47interchangeably use coupling and network,
  • 05:50but networks go far beyond just coupling.
  • 05:55There are minimal.
  • 05:57Overlaps between fundamental
  • 05:59oscillatory outputs of sleep.
  • 06:02So you take the spindle frequency.
  • 06:04You take heart rate,
  • 06:06you take slow oscillation,
  • 06:08you take the cyclic alternating pattern.
  • 06:11You have oscillations in
  • 06:13the range of subsecond to.
  • 06:1630 to 40 seconds and all of these have
  • 06:19to actually occur in some coordinated
  • 06:22interactive way during sleep.
  • 06:25The components of course
  • 06:26in sleep are dispersed.
  • 06:28In biological space, right,
  • 06:30the brainstem is somewhat
  • 06:31away from the cortex.
  • 06:33There is a necessity to travel in time.
  • 06:36We sleep over time.
  • 06:39And individual subsystems have
  • 06:42different driving mechanisms.
  • 06:44So the respiratory driver is different,
  • 06:47the homeostatic driver is different,
  • 06:48the circadian driver is
  • 06:50substantially different,
  • 06:51but all of these somehow had to
  • 06:53work together like a happy family.
  • 06:58One of the most important components of
  • 07:00the brain which enables networks to work,
  • 07:03of course, is the white matter tracks which.
  • 07:07Essentially connect.
  • 07:07Everything which requires to be connected.
  • 07:10So this is just a DTI fibre track just
  • 07:13to just to remind you that without
  • 07:15a good white matter connectivity we
  • 07:18can't do networking for nuts. OK.
  • 07:21Just listing some sleep networks.
  • 07:24You know the cortical network,
  • 07:26there are more cortical loops.
  • 07:28Interthalamic network,
  • 07:30brainstem has numerous networks.
  • 07:33The respiratory network,
  • 07:35the blood pressure regulatory network,
  • 07:38the cardiac output and autonomic
  • 07:41Dr Networks you have networks for.
  • 07:45Feeding control, of course, right?
  • 07:47Extraordinary network.
  • 07:50Which is involved in sleep regulation too.
  • 07:52You have the better reflects the chemo
  • 07:55reflex, the central autonomic network.
  • 07:56You are the R.E.M non R.E.M network.
  • 07:59Sleep. To weight transition,
  • 08:01how does that occur?
  • 08:03Clearly it's a network change.
  • 08:04You have the arousal network,
  • 08:05you have respiratory,
  • 08:07generative and respiratory control networks.
  • 08:09Of course you have the motor network which
  • 08:12can cause periodic and aperiodic outputs.
  • 08:15Yeah, cardio,
  • 08:16autonomic interactions.
  • 08:17They're probably more networks,
  • 08:19but you get the point.
  • 08:20Sleep is 1 big networked family of subnets.
  • 08:28Just a very quick reminder of the
  • 08:31classic networks which we normally
  • 08:33think about when. Talk about sleep.
  • 08:35So you have. The wake network.
  • 08:39You have the orexin which is. Sort of.
  • 08:46A kind of orchestrator of sorts
  • 08:48of how these networks interact.
  • 08:50You have the non R.E.M network and
  • 08:53every several months there is some new.
  • 08:56Cell group which has been discovered
  • 08:59which regulates non REM sleep.
  • 09:02Ohh, you have the R.E.M network.
  • 09:05So you get a point.
  • 09:07Now, these networks,
  • 09:07of course you're familiar with.
  • 09:09This audience is reasonably familiar with.
  • 09:11But if you're not?
  • 09:13The movement between Wake R.E.M
  • 09:15and non R.E.M is dependent on
  • 09:18very different network behaviors.
  • 09:21And different networks which
  • 09:24are mostly non overlapping.
  • 09:27A quick word about the
  • 09:29central autonomic network.
  • 09:30It's all the way to the top of the
  • 09:33neuraxis with the entry singlet,
  • 09:34insular cortex, amygdala, hypothalamus,
  • 09:37the periaqueductal Gray matter,
  • 09:39the parabrachial complex,
  • 09:41the nucleus tractus solitarius,
  • 09:43the ventrolateral medulla.
  • 09:44These are all part of the central
  • 09:47autonomic network and it's
  • 09:49deeply engaged in numerous sleep
  • 09:52physiologies and pathologies.
  • 09:53There are multiple inputs including.
  • 09:57A visceral information which
  • 10:00is topographically arranged.
  • 10:02In the tractor Solitarius,
  • 10:04as well as chemo reflex and
  • 10:06bioflex inflammation going in,
  • 10:08as well as humoral inputs through
  • 10:11the circumventricular organs.
  • 10:12The insula and the amygdala
  • 10:15are quite high order.
  • 10:17Also the ventromedial prefrontal cortex,
  • 10:19and of course these paraventricular nucleus
  • 10:21controls specific subsets of preganglionic,
  • 10:23sympathetic and parasympathetic neurons.
  • 10:25So central Autonomic network is
  • 10:28something which doesn't get much
  • 10:30attention in clinical medicine,
  • 10:32but it's clearly extremely important
  • 10:35and certainly those who deal with
  • 10:38pain and psychotic disorders clearly
  • 10:41have more interest in this network.
  • 10:47I think I'll model it said this OK.
  • 10:50Just a quick shout out to
  • 10:53the respiratory network.
  • 10:54Today will not be a talk on the respiratory
  • 10:57network that probably deserves an entire
  • 10:58half day symposium in its own right.
  • 11:01But you have central pattern generators,
  • 11:02you have the motor output,
  • 11:04you have the breathing plant.
  • 11:05You have. Very specific.
  • 11:09Neuronal firing patterns,
  • 11:11inspiratory neuron post inspiratory firing
  • 11:14during inspiration and active expiration.
  • 11:18Post inspiration and activist exploration.
  • 11:22And of course you have
  • 11:24the read reploid nucleus,
  • 11:25the paraphasias nuclear complex,
  • 11:30the new ataxic center.
  • 11:32Again, all of these have to work as a very
  • 11:35fine network while awake and wirelessly.
  • 11:38A good example of a respiratory network
  • 11:40gone bad, of course, is opiates.
  • 11:41The opiates are not the only one you know.
  • 11:43Opiates, Baclofen, all types of
  • 11:46pathologies in the brainstem area,
  • 11:48whether to stroke, degeneration,
  • 11:51multiple sclerosis.
  • 11:53Sitting balbia and such will of
  • 11:56course disrupt the respiratory
  • 11:57network with direct impact. AI.
  • 12:00Traffic, little sclerosis, polio.
  • 12:02We don't see polio much.
  • 12:03Hopefully we will not,
  • 12:05even though there are some murmurs.
  • 12:07Now one very interesting.
  • 12:10Kind of relearned the feature is that.
  • 12:14The respiratory signal actually
  • 12:16permeates the whole brain.
  • 12:19So it turns out you can find the
  • 12:21as you breathe in and out you can
  • 12:25find the respiratory oscillation.
  • 12:27Encoded in hippocampal oscillations
  • 12:29in the olfactory bulb.
  • 12:32In the no forget what VMS.
  • 12:36In fact, I don't forget what PC is,
  • 12:38but this is the dentate.
  • 12:41Dentate nucleus.
  • 12:42So essentially when you breathe in
  • 12:44and out and the ancient side it,
  • 12:47right?
  • 12:49You can control and influence
  • 12:50the entire brain,
  • 12:51which of course influenced than
  • 12:53the body by paste breathing.
  • 12:56It would make sense that evolution decided
  • 12:58to Co opt signal as strong as respiration,
  • 13:02as fundamental as respiration
  • 13:04for other functions.
  • 13:05So respiration actually
  • 13:07provides a network signal,
  • 13:09a network coordination
  • 13:11signal throughout the brain.
  • 13:14As to sleep spindles,
  • 13:15as an example it provides.
  • 13:17A fast oscillatory network
  • 13:18information throughout the brain.
  • 13:22Hypercapnia has somewhat specific
  • 13:25network going through the lateral
  • 13:27parabrachial nucleus and this was
  • 13:29worked out by Cliff Zapus group and.
  • 13:33Uh. Directly targeting it,
  • 13:36you know, may have a role in
  • 13:39reducing arousals during sleep,
  • 13:41but nevertheless the hypercapnia
  • 13:43network seems to be separate
  • 13:45from the hypoxic network and the
  • 13:48respiratory mechanoreceptor network.
  • 13:53The Telemo cortical network, of course we.
  • 13:57No. Well, there's not need much need to spend
  • 14:00time on this in a Sleep Medicine audience.
  • 14:03But it's a very complex interaction of.
  • 14:07Tell him cortical cell conductance.
  • 14:11The hyperpolarization activated spike
  • 14:14theoretical thymic nucleus spindles,
  • 14:17as well as interaction with the one
  • 14:19to four Hertz Delta. You can think of
  • 14:22the spindles as a 5G cell network.
  • 14:25It's a carrier wave which
  • 14:27allows information to travel.
  • 14:29And allows way to see scale short
  • 14:33short time range synchronization.
  • 14:36The solation. To describe hysteria in 1991.
  • 14:42It's less than one Hertz and
  • 14:44it's on off state of the cortex,
  • 14:46which I will briefly mention later on.
  • 14:49It enables large rail, large,
  • 14:51large range synchrony of.
  • 14:54Cortical activity subcortical activity.
  • 14:58It aggregates spindles.
  • 14:59And spindles, almost certainly.
  • 15:03It's part of the biological glue of sleep.
  • 15:06So just think of the effect of a
  • 15:09benzodiazepine on sleep. Fragmented sleep.
  • 15:11You take a benzodiazepine,
  • 15:13your enormous amount of spindling,
  • 15:15but you also have. Almost a bland.
  • 15:20Uh. Cortical architecture.
  • 15:22You have ample spindling.
  • 15:25You have reduction in slow wave sleep.
  • 15:27Every epic moralist looks like the other.
  • 15:31And almost certainly it has glued the
  • 15:33sleep in a way which is fairly unique
  • 15:36through the spindling mechanisms.
  • 15:37Now if you take a sodium oxybate,
  • 15:39you're gluing it through non
  • 15:42spindling mechanisms,
  • 15:43probably direct cortical network mechanisms.
  • 15:48Oh, a spindling is not necessary
  • 15:50to increase cohesion,
  • 15:51as we know from anyone who uses an
  • 15:53oxybate or atypical antipsychotics,
  • 15:56because.
  • 15:58Network strengthening at the cortical level.
  • 16:03I will be spending a bit of time
  • 16:06on restless legs and periodic
  • 16:07limb movements as we go along.
  • 16:09It's the ultimate networked
  • 16:12Physiology and pathology.
  • 16:14And there's this really
  • 16:17elegant paper from John Liu.
  • 16:20And Patrick Fuller's group here where
  • 16:23they targeted multiple sites of ablation.
  • 16:28Across the striatum, globus pallidus.
  • 16:32And even the cortex,
  • 16:33motor cortex and showed that at all
  • 16:36of these levels you actually had.
  • 16:38The induction of theoretical movements,
  • 16:41the kind which you see with restless legs,
  • 16:44as well as as iron deficiency in rodents.
  • 16:49A classic example of a network
  • 16:53regulatory dysfunction.
  • 16:57Now the. Periodically movement network.
  • 17:00The restless legs periodic limb
  • 17:02movement network is tightly
  • 17:04linked to autonomic activation.
  • 17:05You all know that.
  • 17:07Whenever there is a periodical improvement.
  • 17:11Usually there is at least a
  • 17:13little bit of cardio activation.
  • 17:15Sometimes they're truly blind.
  • 17:17You see nothing happening.
  • 17:18But more often than not, you see.
  • 17:21Blood pressure surges.
  • 17:23You see arousals,
  • 17:24and the degree of arousal
  • 17:26correlates reasonably well with the
  • 17:28amount of blood pressure surge.
  • 17:30There's increasing evidence.
  • 17:32Suggestive evidence that adverse
  • 17:34cardiovascular outcomes are
  • 17:36epidemiologically linked to restless legs.
  • 17:39Periodically,
  • 17:39movements are really quite severe
  • 17:40in heart failure and renal failure
  • 17:42patients and likely contributes to
  • 17:44pathological and nocturnal hemodynamics.
  • 17:45Because these people really kick.
  • 17:47I mean they kick.
  • 17:48You can even without measuring
  • 17:49blood pressure,
  • 17:50you can see the applet signal,
  • 17:52you know, squeeze,
  • 17:53you can see the heart rate
  • 17:55bump up and and so on.
  • 17:56That can't be a good thing.
  • 17:59So let's spend a little bit of time on.
  • 18:04PLM's. Just a bit now, but more to come.
  • 18:08So this is this of course is an
  • 18:09example of periodic limb movements,
  • 18:11but notice how there's really
  • 18:13not much autonomic activation.
  • 18:15There's very little change in cardiac rate.
  • 18:19In fact, any change in RR.
  • 18:23Is not related to the
  • 18:26leg movements themselves.
  • 18:28You can see the plate signal
  • 18:32showing essentially very. What?
  • 18:34Actually no reduction in amplitude,
  • 18:37suggesting there is not recurrent
  • 18:39sympathetic activation with
  • 18:40these periodic limb movements.
  • 18:42So these are what I call dumb
  • 18:44periodic limb movements.
  • 18:45Dumb meaning they don't do anything.
  • 18:48I will show you later on not very
  • 18:50dumb periodical improvements.
  • 18:54This is the only classic slide
  • 18:56I will show you and this of
  • 18:58course is the two process model.
  • 19:00If that is not the ultimate
  • 19:02network interaction, what is right,
  • 19:03you have the whole sleep system now
  • 19:05interacting with the circadian system
  • 19:07which has its own down network.
  • 19:09I'm not going to be talking much about
  • 19:12circadian networks today because
  • 19:13obviously I'm not the best person for
  • 19:15that and we will not have enough time.
  • 19:17But the circadian network of course
  • 19:20interacts with the sleep network and
  • 19:22is its own entity in its own right,
  • 19:24having his own networks.
  • 19:27Uh, in the brain, in the liver,
  • 19:29in the body, you name it.
  • 19:30The circadian system is probably.
  • 19:33And even more impressive network
  • 19:35than the Sleep network.
  • 19:37But this is remind you that everybody has
  • 19:39a sleep system and a circadian system,
  • 19:41and both these network states
  • 19:43do interact immensely.
  • 19:47So how can you measure the
  • 19:48network health of sleep?
  • 19:51So it turns out that we are
  • 19:53doing this a lot of the time.
  • 19:56Classic polysomnography of course measures.
  • 20:00The Sleep network,
  • 20:01but the scoring of course,
  • 20:02does not think network.
  • 20:05The closest to network would be arousal after
  • 20:10respiratory event or desaturation linked to.
  • 20:13A respiratory event, perhaps?
  • 20:16But the standard manual very very
  • 20:19precisely tries to steer us away from
  • 20:23thinking of the brain and sleep as
  • 20:26a large scale integrated network.
  • 20:28But from classic polysomnography
  • 20:30you can certainly extract all
  • 20:32kinds of network behaviors.
  • 20:34Functional MRI or sleep very clearly
  • 20:36shows the network behavior of sleep.
  • 20:39And I'll give you a small
  • 20:40sample high density,
  • 20:41EG it's superficial cortical networks.
  • 20:46But you can do high density EEG
  • 20:49polysomnography with including,
  • 20:50you know, autonomic Dr and so on,
  • 20:52and map out how the cortical
  • 20:55activity changes with the breathing,
  • 20:57with arousals, with muscle,
  • 20:59sympathetic nerve activity and so on.
  • 21:02Death recordings, uh,
  • 21:04where they also is hemodynamics,
  • 21:06respiration, ECG can again map out.
  • 21:10Networks,
  • 21:10I'm not going to be killing you
  • 21:13with intensely mathematical.
  • 21:15Uh, displays on this talk.
  • 21:20I'm trying to keep it as user
  • 21:22friendly as possible, but there are.
  • 21:23If you go to pub Med and put
  • 21:25these keywords in,
  • 21:26you will get ample sophisticated papers,
  • 21:30which honestly many of them are over my head.
  • 21:34Analysis of coupled oscillations.
  • 21:35I will talk a little bit about that.
  • 21:37I will talk a little bit
  • 21:39about time delay stability,
  • 21:40because this is a method.
  • 21:43Described by Plaman Ave.
  • 21:46And his group from Boston University.
  • 21:49Which talks about the kind.
  • 21:52It's a measure of network strength and.
  • 21:56We look at sleep studies where there's
  • 21:58pathology across multiple systems.
  • 21:59So as an example you have.
  • 22:02Periodically movement coinciding with
  • 22:04the respiratory arousal coinciding with.
  • 22:07Cardio acceleration coinciding with
  • 22:08the cortical arousal coinciding
  • 22:10with the whole body movement.
  • 22:14The strength of that connectivity
  • 22:16can be described in different ways,
  • 22:19but one of them is called
  • 22:21the time delay stability.
  • 22:23Of course there is a graph based analysis,
  • 22:25network strength measures like path length,
  • 22:27etcetera. I will not be going into that.
  • 22:30Similarly, the integrated analytics such
  • 22:32as I'm just loosely calling the sense,
  • 22:35Wellman, Azerbaijan, Scotty,
  • 22:38Andrew and Alice computations and algorithms,
  • 22:41I will not be discussing that today,
  • 22:44but they are very dependent
  • 22:46on network connectivity.
  • 22:47The heart rate response to arousal is
  • 22:50an example that's a good example of.
  • 22:52Not calling it network,
  • 22:54but believe me it is as network as they come.
  • 22:59Just to remind you that we of course
  • 23:02measure the network all the time.
  • 23:08This is an example of.
  • 23:11The network of sleep at work.
  • 23:13So you have air flow and then you have
  • 23:15the peripheral arterial tonometry signal
  • 23:17and your arterial blood pressure.
  • 23:20Typically, when any individual
  • 23:22signal shows a major deviation,
  • 23:24a major perturbation, a major transient,
  • 23:27these others will have it at
  • 23:29the same time because they are.
  • 23:31Tightly linked. And network.
  • 23:33Now whether you call them coupled or
  • 23:35network is the same thing here anyway.
  • 23:37But what it tells you is that you can
  • 23:39use any of these individual signals in
  • 23:42the right context to predict the other.
  • 23:44So if you're sleeping and your blood
  • 23:45pressure is showing the profile,
  • 23:46which is seen here.
  • 23:48Either you have sleep apnea or
  • 23:50you have periodically movements,
  • 23:52or or someone has slapped headphones on
  • 23:54you and I'm beeping you every 35 seconds.
  • 23:58If respiration of course looks like that,
  • 24:00you know there is apnea and you
  • 24:03will almost certainly have blood
  • 24:05pressure tracking along with it.
  • 24:09A cyclic alternating pattern or
  • 24:12cap is the best. Day-to-day.
  • 24:15Example of the cortical network
  • 24:18stability or instability.
  • 24:20Remember networks can be vertical.
  • 24:22You know across different levels
  • 24:23of the brain it can be horizontal.
  • 24:26Across the same level.
  • 24:27So the slow oscillation is
  • 24:29a cortical network thing.
  • 24:33Cyclic alternating pattern is
  • 24:35both horizontally integrated as
  • 24:37well as vertically integrated.
  • 24:40So when you have. The A phase and B
  • 24:43phase alternating you have immense
  • 24:45changes in the cortical network.
  • 24:48Uh activity, and this is a period
  • 24:51of cap of cyclical training pattern.
  • 24:54Which is markedly amplified by
  • 24:56disease and here you have non cap
  • 24:58or non cyclic alternating pattern.
  • 25:00You will notice that I'm sort of
  • 25:02repackaging a lot of things that
  • 25:04you already know in a network
  • 25:06in kind of network thing.
  • 25:09Now, it's very important to note that
  • 25:11when there is a cortical perturbation,
  • 25:13there is usually a downstream perturbation.
  • 25:15So it turns out whenever you have K
  • 25:18complexes and phasic egg activity,
  • 25:20you will have an increase in blood pressure,
  • 25:21increase in heart rate,
  • 25:23increase in tidal volume,
  • 25:25the flat signal amplitude will drop.
  • 25:28You will have a blood pressure surge.
  • 25:31Maybe a little blimp,
  • 25:33maybe not necessarily a surge,
  • 25:34but these will occur very reliably as the
  • 25:38reflection of the integrated network.
  • 25:40Just to remind you that you may
  • 25:42have deep sleep which is unstable.
  • 25:44So this is M3 showing a lot of
  • 25:47intermittent phasic activity.
  • 25:49So deep and stable is now this is
  • 25:51so you can be deep and stable,
  • 25:53deep and light, light and stable and such.
  • 25:56So all all the combinations are actually
  • 25:58possible. And this is an example of.
  • 26:01The network of sleep being the cortical
  • 26:04network at least at a minimum,
  • 26:05being unstable despite sleep
  • 26:07being quite deep.
  • 26:10Network switching.
  • 26:11We see this on every polysomnogram.
  • 26:13If you choose to look at it,
  • 26:14well, almost every.
  • 26:15So on the top left you have
  • 26:17ongoing respiratory events,
  • 26:19fragmented sleep.
  • 26:20In the middle you see the abrupt switch.
  • 26:24Ohh there's no change in body position.
  • 26:26This is not a therapy study.
  • 26:28We did nothing spontaneous
  • 26:29switched to stable state.
  • 26:31This is how non REM sleep works.
  • 26:33Not N 1, N 2, N 3, N 4, N 5, N 6.
  • 26:35This is how non REM sleep works.
  • 26:37You have two fundamental network behaviors,
  • 26:40one where you have low frequency
  • 26:43oscillations dominating.
  • 26:44The other way you have essentially everything
  • 26:48synchronized around respiratory frequency,
  • 26:50individual breath frequency.
  • 26:51So this is an example of network switching.
  • 26:55Sleep onset is another great
  • 26:56example of network switching,
  • 26:58which we'll go into a little bit of detail.
  • 27:01So delving a little bit more into the,
  • 27:03you know,
  • 27:04horizontal network of non REM sleep,
  • 27:06the glue of non REM sleep.
  • 27:09Up here you have cortical network.
  • 27:13Local field potentials.
  • 27:14This is what we see on the brain.
  • 27:16You know a slow oscillations but
  • 27:18if you measure multi unit activity
  • 27:20you will find firing silence,
  • 27:22firing silence.
  • 27:23So this is the on off state of non REM
  • 27:26sleep which is a fundamental building
  • 27:28block of non REM sleep on top of which
  • 27:32everything else essentially rides.
  • 27:33And there's some examples
  • 27:35where the cortical network,
  • 27:36the non R.E.M slow oscillation breaks down.
  • 27:39And the best example would be
  • 27:41perhaps Alzheimer's disease.
  • 27:43You know, as your cortex breaks down,
  • 27:44your slow oscillation will also start
  • 27:47disappearing and start fragmenting.
  • 27:51The small isolation builds up
  • 27:54in frequency and spatial extent
  • 27:56as sleep starts and deepens.
  • 27:58And essentially, as we fall asleep, we have.
  • 28:01The network, uh, is.
  • 28:04The Sleep network is quite unstable,
  • 28:06so we have microsleeps.
  • 28:07You have a little bursts of sleep,
  • 28:09K complexes, some spindles alpha in and out.
  • 28:12And as we go further and further
  • 28:14and further you have coalescence
  • 28:15of the slow oscillation resulting
  • 28:17in you know in three or stage
  • 28:20four or stable non REM sleep.
  • 28:21And if you count the number
  • 28:23of slow oscillations per unit
  • 28:25time it progressively increase.
  • 28:27So essentially the cortical network can
  • 28:30be vulnerable when the cycles per minute
  • 28:33of slow oscillation is at the lower end.
  • 28:35And it becomes very stable.
  • 28:37It's like a very good self sealing tire.
  • 28:40You book the the sleeping brain.
  • 28:44When the slow oscillation density is high,
  • 28:46the network is very stable,
  • 28:48while if you poke it when it is not
  • 28:51so stable you will have an awakening
  • 28:53or you will have a arousal with
  • 28:56respiratory transient and so on.
  • 28:59OK.
  • 28:59So the slow oscillation and
  • 29:01and slow waves you know,
  • 29:03for for a while it was thought
  • 29:04to be fairly passive things,
  • 29:06but it turns out they're not passive.
  • 29:08And using simultaneous EEG and fMRI.
  • 29:11Uh,
  • 29:12this is very elegant paper in PNAS in 2008
  • 29:15actually showing that many cortical areas,
  • 29:19including inferior frontal,
  • 29:21middle prefrontal precuneus,
  • 29:22the posterior singlet, all actually
  • 29:25activate during slow oscillation.
  • 29:27So slow oscillation.
  • 29:28What you see on the surface,
  • 29:30there's a slow wave, it's an inhibitory wave.
  • 29:32That is true.
  • 29:33That's the off state.
  • 29:34And then you have the on
  • 29:36state with activation.
  • 29:37So it turns out that.
  • 29:39The driver of this on,
  • 29:41off, on,
  • 29:41off is not some passive Zen
  • 29:43kind of state of the brain,
  • 29:45but very active engagement by
  • 29:47a fairly widespread network of
  • 29:49very specific areas in the brain.
  • 29:51And this is just the pictorial representation
  • 29:53of the areas which I laid out to you.
  • 29:56And of course,
  • 29:58this breaks down when you have insomnia.
  • 30:00Uh, you know, Parkinson's disease.
  • 30:02Uh, I think of other,
  • 30:03you know, bad brain states,
  • 30:05traumatic brain injury.
  • 30:07A word on time delay, stability.
  • 30:09So basically what it means is that you have,
  • 30:12you know, your EKG, heart rate,
  • 30:14respiration, eye movements.
  • 30:15If all of them are very tightly synchronized,
  • 30:19it means you have short time delay stability.
  • 30:23The DPO sleep the less
  • 30:25synchronized these are.
  • 30:26So the lighter your sleep,
  • 30:28the more synchronized in
  • 30:30time these signals are.
  • 30:32And the deeper you sleep,
  • 30:34the more apart.
  • 30:35So it becomes a way to
  • 30:36measure the integrated step,
  • 30:38the integrated.
  • 30:39A network behavior of sleep and if
  • 30:42you spend all your night being in
  • 30:45short range time delay stability,
  • 30:48it means that you have these
  • 30:49ongoing transients all night long,
  • 30:51which of course would be a bad thing.
  • 30:54OK. Why bother? Well, it turns out that.
  • 31:01The Sleep network is dysfunctional
  • 31:04in epilepsy, dementia, stroke,
  • 31:06itel fibrillation, heart failure,
  • 31:09neuromuscular disorders.
  • 31:12Parkinson's disease?
  • 31:13Disneya syndromes.
  • 31:14That should be reason enough, right?
  • 31:17But it turns out that a
  • 31:18lot of our sleep disorders,
  • 31:20things which bother us a lot,
  • 31:22are classic network disorders.
  • 31:23Insomnia is a network disorder.
  • 31:25I think hypersomnia is also
  • 31:27a network disorder. Now.
  • 31:29Eclipse, of course, is a network disorder.
  • 31:32Brain injury and sleep apnea
  • 31:34certainly affects the white matter.
  • 31:36So it should make the network weaker.
  • 31:39Of course we have enormous resilience.
  • 31:41We have lots of white matter
  • 31:42and lots of redundancy,
  • 31:43but still white matter injury is,
  • 31:46you know,
  • 31:47the most tightly linked brain
  • 31:49pathology to sleep apnea.
  • 31:54Just checking my time.
  • 31:56So what can network Physiology do for
  • 31:58sleep science and Sleep Medicine?
  • 32:00If you think network,
  • 32:01remember we only do network,
  • 32:02but we don't think network.
  • 32:05What is this glue of sleep which holds?
  • 32:09Disparate oscillations and synchrony.
  • 32:11You know, wakeful cognition has always
  • 32:14had this, you know, binding problem.
  • 32:18How does? Different parts of
  • 32:20the brain hold things together.
  • 32:22The gamma Oscillation is one of the binders,
  • 32:24but. It's always been, uh,
  • 32:26the issue of consciousness.
  • 32:27When we are conscious, how do we bind?
  • 32:31Brain information networks together.
  • 32:33So let's flip to sleep.
  • 32:36Clearly there is a glue of sleep which holds
  • 32:38all these crazy oscillations and synchrony.
  • 32:41So we have a binding problem in sleep
  • 32:43and how does this inform consciousness?
  • 32:46What is the minimum unit of
  • 32:48sleep to perform function?
  • 32:50There must be some universal
  • 32:51law of tolerance of sleep,
  • 32:53fragmentation and arousals.
  • 32:54A few decades ago there was a paper.
  • 32:58I think it's Mark Bonnet.
  • 33:01Ohh, where they woke up healthy
  • 33:03volunteers every so often,
  • 33:05starting at once an hour,
  • 33:06every half an hour, etcetera,
  • 33:08to see the effect the next day on,
  • 33:09I believe there were MSLT,
  • 33:11so at least there was, you know,
  • 33:14some measurement of alertness.
  • 33:16So it turned out that once
  • 33:17you came to 10 minutes,
  • 33:18all hell broke loose at about 30 minutes.
  • 33:21You had an irritated person,
  • 33:23but their data and performance
  • 33:24the next day was reasonable.
  • 33:26So that's an example of trying to figure
  • 33:29out what is the minimum unit of sleep.
  • 33:31Has to be left alone to do his thing.
  • 33:34And of course, how many such
  • 33:35units do we need to string along?
  • 33:38To figure out what is adequate
  • 33:40sleep versus not,
  • 33:41why are certain individuals
  • 33:43incredibly fragmented sleep?
  • 33:45Who seem asymptomatic and vice versa.
  • 33:48Of sleep apnea is an example.
  • 33:50All of you know that substantial
  • 33:52minority of sleep apnea patients,
  • 33:54especially in epidemiological studies.
  • 33:57Really. They said they feel fine.
  • 34:00Don't bother me.
  • 34:00If they brought to the sleep link,
  • 34:02they're sulking there while they're partners,
  • 34:04you know?
  • 34:06Snitching on them about
  • 34:07snoring and gasping and so on.
  • 34:09Do a sleep study. They look terrible.
  • 34:11They say they feel fine.
  • 34:13You convince them to somehow try CPAP.
  • 34:15They're using it.
  • 34:16They don't feel any different.
  • 34:18How do you explain that?
  • 34:21Can the disruption grade of
  • 34:24pathology be quantified better
  • 34:26than just counting arousals or
  • 34:28counting brief transience?
  • 34:30And is a kind of network map of
  • 34:32sleep useful in clinical practice?
  • 34:36Some examples of of course.
  • 34:39You know breakdown syndromes?
  • 34:41Heart failure? It will.
  • 34:43Fibrillation, severe TBI treatment,
  • 34:46resistant depression,
  • 34:47mania, neurodegeneration.
  • 34:48The Sleep network breaks down.
  • 34:51There's no shortage of
  • 34:52evidence that it breaks down.
  • 34:54And the binding mechanisms
  • 34:56like the slow oscillation,
  • 34:57the cyclic alternating pattern,
  • 34:59the PGO waves,
  • 35:00these are all vulnerable as
  • 35:02the brain starts breaking down.
  • 35:03The worst degrees of sleep
  • 35:05fragmentation we will see in the
  • 35:06sleep clinic is heart failure.
  • 35:08Parkinson's, it'll fibrillation.
  • 35:11Probably the worst,
  • 35:12and sometimes patients with
  • 35:14irregular sleep wake cycle
  • 35:15disorder or very severe kind of
  • 35:18complex psychiatric comorbidities,
  • 35:19PTSD plus psychosis and such.
  • 35:22You can have incredibly fragmented
  • 35:23sleep where they look like rats,
  • 35:24and I'll show you a sample anyway.
  • 35:30Network breakdown so.
  • 35:32Look at the cortical level.
  • 35:35It's normally fairly resilient and redundant.
  • 35:38As an example, there is virtually no
  • 35:41description of loss of sleep with a whole
  • 35:44range of strokes of any kind of any severity.
  • 35:48And distribution.
  • 35:50Traumatic brain injury, of course,
  • 35:52badly affects the sleep network.
  • 35:54But a stroke is another good example
  • 35:56where the sleep network starts getting
  • 35:58unstable but does not disappear.
  • 36:00As almost Parkinson's epilepsy,
  • 36:02not your typical you know, average epilepsy.
  • 36:05But once you go to epilepsy needing
  • 36:08multidrug therapy treatment resistant
  • 36:10by whatever definition, thinking about,
  • 36:13you know, epilepsy surgery and such,
  • 36:16there's ample data there that sleep
  • 36:18is poor even by conventional metrics.
  • 36:20You know, in one and two, etcetera.
  • 36:22Sleep is more fragmented,
  • 36:24never mind going deeper into it.
  • 36:26The Thermo Cortical network
  • 36:28of course is disabled.
  • 36:29Terribly by fatal familial
  • 36:31insomnia and other prion disorders.
  • 36:34That immediate till I make stroke
  • 36:36has hypersomnia as a network output.
  • 36:39And of course, the tumors in the
  • 36:42area can disrupt sleep state.
  • 36:43The sleep Wake Transition Network
  • 36:45is an amazing network, isn't.
  • 36:46Just think of it.
  • 36:48We are moving from this.
  • 36:50You know,
  • 36:51hopefully most of us on this call are,
  • 36:53you know,
  • 36:54pretty good sleepers and we just
  • 36:56switch on the system and switch off
  • 36:58the system and these systems are
  • 37:01completely different and we make
  • 37:03this transition almost effortlessly.
  • 37:07But those who don't make it effortlessly,
  • 37:09of course, have insomnia with
  • 37:13various driver mechanisms.
  • 37:15Amygdala Bay syndrome.
  • 37:16So anxiety, fear, PTSD makes the
  • 37:18sleepy transition network unstable.
  • 37:20Of course, pain and stress
  • 37:21will do the same thing too.
  • 37:26More on bad networks,
  • 37:27the REM sleep network breakdown
  • 37:29and the disorders you know well.
  • 37:31Non REM sleep network and sleepwalking as
  • 37:34well as depression, the arousal network.
  • 37:37It's unstable inclined Levine syndrome
  • 37:39and bipolar disease on a long on a long
  • 37:43on an intermediate to long time scale.
  • 37:45A hypoactive arousal network,
  • 37:46of course, anesthesia, comma, etcetera.
  • 37:49And hyperactive arousal network is seen
  • 37:52in PTSD stress, abnormal respiration.
  • 37:57So what are the consequences of
  • 37:59the time of night distribution
  • 38:01of the slow oscillation glue?
  • 38:03Our network is more vulnerable later in the
  • 38:05night than earlier in the night, clearly.
  • 38:08Uh, and they're usability.
  • 38:09You know, changes across the night.
  • 38:12Successful insomnia treatment almost
  • 38:15certainly improves effective.
  • 38:17Glumness of the slow oscillation,
  • 38:19although as far as I know this has not been
  • 38:22explicitly studied and analyzed and computed.
  • 38:26I've got sleep goes in cycles,
  • 38:28so you have critical points of weakness
  • 38:30occurring regularly across the night.
  • 38:31We always have light periods,
  • 38:33we always have unstable periods.
  • 38:35We always have sleep cycles.
  • 38:37So if you're a light sleeper,
  • 38:40genetically predisposed
  • 38:41to have not great glue.
  • 38:43These critical points of weakness
  • 38:45is where you have breakdown of the
  • 38:48slow oscillation and this blueness.
  • 38:50And of course,
  • 38:51then you respond to stressors and
  • 38:53environmental noise and so on.
  • 38:54And of course the slow oscillation
  • 38:56breaks down with a whole range of
  • 38:59abnormal cortical health conditions.
  • 39:01Almost certainly genetic factors are
  • 39:03associated with sleep resilience and
  • 39:05likely impact the slow oscillation.
  • 39:07Uh, we don't.
  • 39:08We don't have great individual genes.
  • 39:10Um. At the individual level.
  • 39:15But certainly at the jeevas level you
  • 39:17have neiss an example which covaries
  • 39:19with insomnia as well as the restless legs.
  • 39:22That's probably not just by chance.
  • 39:25And of course, in insomnia,
  • 39:26pharmacotherapy is,
  • 39:27from one view, illogical.
  • 39:29You need the greatest help in
  • 39:32the second-half of the night,
  • 39:34when the slow oscillation is the weakest.
  • 39:36But guess what?
  • 39:37We get most of our help in the
  • 39:38first half of the night.
  • 39:40Of course,
  • 39:40if a sleep onset problem is just
  • 39:41a dominant problem,
  • 39:42it's probably circadian.
  • 39:46A quick word on respiratory
  • 39:47network dysfunction.
  • 39:48You know, all of this,
  • 39:49I don't have to, you know,
  • 39:50really go through any of these.
  • 39:54OK. Sleep onset. Let's spend a few
  • 39:56minutes talking about sleep onset,
  • 39:58because it's a big deal for insomnia.
  • 40:01For sleep apnea. For vigilance.
  • 40:05This is probably the best single paper
  • 40:08I come across looking at sleep onset.
  • 40:10I don't know if Mike is on this call,
  • 40:12but if he is shut out, it's a great paper.
  • 40:16Really worthwhile reading.
  • 40:18So essentially.
  • 40:21This is a very geeky kind of paper.
  • 40:24So you have this ball which you squeeze.
  • 40:26You're measuring respiration.
  • 40:29You're measuring the.
  • 40:32The long flexor?
  • 40:34The flexor historeum profundus.
  • 40:37You are. Tracking.
  • 40:42Responses. This is a breathing.
  • 40:46There's a, there's a metronome.
  • 40:48Essentially you're breathing
  • 40:49in relation to that.
  • 40:51And you have the squeeze amplitudes.
  • 40:55Dropping overtime as we
  • 40:56transition into sleep.
  • 40:57OK, so this is just. The base.
  • 41:00And how they compute the squeeze amplitude,
  • 41:04the alpha power, the Theta and delta power.
  • 41:07Behavioral responses gone.
  • 41:09Weak probability curves.
  • 41:12And of course the ospital spectrogram.
  • 41:13You see the alpha dying out and
  • 41:16Theta and delta building up.
  • 41:22Any further, you have response
  • 41:23probability in the squeeze amplitude.
  • 41:25Just another depiction of essentially
  • 41:27the same thing. But just think,
  • 41:29every time you're falling asleep,
  • 41:30there's this whole book
  • 41:32on sleep onset by Ogilvy.
  • 41:34It's yo thick and really great reading.
  • 41:39It's. At the end of my fellowship,
  • 41:41so it's over 20 years old,
  • 41:43but there's a lot of good data there listing
  • 41:45all the different kinds of physiological
  • 41:47changes which occur at sleep onset,
  • 41:49evoke responses, and so on.
  • 41:52So just think of the importance of
  • 41:54sleep onset and the incredible network
  • 41:56change which occurs during sleep onset,
  • 41:59respiration, EG behaviors.
  • 42:02And even in the brain.
  • 42:06Multi level changes which occur and
  • 42:08we have to navigate this and we do
  • 42:10this effortlessly most of the time,
  • 42:11unless of course you have insomnia
  • 42:13and heart failure and such.
  • 42:14Then of course the sleep onset has to.
  • 42:18You have to redo your sleep onset
  • 42:21multiple multiple times across the night.
  • 42:24Obviously that will be hostile,
  • 42:26and blood pressure of course
  • 42:28fluctuates up and down and rises if
  • 42:31you keep having these transients.
  • 42:33Now what's the clinical equivalent
  • 42:35of that what I call?
  • 42:37Of state amplified wake sleep,
  • 42:39transitional instability.
  • 42:40If you have a better acronym, send it to me.
  • 42:43So this is a 10 minute.
  • 42:46Yes, a 10 minute snapshot.
  • 42:49And technically scored mostly as weak.
  • 42:52I hope I don't have to convince
  • 42:54you that this is serious pathology,
  • 42:56where you have central apneas,
  • 42:57you have some periodical movements and such.
  • 43:00This is another example of amplified
  • 43:02wake sleep transitional instability.
  • 43:04The conventional scoring may
  • 43:06exclude a lot of this.
  • 43:07Don't do that,
  • 43:08because this is clearly very
  • 43:10pathological and these patients will
  • 43:12always complain of onset insomnia,
  • 43:13at least a lot of insomnia.
  • 43:15And of course it's more common with heart
  • 43:18failure and it'll fibrillation and such.
  • 43:21So. Some examples of interesting
  • 43:25coupling and networking.
  • 43:27Of various physiologies which you see.
  • 43:30So periodically movements
  • 43:32these are alternating.
  • 43:34And.
  • 43:37One lot is roughly synchronizing
  • 43:39with the respiratory recovery,
  • 43:41it seems, but apparently it's
  • 43:43just following his own rhythm.
  • 43:45Now here it is in the center, respiration.
  • 43:46Here it's.
  • 43:47You know in the middle of it,
  • 43:50while these less frequently
  • 43:53occurring periodically movements are
  • 43:55right in the middle of the event.
  • 43:58So. There is variable coupling.
  • 44:01And and sleep is basically slow wave sleep,
  • 44:05so each one is pretty much doing his thing.
  • 44:09Without really caring too much
  • 44:10what the others do.
  • 44:11Now there are some fundamental
  • 44:13frequencies in sleep which occur and
  • 44:15they are expecting that, but overall.
  • 44:17The various networks now, uh,
  • 44:19you know, somewhat uncoupled.
  • 44:23Thinking more about the respiratory
  • 44:24motor complex, so here you have.
  • 44:28Well, these are not typical
  • 44:29periodic limb movements.
  • 44:29You will agree. These are.
  • 44:34We are having these clusters of limb
  • 44:38movements occurring periodically.
  • 44:40With the right periodic sequence,
  • 44:42except that it's clusters and when you
  • 44:45go and take a close look you see that.
  • 44:47This oscillatory there's reverberatory
  • 44:50interaction between respiratory
  • 44:52output and the motor systems output.
  • 44:56Is alternating too.
  • 44:59Of course, you have huge arousal here.
  • 45:00Isn't that fascinating?
  • 45:05What drives and what follows
  • 45:06is a never ending question.
  • 45:07And when someone has periodical
  • 45:10movements and has apnea here,
  • 45:12it looks like well,
  • 45:13maybe the respiration is driving.
  • 45:17The periodic elements.
  • 45:19The same person slightly later,
  • 45:21where the limb movements are
  • 45:24occurring with the recovery
  • 45:25breath in the middle of an apnea.
  • 45:28It's doing his own thing.
  • 45:31This is an example of dumb
  • 45:34periodic limb movements.
  • 45:35And now you have not done
  • 45:37periodic limb movements.
  • 45:38This is blood pressure.
  • 45:39You know here is blood pressure.
  • 45:40So you have repeated surges of
  • 45:43blood pressure occurring with
  • 45:44every single leg movement.
  • 45:46This is not dumb.
  • 45:47Periodically movements
  • 45:48and a great example of a
  • 45:50pathological sleep network.
  • 45:57You can. Now there is some scoring
  • 46:00guidelines say nothing about the chin
  • 46:03EMG during non REM sleep basically.
  • 46:06Yeah, it may be low, you know,
  • 46:08but there's an enormous amount
  • 46:10of information in the chin EMG.
  • 46:12You can see how this coupling between
  • 46:15cortical transients and chin transients,
  • 46:17cortical chin, cortical chin cortical thin,
  • 46:20and you can extract the quality
  • 46:22of sleep by EMG analysis.
  • 46:25I'm sure not done it yet.
  • 46:27Hopefully.
  • 46:27Hopefully you can another example
  • 46:29of cortical motor network connection
  • 46:32while breathing is reasonably good.
  • 46:34Breathing is reasonably good,
  • 46:35but all hell is breaking loose
  • 46:37when it comes to sleep quality.
  • 46:39So now you have a dissociation of.
  • 46:41So the cortical motor system is
  • 46:42doing its thing in a different way,
  • 46:44while respiration says I don't really care,
  • 46:47I'm going to do my own little thing.
  • 46:50This person is on a postulator pressure.
  • 46:55On the left, you have network
  • 46:57success where you have good
  • 47:00behavior while there's some R.E.M,
  • 47:01dominant apnea, etcetera.
  • 47:03On the right, you have network failure.
  • 47:06This looks like a poorly sleeping rat.
  • 47:09Not only a good rat, but a poor rat.
  • 47:12And below. Essentially the same
  • 47:15person getting a titration.
  • 47:17Another night and sleep remains very,
  • 47:20very poor. It's a network failure.
  • 47:26Let's see. OK, just do three more minutes.
  • 47:29So. Once you start thinking network,
  • 47:33you start seeing a lot of really
  • 47:35interesting kind of features.
  • 47:37So this is a hypnogram
  • 47:39from the home sleep study.
  • 47:41Showing craziness of the heart rate, right?
  • 47:45Going down and then OK,
  • 47:47then down and OK.
  • 47:48And there is some respiratory stuff,
  • 47:50there's some you know oxygenation stuff,
  • 47:53some snoring now take a close up look.
  • 47:57So you have stable breathing.
  • 48:01Sorry, stable breathing,
  • 48:02a little bit of snore and the
  • 48:06plate signal behaves well.
  • 48:08You have on the other end on the right.
  • 48:11You have abnormal respiration.
  • 48:14And you have bradycardia return to normal,
  • 48:18bradycardia return to normal.
  • 48:19And I just put a line around
  • 48:22the respiratory recovery starts.
  • 48:24It's not synchronized.
  • 48:26However, when state is stable.
  • 48:30This cardiac arrhythmia is not occurring.
  • 48:33So it is not entrained,
  • 48:34but perhaps stable,
  • 48:36unstable sleep networks enable.
  • 48:38Certain pathologies to come through.
  • 48:41So this is a transition where breathing
  • 48:43is starting to look a little unstable.
  • 48:45A little bit of snoring coming
  • 48:47in and you can start seeing this
  • 48:50intermittent bradyarrhythmia coming.
  • 48:54You do better than hypersomnia.
  • 48:56Can be thought of as a network of failure.
  • 48:59We got to get up in the morning.
  • 49:01We need a wake network to take
  • 49:03over as an example of a patient
  • 49:05who keeps sleeping, sleeping,
  • 49:06sleeping, not very fragmented.
  • 49:08This one is much more sleep,
  • 49:09sleeping, sleeping, sleeping.
  • 49:11But it's very fragmented and
  • 49:13sleeps I think 22 hours, 22 hours.
  • 49:19So it's a speculative hypothesis, but.
  • 49:23Perhaps hypersomnia can be
  • 49:24considered a failure of a switch
  • 49:27from the sleep to awake network.
  • 49:33Uh. I should probably stop here.
  • 49:43So. Stabilizing networks
  • 49:46to target sleep disorders.
  • 49:48We do it all the time.
  • 49:49Here are examples.
  • 49:50We don't think network,
  • 49:51but we're doing it all the time.
  • 49:53Successful sleep treatment
  • 49:54of any kind requires.
  • 49:57Stabilizing sleep networks.
  • 50:01And I think I will stop with that if there's
  • 50:04you know if there's some time I can go back
  • 50:06and show you cardio pulmonary coupling,
  • 50:08how it can be used to track network
  • 50:10stability in an ambulatory way,
  • 50:12but it's not necessary to make my point.
  • 50:14So in summary, sleep is a unique
  • 50:16network state, it's multi Physiology,
  • 50:18it has multiple dimensions including time,
  • 50:21it's dynamic,
  • 50:22it's morphing the phase transitions,
  • 50:25predictable changes in disease,
  • 50:26predictable effects of treatment
  • 50:28which works or doesn't work.
  • 50:30And network analysis is severely underused.
  • 50:32And sleep research and
  • 50:34nonexistent and sleep practice.
  • 50:36Network science is alive and well.
  • 50:39There are.
  • 50:40In so many different metrics which can
  • 50:42be generated and potentially useful
  • 50:44and track disease and quantify beyond.
  • 50:47You know we keep talking beyond the
  • 50:49HI beyond the HI yeah this is one of
  • 50:52those beyond the HI kinds of things
  • 50:54which almost certainly can be useful.
  • 50:56A call out for frontiers
  • 50:59and network Physiology.
  • 51:00It's a new subsection of frontiers.
  • 51:02The new journal within Frontiers
  • 51:05and Ivanov is the. Is the edit.
  • 51:09El France chief overall editor.
  • 51:12It has a section on sleep
  • 51:13and circadian systems,
  • 51:14networks and sleep and circadian systems.
  • 51:17And you know,
  • 51:18if you have a network key kinds
  • 51:20of things you like to publish,
  • 51:22we are open and happy.
  • 51:25I don't know if Flammen is on this call.
  • 51:28I hope he could join,
  • 51:29but we're very excited about the
  • 51:33potential of rapid translation.
  • 51:35Uh, not just uh, you know,
  • 51:39interesting theoretical physics,
  • 51:40see kind of papers,
  • 51:42but really trying to better
  • 51:44understand disease and tracking
  • 51:46and pathophysiology by looking
  • 51:48at multi system integration and
  • 51:50breakdown in the sleep state.
  • 51:52So with that I shall stop and happy
  • 51:55to take questions and similar things.
  • 51:58Thank you.
  • 52:00Great. Well, thank you very much
  • 52:02Robert for this tour, tour of the.
  • 52:05Sure. Of the sleep network.
  • 52:07So I will moderate the questions
  • 52:09that you guys have them,
  • 52:10please put them in the chat
  • 52:11and if you have a question,
  • 52:12just raise your hand and I will unmute you.
  • 52:15So that you can go ahead and and do that.
  • 52:17And so it looks like Mayor Krieger,
  • 52:19you have a question.
  • 52:19Go ahead Mayor
  • 52:21Robert, I didn't see oxygen on your slide
  • 52:24where you talked about potential therapies
  • 52:28for some of these network problems.
  • 52:31I found in Canada was it was easy
  • 52:33to order oxygen for heart failure.
  • 52:36And some of the patients did incredibly well.
  • 52:41That is true.
  • 52:42Oxygen definitely should be there.
  • 52:44In fact, one of my earliest oxygen
  • 52:47successes was a patient who had very
  • 52:49severe he had changed strokes and clinic.
  • 52:52He was that severe.
  • 52:53And on oxygen therapy,
  • 52:55he was back to hunting.
  • 52:56So you know that is true.
  • 52:58I just wish it was more predictable.
  • 53:01But that is true.
  • 53:02Patients can have really
  • 53:04great benefit from oxygen,
  • 53:05but it's just less predictable than ideal.
  • 53:08Oxygen of course changes the sleep
  • 53:10network in many ways because the
  • 53:11current it will suppress curby
  • 53:13body firing to some extent.
  • 53:14And the current body of course
  • 53:17influences the tractor solitarius,
  • 53:18which then of course influences just about
  • 53:21everything in the brain and brainstem.
  • 53:23So.
  • 53:24Yeah, oxygen, definitely.
  • 53:25I should add that.
  • 53:31All right, very good.
  • 53:34I have a question Robert and so.
  • 53:36Is very interesting you pointing out the
  • 53:39differences between stable and unstable
  • 53:41and to sleep and in many cases where
  • 53:44it just seems to occur spontaneously.
  • 53:46Do you have an idea or at least
  • 53:48if you have some hypothesis as
  • 53:50to why might that occur?
  • 53:51So we we do see that in PSG's
  • 53:54quite often where you have.
  • 53:56Stage two sleep, but one is stable,
  • 53:58one is not stable.
  • 53:58Do you have a sense of why that might occur?
  • 54:01Honestly, no. Some ideas as to why,
  • 54:05how, how it could occur? Umm. No.
  • 54:11The Telemac cortical system has a membrane
  • 54:15oscillations which can change over.
  • 54:18You know. Multiple seconds and minutes.
  • 54:23And because they are so central in the, you
  • 54:26know, between the cortex and the brain stem.
  • 54:29It's possible that changes in
  • 54:30telemac cortical conductance,
  • 54:31especially potassium conductance,
  • 54:33could be a switching kind of mechanism.
  • 54:37It's possible that in stable state
  • 54:40there is mild hypercapnia mild.
  • 54:43Even in normal stable state.
  • 54:45That it after a certain point
  • 54:49it triggers a hyper. Well.
  • 54:52Hard to call it hypercapnia,
  • 54:54but the CO2 mediated.
  • 54:57Arousal, which enables the switch to.
  • 55:00Another state.
  • 55:01But in reality, I'm just guessing.
  • 55:03I simply do not know.
  • 55:05I have for many years.
  • 55:07I tried to convince Cliff shaper,
  • 55:10Jerome Siegel,
  • 55:11and a few others to try to study this state.
  • 55:15Which of course occurs in rats and mice.
  • 55:18Called intermediate sleep I believe.
  • 55:21And that's what actually is our enemy.
  • 55:23I mean, our enemy is what the
  • 55:26neurobiologists kind of, sort of ignore.
  • 55:28Uh, we have, uh, less worry about.
  • 55:32Nice, good.
  • 55:33Delta enhanced sleep in the clinic.
  • 55:36Much of what we deal with is unstable,
  • 55:38so it is a mystery and I wish.
  • 55:41Neurobiologists would actually
  • 55:42try to figure it out because
  • 55:44that's what we struggle with.
  • 55:46That's our enemy.
  • 55:49But we see it all the time, right?
  • 55:50I mean, anyone who chooses to
  • 55:51look for it, you'll see it.
  • 55:53And stable breathing periods
  • 55:54have been described, you know,
  • 55:56so many times over the years
  • 55:58in sleep apnea patients.
  • 56:00It's almost certainly a it's not
  • 56:02just a genioglossus activity,
  • 56:04or it's not one thing.
  • 56:05It is the whole state switching.
  • 56:11Gotcha. OK. Anybody else have
  • 56:13any other questions I wanted to
  • 56:16to bring up during this talk?
  • 56:20Want to join in and drop
  • 56:21some words of wisdom?
  • 56:26You too.
  • 56:28Try to try to doctor Evanoff.
  • 56:30Try to meet yourself.
  • 56:37Yes, OK.
  • 56:39So take your other wonderful presentation
  • 56:41and so you paint the big picture and I
  • 56:44think with the big potential for the future,
  • 56:46but basic science and clinical practice.
  • 56:50In general, just West of perhaps the question
  • 56:54about stability and instability and whether
  • 56:57we have a true human studies in sleep,
  • 57:00it seems like, at least from a
  • 57:03certain indication of these dynamics,
  • 57:05whether of individual systems or from
  • 57:07point of view of network interactions,
  • 57:10that sleep has also certain
  • 57:14characteristics of criticality
  • 57:17and this system at criticality.
  • 57:20What this would mean is that
  • 57:23there is certain not local, but.
  • 57:27Global interactions that allow a
  • 57:31transition that are spontaneous.
  • 57:34If we have a system which is truly
  • 57:36in equilibrium then it takes a
  • 57:38lot of energy to drive the system
  • 57:40Congress sleep stage to another or
  • 57:42from one sub state within the sleep
  • 57:45stage to another and having you
  • 57:48know certain critical features from
  • 57:51actually physics point of view of
  • 57:54criticality perhaps has a place.
  • 57:56To better understand the dynamics
  • 57:59and regulation of sleep.
  • 58:01So this in relation to the last
  • 58:04question that was asked about
  • 58:06stable and unstable states.
  • 58:08This transitions across spontaneously
  • 58:10and there could be facilitated by
  • 58:14dynamics which are non equilibrium
  • 58:16non homeostatic dynamics that occur
  • 58:19at small timescales of seconds up
  • 58:21to minutes of course when you speak
  • 58:24about the large window of hours or.
  • 58:27Well, you know the sleep wake cycle
  • 58:29of the 24 hours day then for you have
  • 58:33certain homeostatic control there.
  • 58:34So it's seems that there is some
  • 58:37duality and I think these network
  • 58:39approaches also could perhaps will be
  • 58:42useful in this direction in the future.
  • 58:45But you painted a very really deep and
  • 58:50broad picture and a lot to be investigated,
  • 58:53so obviously this is a visual
  • 58:57one can see traces that.
  • 59:00Visually you can see certain
  • 59:03correspondence in patterns,
  • 59:04but the big challenge is,
  • 59:06as you also pointed out,
  • 59:07is to find reliable metrics.
  • 59:10And in basic science we do not quite
  • 59:15have reliable metrics for systems
  • 59:17which are diverse in nature and that
  • 59:20also work in parallel but work in
  • 59:23parallel across multiple scales.
  • 59:25So simple cross correlation simple
  • 59:28metrics don't quite work or not.
  • 59:31Do not work reliably.
  • 59:32And so in that sense I just would like to,
  • 59:35you know, caution the community.
  • 59:37Of course the horizon is there
  • 59:40and very exciting,
  • 59:41but one also has to be careful
  • 59:43of how to how do we technically
  • 59:46approach the question of extracting
  • 59:49information that is hidden in the
  • 59:51dynamics of the signals and in
  • 59:53their couple. Very good.
  • 59:55Well, thank you.
  • 59:56Thank you very much, Doctor Evanoff.
  • 59:57Thank you very much, Doctor Thomas.
  • 59:59It was a great talk, great conference,
  • 01:00:02lots of stimulation for thought and ideas
  • 01:00:05for future research and collaboration.
  • 01:00:07And thank you all for attending another
  • 01:00:10edition of the joint conference.
  • 01:00:13And we will see you guys back on
  • 01:00:14the 12th of October when Doctor
  • 01:00:16Ali Azar Brazil will join us from
  • 01:00:18pregnant Women's Hospital and talk
  • 01:00:19about his work in examining others,
  • 01:00:23by the way will be networking. Yeah.
  • 01:00:27Okey Doke, very good.
  • 01:00:28So we'll see you guys in October.
  • 01:00:30Have a great week.