Skip to Main Content

Pathology Grand Rounds: April 27, 2023 - Jeffrey A. Golden, MD

April 28, 2023
  • 00:00All right. Welcome
  • 00:02everyone. Thanks for coming today.
  • 00:06It is my honor to introduce my friend
  • 00:10and mentor and fellow neuropathologist,
  • 00:13Doctor Jeffrey Golden.
  • 00:16Some of you may know Jeff is
  • 00:18actually my PhD mentor and advisor
  • 00:20at University of Pennsylvania.
  • 00:22He's currently the director of
  • 00:24the Burns and Allen Research.
  • 00:26Institute at Cedars Sinai and also the Vice
  • 00:29Dean of Research and Graduate Education.
  • 00:32Jeff I graduated from University
  • 00:34of Pennsylvania School of Medicine
  • 00:36and after that went on to do
  • 00:38his neuropathology fellowship
  • 00:39and and postdoc training at MGH.
  • 00:43And he did his postdoc in Connie
  • 00:45Sepco's lab and really launched
  • 00:47his career into studying the
  • 00:50cellular and molecular basis of
  • 00:52neurodevelopmental disorders.
  • 00:53He did.
  • 00:55Really pioneering work studying non
  • 00:57radial cell migration and its critical
  • 00:59role in interneuron migration during
  • 01:02neocortical development and his
  • 01:05research has made he's made really
  • 01:08many contributions in understanding
  • 01:09the molecular and cellular basis of
  • 01:11several malformations of cortical
  • 01:14development as well as epilepsy and
  • 01:16and other intellectual disorders. So I.
  • 01:22Won't really take up any more time,
  • 01:24but it's it's just
  • 01:26such an honor to have Jeff here.
  • 01:28We've been trying to get him here
  • 01:29since before the pandemic and so
  • 01:32finally he's here. I'm so pleased.
  • 01:33Please join me in welcoming him.
  • 01:40Well, first, thank you,
  • 01:42Paula V for the invitation.
  • 01:44This is I think the third of several
  • 01:46aborted attempts to get here.
  • 01:48And so I'm really pleased to be here.
  • 01:50And actually since the pandemic,
  • 01:52this is I think the only,
  • 01:53this is only my second visit to a university,
  • 01:56so it's great to see people in
  • 01:58person and be able to start feeling
  • 02:00like we're getting back to normal.
  • 02:03So today what I want to
  • 02:05do is take you through
  • 02:10we've been doing over.
  • 02:29Technology have started to really
  • 02:32just out
  • 02:35there
  • 02:38we go. So I like to start
  • 02:41out there was playing
  • 02:45that's good okay there we
  • 02:49go case and and what you see here is a mom
  • 02:55playing with her son in a crib.
  • 02:58The sun has a cap on which is an
  • 03:00EEG recording is a video EEG and
  • 03:02and you see him almost kind of
  • 03:05flop over as if almost playing.
  • 03:07But in fact this is a what's called
  • 03:10a drop seizure or a epileptic
  • 03:13seizure that this child has.
  • 03:16And the type of seizure actually pretends
  • 03:20a very poor prognosis for this individual.
  • 03:23This child will and actually did go on.
  • 03:26To have a variety of different types of
  • 03:30seizures and became increasingly severe.
  • 03:33Also had cognitive
  • 03:37disabilities and had
  • 03:40autism spectrum disorder.
  • 03:42And so from this very simple thing we
  • 03:52neurobiology that must underlie.
  • 03:54These defects and and that's really
  • 03:58what I've been thinking about for
  • 04:00most of my scientific career.
  • 04:02So let me just tell you a couple
  • 04:04basic things about epilepsy and then
  • 04:06we'll get into some other things.
  • 04:07It's actually pretty common.
  • 04:09It's more common than stroke,
  • 04:12which we hear about a lot.
  • 04:14It is actually about as prevalent as asthma,
  • 04:18which is heard about a lot.
  • 04:20So epilepsy is a major healthcare issue.
  • 04:25And it's really hard to control.
  • 04:32Most Asian even with extensive
  • 04:39multi drug therapy do not come under
  • 04:42control and only a subset of them
  • 04:45are able to be managed with surgery.
  • 04:47So there are some that can
  • 04:48be managed with surgery,
  • 04:49but it's just a subset.
  • 04:51So really we need to develop a
  • 04:54far better understanding of the,
  • 04:56the underlying biology to
  • 04:59really begin to think about
  • 05:01biologically relevant therapeutics.
  • 05:02So we can talk about that
  • 05:04a little bit at the end.
  • 05:07So what are the causes of epilepsy
  • 05:09where they're quite varied,
  • 05:10They include things like brain injury,
  • 05:12so trauma to the brain and lead
  • 05:15to its stroke tumors and there's a
  • 05:18variety of different genetic defects.
  • 05:21But at least 50% of them,
  • 05:22we really don't know the underlying cause,
  • 05:24although it's believed that
  • 05:26many of those are genetic.
  • 05:29And that's really where this is
  • 05:31led me to kind of start thinking
  • 05:34about this and and this started
  • 05:37a number of years ago when I got
  • 05:39invited to give a talk at a.
  • 05:42Early infantile epileptic encephalopathy,
  • 05:45it's a type of syndrome that you
  • 05:47see and at the time I, I,
  • 05:49I started looking and I asked
  • 05:51the question well how many,
  • 05:52what do we know about the genes
  • 05:54that are involved at that time And
  • 05:56this list is much longer but this
  • 05:57is where I started and and at the
  • 05:59time there was just this relatively
  • 06:01small number of genes that are
  • 06:03involved there's there's now at
  • 06:04least 152 genes involved in or
  • 06:06associated with epilepsy and and I.
  • 06:09I just started kind of thinking
  • 06:11about this was kind of my own Geo,
  • 06:14my own you know,
  • 06:16kind of analysis and I was able to kind
  • 06:20of orc or divide them up into nuclear genes,
  • 06:24membrane associated genes.
  • 06:26These are mostly receptors,
  • 06:30transporters and channels,
  • 06:33cytoplasmic proteins.
  • 06:35And then interestingly,
  • 06:37a number of mitochondrial
  • 06:39disease genes came up.
  • 06:41And So what I'm going to do today is
  • 06:43take you through two of these because
  • 06:45we've started studying the nuclear
  • 06:47genes and I'm going to talk about a RX,
  • 06:49the first one in that line,
  • 06:51for quite a bit of time.
  • 06:53And then I'm going to switch over
  • 06:54and tell you a little bit about
  • 06:56mitochondrial ones and how those
  • 06:57actually came together in thinking
  • 06:59about these different disorders.
  • 07:02So just as a very brief primer to get
  • 07:05started, just to remind everyone about the
  • 07:08neocortex and for some of the audience,
  • 07:10I'm sure this is review,
  • 07:11but just to remind people,
  • 07:14I think you can see my pointer,
  • 07:15yeah, so layer one of the cortex
  • 07:17is known as the molecular layer.
  • 07:19This is in the mature
  • 07:20brain during development.
  • 07:21It's very important.
  • 07:22It's also known as the marginal zone
  • 07:24where there's a cell type called
  • 07:26the Chorizius neuron which makes.
  • 07:28Realin, which is important for the
  • 07:30organization of the sixth layer,
  • 07:32the laminar cortex layer two is known
  • 07:34as the external granular cell layer.
  • 07:37This is the mostly an input layer,
  • 07:40a local input layer to the neocortex layer
  • 07:44three is the external pyramidal cell layer.
  • 07:47Now I want to take this moment to say
  • 07:51that all layers have both excited to
  • 07:54have pyramidal and granular neurons.
  • 07:56It's just the relative distribution that
  • 07:58give rise to it being an an external,
  • 08:01I'm sorry, a granular layer versus
  • 08:03a paramidal cell layer.
  • 08:05So we have an external paramidal cell layer,
  • 08:08layer 3.
  • 08:09Then you have a internal granular
  • 08:12cell layer which is layer 4.
  • 08:14These layers actually define
  • 08:16the different cortical regions.
  • 08:18So for example,
  • 08:18if you look in the visual cortex,
  • 08:20this is from motor cortex,
  • 08:21if you look at the visual cortex.
  • 08:23Layer 4 is dramatically expanded and
  • 08:25it's the major input from the thalamus,
  • 08:28whereas in the motor cortex there's
  • 08:31almost no layer four at all.
  • 08:33In contrast, layer 5,
  • 08:35which is your primary
  • 08:39output pyramidal cell layer,
  • 08:41that goes, for example,
  • 08:42the large bed cells that go
  • 08:44down in the spinal cord,
  • 08:45those are all in layer 5.
  • 08:47Those project to other parts of the brain,
  • 08:49including to the other side
  • 08:50of the brain and then layer 6.
  • 08:53Which is against the white matter.
  • 08:55This is the what's known as the multiform
  • 08:58or fusiform layer and it consists
  • 09:00of a variety of different neuronal
  • 09:02types and has both input and output.
  • 09:04And then below that is the white matter.
  • 09:06So that's the six layers of the cortex.
  • 09:08And these layers are composed,
  • 09:11as I said of of these granular neurons,
  • 09:14here labeled non spining neurons and the
  • 09:17spining neurons or the pyramidal cells now.
  • 09:21These two cell types compose the
  • 09:24major 2 cell types in the cortex,
  • 09:26the excitatory neurons,
  • 09:27those are the spiny ones,
  • 09:29they use glutamate as a neurotransmitter,
  • 09:31and the non spiny or the inhibitory
  • 09:35interneurons which all use GABA.
  • 09:37But as you can see here,
  • 09:38there's actually many different forms
  • 09:39of each of these different neurons,
  • 09:41and there's.
  • 09:42Probably on the order of 30 to 40
  • 09:44or more of the excitatory neurons
  • 09:46and in the order of 60 of different
  • 09:49types of inhibitory neurons or
  • 09:51the these non spiny interneurons.
  • 09:55Normal cortical development requires
  • 09:59an integration of both of these
  • 10:03types of neurons.
  • 10:06If you don't have the excitatory
  • 10:08neurons and the inhibitory neurons you
  • 10:10get disruptions in normal cortical.
  • 10:12Functions,
  • 10:12circuits that result in functional
  • 10:15defects and those give rise to a
  • 10:18variety of different developmental disorders.
  • 10:21And what you can see here is the
  • 10:23different types of excitatory and
  • 10:25inhibitory neurons connect in different ways.
  • 10:27So there are some inhibitory neurons
  • 10:29like this green one that predominantly
  • 10:31function by connecting on the Axon and
  • 10:34the proximal Axon and the cell body,
  • 10:37whereas others are going to
  • 10:39connect more on dendrites.
  • 10:41And some just on the cell body itself,
  • 10:43like this purple one.
  • 10:45And these are all required to appropriately
  • 10:48modulate circuits in the brain.
  • 10:52So you need all these different types
  • 10:54to get the appropriate development
  • 10:56and function of the cerebral cortex.
  • 11:02Well, what's interesting also about this
  • 11:04is that these two different neuronal types
  • 11:07come from different progenitor zones.
  • 11:10Now in the for the cortical excitatory
  • 11:14neurons, they are essentially
  • 11:16all driven from or derived from
  • 11:18the cortical ventricular zone,
  • 11:21that dorsal portion of progenitor cells
  • 11:23that are along the ventricle and these
  • 11:27cells divide and they migrate out.
  • 11:29This a lot of this work fundamentally
  • 11:31done by Doctor Rakesh who's sitting
  • 11:33here in the front row.
  • 11:35Has shown that these cells migrate out the
  • 11:37first form of what's called a preplate A,
  • 11:40the call ritzius neuron,
  • 11:41and then a subplate neuron,
  • 11:42which is really important for the
  • 11:45ultimate connection between the thalamic
  • 11:48nuclei and the cortical region.
  • 11:51So defining, for example,
  • 11:52the visual cortex in the lateral
  • 11:54geniculate body.
  • 11:55For those connections to be made,
  • 11:57it requires those subplate neurons.
  • 11:59They that the call ritzius neuron and
  • 12:02the subplate neuron are important.
  • 12:04We're also separating and the
  • 12:06definitive cortex,
  • 12:08that layer 2 through 6.
  • 12:10Fill in between those and they fill
  • 12:12in in an inverse pattern from the
  • 12:15deep layer to the superficial layer.
  • 12:18In mice,
  • 12:19all of the excitatory neurons
  • 12:21come from this region,
  • 12:23and it looks like in humans too,
  • 12:25inhibitory neurons,
  • 12:26which I'll show you on the next slide.
  • 12:28There may be some that are derived
  • 12:30from the ventricular zone in
  • 12:32the in the human dorsal cortex,
  • 12:34but it does not appear to be in the mouse.
  • 12:37In the human and in the mouse,
  • 12:40the vast majority are from another
  • 12:42progenitor region and I'll show you that.
  • 12:44Next I do want to just briefly mention,
  • 12:46because I'm going to talk about it,
  • 12:47there's an intermediate progenitor zone.
  • 12:49This is called the sub ventricular
  • 12:51zone and this is where cells.
  • 12:54Actually go from the ventricular
  • 12:56zone being a stem cell to this
  • 12:59this subventricular zone where they
  • 13:01form an intermediate progenitor.
  • 13:03Go through one or several divisions
  • 13:06before ultimately and exiting the cell
  • 13:08cycle and going up into the cortex.
  • 13:13Now this, this is the what I was showing you.
  • 13:16Here's the this is a hemi section.
  • 13:19Imagine this being a hemi
  • 13:21section through the brain.
  • 13:22And it could be any mammalian species.
  • 13:24And it's one side.
  • 13:26So this is the midline,
  • 13:28this is dorsal or the top, this is ventral,
  • 13:31the bottom and this is lateral.
  • 13:33The cortical ventricular zone
  • 13:34that I've been talking about,
  • 13:35where the excitatory
  • 13:36neurons come from is here.
  • 13:38And then there's this structure
  • 13:40known as the ganglionic eminence
  • 13:42in the ventral part of the brain.
  • 13:44This is where these inhibitory
  • 13:45neurons are derived from.
  • 13:46And what's interesting is that if you look.
  • 13:50Along the dorsal,
  • 13:51ventral axis from the neural at the beginning
  • 13:54of the nervous system in this neural tube,
  • 13:56from dorsal to ventral,
  • 13:58you get different neural types defined
  • 14:01by different genetic pathways.
  • 14:03And you see that also in the forebrain.
  • 14:06So this is in the spinal cord,
  • 14:07but in the forebrain.
  • 14:08And there's specific genes that are
  • 14:10expressed in these different regions
  • 14:11that give rise to these different
  • 14:13progenitors and different cell types.
  • 14:15And again,
  • 14:15I'm going to come back to
  • 14:16talk about that as well.
  • 14:19So what does this look like in the human?
  • 14:21So on your left side,
  • 14:23that's a human about 7 1/2 weeks gestation.
  • 14:27And you can see the ganglionic eminence where
  • 14:30these inhibitory neurons are predominately
  • 14:32coming from the ventricular zone.
  • 14:34And this here is the preplate and
  • 14:36you can see the preplate forming,
  • 14:38it's a little larger,
  • 14:39It actually starts in the ventrilateral
  • 14:42location and it moves. Moves up,
  • 14:45so it doesn't all develop at the same time.
  • 14:48It also goes from anterior to posterior,
  • 14:50so you can see that in
  • 14:52the very dorsal regions,
  • 14:53there's not even a preplate yet.
  • 14:54At 7 1/2 weeks by 20 weeks,
  • 15:00the station which is here,
  • 15:01you can see the hippocampus.
  • 15:03So we're looking at the temporal lobe,
  • 15:04and essentially all the neurons,
  • 15:07with a few exceptions,
  • 15:09are already in the cortex.
  • 15:11So they've migrated out,
  • 15:12but they haven't yet differentiated
  • 15:14and defined themselves in
  • 15:15different laminar organization,
  • 15:16but they are molecularly
  • 15:17defined by that time.
  • 15:18All right.
  • 15:19So that's a little bit of a kind
  • 15:21of just background to get you
  • 15:24to thinking about the studies
  • 15:25I'm going to tell you about.
  • 15:27I'm going to start out telling you
  • 15:29about that transcription factor
  • 15:30that is associated with epilepsy.
  • 15:32It's actually associated with
  • 15:33structural defects of the brain as well.
  • 15:36It's called ARX.
  • 15:37Aristoless related homeobox gene,
  • 15:39so it's related to the Drosophila
  • 15:42ristolis gene and this gene is on
  • 15:45the X chromosome and in males what
  • 15:48you see is a spectrum of neurologic
  • 15:52and cognitive deficits and and
  • 15:54and I really was attracted to this
  • 15:57gene because of this spectrum.
  • 15:59So some mutations resulted in
  • 16:01hydrocephalus and and abnormal genitalia.
  • 16:05That's a pretty rare one.
  • 16:06What's most common was the second
  • 16:07one on there, something called X lag,
  • 16:10X link lizencephaly with ambiguous genitalia.
  • 16:13But you could also have a genesis,
  • 16:14the corpus callosum.
  • 16:16You could have infantile spasms.
  • 16:18That was what I showed you in
  • 16:20that child earlier.
  • 16:20That does have an ARX mutation
  • 16:23and a variety of other things,
  • 16:24down to intellectual disabilities
  • 16:26with seizures.
  • 16:30What I want to point out is that
  • 16:32at the top of this list are
  • 16:33structural defects of the brain,
  • 16:35lizencephaly and a genus of the corpus
  • 16:38callosum, and they have epilepsy
  • 16:41and intellectual disabilities.
  • 16:42At the bottom of the list are brains
  • 16:45that are structurally normal but
  • 16:48have the exact same intellectual
  • 16:50disabilities and epilepsy,
  • 16:51presenting at almost the exact same time.
  • 16:54And this was really curious to me.
  • 16:55How can you have these two different?
  • 16:58Kind of phenotypes,
  • 16:59one with a structurally abnormal
  • 17:00brain and one with a normal brain,
  • 17:02what's structurally normal based on MRI
  • 17:05and have the same neurologic presentation.
  • 17:10And I want to try and address that
  • 17:12and I hope by the end of today I
  • 17:14will be able to have addressed that.
  • 17:16I will say that females were
  • 17:18thought to have a genesis of the
  • 17:20corpus callosum in some cases,
  • 17:22otherwise they were normal.
  • 17:23We have shown in publications
  • 17:25that that's actually not true.
  • 17:27I'm not going to talk about females today,
  • 17:28but I'm happy to talk if you want to at the
  • 17:31end about the genetics and how that happens.
  • 17:34So let's talk about a RX.
  • 17:35It's a, it's got 5 exons.
  • 17:38The first, sorry,
  • 17:39the 2nd exon at the very end.
  • 17:42The second exon's the
  • 17:43largest at the very end.
  • 17:44It has the beginning of its paired like
  • 17:47transcription homeo domain component.
  • 17:49So it's got a paired like homeo domain.
  • 17:52It's at the end of the second,
  • 17:54going through the 3rd and into
  • 17:56the beginning of the 4th exons,
  • 17:58but I also and and and.
  • 18:00So mutations at the top of that
  • 18:02which are frame shift or deletions,
  • 18:04give you that very severe
  • 18:05structural defect of the brain,
  • 18:07including the lizencephaly and
  • 18:11the agents of the corpus callosum.
  • 18:14But at the bottom, I'm not sure.
  • 18:17Why the lettering didn't come out right,
  • 18:19but it's at the bottom are some
  • 18:22interesting mutations that are
  • 18:25point mutations in very highly
  • 18:27conserved amino acids and also
  • 18:30these alanine tract expansion.
  • 18:32So ARX has four alanine tracts in it.
  • 18:38We really don't know much
  • 18:39about these alanine tracts,
  • 18:40but we now know that there's about 450
  • 18:43annotated genes in the human genome
  • 18:45that have an alanine tract in them.
  • 18:4875% of those are transcription factors.
  • 18:49So we think that it has something to
  • 18:51do with transcriptional regulation.
  • 18:53And there's now 9 or 10 genes that
  • 18:59have an alanine tract expansion
  • 19:01like you see in glutamine tract
  • 19:05expansions like Huntington's disease,
  • 19:06Kennedy syndrome, some of the HS P's,
  • 19:12those expansions in alanine tracts
  • 19:14though are relatively small.
  • 19:16On the order of three to
  • 19:1810 additional alanines.
  • 19:19And unlike the Huntington's disease and
  • 19:22some of the others that show anticipation
  • 19:25with very long glutamine tract expansions,
  • 19:27these are very short and they're all
  • 19:31developmental, they're not neurodegenerative,
  • 19:34these are neurodevelopmental disorders.
  • 19:37So we were quite curious about them
  • 19:40because these give rise to the the same.
  • 19:43Intellectual disabilities and epilepsy,
  • 19:45but structurally normal brains and
  • 19:48I'll be telling you about that today.
  • 19:51So where is a RX expressed?
  • 19:54Well, work from John Rubenstein's lab out
  • 19:57in San Francisco showed that it's expressed
  • 19:59early on in the ganglionic eminence.
  • 20:01Here you can see by insight to
  • 20:04hybridization and throughout the cortex
  • 20:06and here we did an immunohistic chemistry
  • 20:08with an antibody that we developed.
  • 20:10And you can see that it's expressed
  • 20:12strongly in the ganglionic eminence as
  • 20:14well as in the cortical ventricular zone.
  • 20:16But if you look in the cerebral cortex,
  • 20:18it's only expressed in a subset of cells,
  • 20:20And this was the first clue that it
  • 20:22may be different in the inhibitory
  • 20:24in the excitatory neurons.
  • 20:26And it turns out that ARX is expressed in
  • 20:29all of the ventricular zone of the cortex,
  • 20:32but it turns off as soon as those
  • 20:34cells exit the cell cycle.
  • 20:36However,
  • 20:36in the inhibitory neurons coming
  • 20:38out of that ganglionic evidence.
  • 20:40It stays on all the way into adulthood.
  • 20:43So it seems to have different
  • 20:45role differential roles in these
  • 20:47two different stem cells all the
  • 20:49way into the mature neuron.
  • 20:52So to study this what we did is
  • 20:55we generated a mouse that had
  • 20:57a that that was a Flox mouse.
  • 20:59It has we inserted crease it
  • 21:01was on either side of Exxon two.
  • 21:03Remember Exxon two at the beginning of
  • 21:06the homeo domains when we remove Exxon 2.
  • 21:08It actually everything else is out of frame.
  • 21:10You have no homeo domain and we
  • 21:13believe this is a null mutation.
  • 21:16We could then cross this mouse
  • 21:18that we generated with a variety of
  • 21:21different Cree driver mice and we
  • 21:23could remove a RX from just excitatory
  • 21:25or just inhibitory mice, sorry cells.
  • 21:30Now to take one step back,
  • 21:31a knockout,
  • 21:32A germline knockout had been made by Doctor
  • 21:35Kitamura in Japan and that was lethal.
  • 21:37So we couldn't study any later
  • 21:39effects with it.
  • 21:40So we wanted to really hone down
  • 21:42on different regions of the brain.
  • 21:44So that's what we did.
  • 21:44So here we've crossed this mouse
  • 21:47with one that expresses creed
  • 21:49just in the ganglionic eminence.
  • 21:51And so ARX is normal in these
  • 21:54excitatory radially migrating cells,
  • 21:55but it's completely absent
  • 21:57from the ganglionic eminence.
  • 21:59And when we do that,
  • 22:01what we see and hopefully I'll
  • 22:02get this to work.
  • 22:06There we go. So on your left
  • 22:08side is a normal brain.
  • 22:10We've genetically labeled all the
  • 22:13inhibitory neurons with a green dye
  • 22:16GFP and you can see them migrating
  • 22:18out of the ganglionic eminence
  • 22:20up towards the cortex and then
  • 22:22turning and going into the cortex.
  • 22:24Now what happens in the mutant
  • 22:26is that these cells,
  • 22:27you don't see quite the connection,
  • 22:28but they're migrating out
  • 22:30of the ganglionic eminence,
  • 22:32they're going up into the cortex.
  • 22:34But I'm sorry going up door sleep,
  • 22:36but they don't turn out of their
  • 22:38stream and go out into the cortex.
  • 22:40And also I don't know how
  • 22:42well you can see it,
  • 22:43but there's cells migrating out
  • 22:44here in the marginal zone and those
  • 22:46are completely absent from the ARX.
  • 22:49So it's completely eliminated one
  • 22:51stream of these inhibitory neurons
  • 22:53and it's there's a defect in the
  • 22:56migration of that other stream.
  • 22:58Now when we study these mice,
  • 23:00I can tell you the brain
  • 23:01is structurally normal.
  • 23:02This has all been published,
  • 23:03so I'm not going to go through details.
  • 23:04The brain is structurally normal.
  • 23:06The corpus callosum is intact.
  • 23:08The cell layers are intact.
  • 23:11Every single one of these mice that
  • 23:13we've tested has epilepsy from the
  • 23:15very first time we can look at it.
  • 23:18So they develop different types of epilepsy.
  • 23:20We see these spikes and slow wave
  • 23:22with an electrical decrement like
  • 23:24here that's what you see in children
  • 23:26with infantile spasm syndrome,
  • 23:28exactly like what you see with
  • 23:31the ARX mutations.
  • 23:32These are these Jacksonian type
  • 23:34seizures which start and then
  • 23:36propagate throughout the brain.
  • 23:38So we see an evolution and different
  • 23:41types of seizures starting from
  • 23:43the earliest time we can record it.
  • 23:45So these mice all have seizures.
  • 23:48And the brains, as I said,
  • 23:51are completely normal.
  • 23:54Now in contrast, if we remove a RX,
  • 23:57instead of from the ganglionic Eminence,
  • 24:00we remove it from the excited where
  • 24:02the excitatory neurons are being
  • 24:05derived using a different Cree driver,
  • 24:07what we see is something
  • 24:10completely different.
  • 24:10Now what you see is that
  • 24:12the the brains are small.
  • 24:14Remember I told you the brains
  • 24:16are structurally normal with the
  • 24:18removal from the inhibitory neurons.
  • 24:20The cortex is very thin,
  • 24:21it's actually dislaminated and
  • 24:23you have no corpus callosum.
  • 24:25So here you can see a normal
  • 24:28corpus callosum and here you see
  • 24:29a genesis of corpus callosum.
  • 24:31So by removing it from the
  • 24:34excitatory projection neurons you
  • 24:36get all the structural defects.
  • 24:38That you get none of the epilepsy.
  • 24:40We've looked at hundreds of mice.
  • 24:42We've never seen a seizure.
  • 24:44So now we're starting to
  • 24:46distinguish where does the kind
  • 24:48of cognitive behavioral and where
  • 24:50do some of the other neurologic
  • 24:52phenotypes where they derive from,
  • 24:55from a cell migration standpoint
  • 24:56and and this just shows.
  • 24:58The disruption of the cortical lamination,
  • 25:00we can look at at specific genes that
  • 25:03are expressed in specific laminate
  • 25:06and show defects predominantly
  • 25:08in the upper layer cells.
  • 25:10And those upper layer cells in the
  • 25:12mouse come from those intermediate
  • 25:15progenitor cells that I mentioned.
  • 25:17And so we looked at those intermediate
  • 25:19progenitor cells and what we found
  • 25:21doing single cell is that they are
  • 25:24missing something called CDKN 1C.
  • 25:27ARX is regulating CDKN 1C,
  • 25:30and that's a gene that's important for
  • 25:31cells being able to reenter the cell cycle.
  • 25:33So what's happening is these cells,
  • 25:35because they no longer express the CDKN 1C,
  • 25:39are exiting the cell cycle prematurely.
  • 25:42Therefore you're not getting the
  • 25:45correct number of of rounds of mitosis
  • 25:48in the intermediate progenitor
  • 25:49cell and you've got a defect in
  • 25:52those mostly upper layer neurons.
  • 25:56We can actually test that by going in
  • 25:59and we built a a system to actually
  • 26:03repress actually what happened.
  • 26:05Sorry, I just misspoke.
  • 26:06There's over expression of the CDKN 1C.
  • 26:08The ARX is a repressor of that.
  • 26:10We then knocked it down and we can see
  • 26:13the return of the proliferation and we
  • 26:16actually can rescue that phenotype of
  • 26:18loss of the mostly superficial neurons.
  • 26:21So this showed that that was one of the
  • 26:24mechanisms by which ARX was functioning.
  • 26:28So with this all in mind,
  • 26:31I want to turn our attention
  • 26:33to this set of two kids.
  • 26:35Now at the top is a child
  • 26:38who has an ARX mutation.
  • 26:40And that mutation, that child has had
  • 26:43epilepsy starting at about six months
  • 26:45in severe intellectual disabilities,
  • 26:47never was able to feed himself.
  • 26:50And you can see has the so-called
  • 26:53lizencephaly, the smooth brain
  • 26:54Lysos meaning smooth encephaly,
  • 26:56brain lizencephaly.
  • 26:57And you can see predominantly
  • 27:00in this posterior part.
  • 27:02There's no suicide here at all.
  • 27:05Here you can see posteriorly.
  • 27:07Now the child at the bottom also has
  • 27:10an ARX mutation, also had epilepsy.
  • 27:13It didn't start till about 9 to 10 months
  • 27:17and had severe intellectual disability.
  • 27:20But this brain is structurally normal.
  • 27:24The brain of the child at the top had
  • 27:27a truncation mutation loss of a RX.
  • 27:31The child at the bottom had an
  • 27:34alanine tract expansion mutation.
  • 27:36Now if we think about what I just told you,
  • 27:39the brain is structurally normal,
  • 27:41but you have the seizures if you have
  • 27:44a defect in your inhibitory neurons.
  • 27:47But it's only when you have
  • 27:48a defect in the migration,
  • 27:49the movement of those excitatory neurons
  • 27:52that you have the structural defects.
  • 27:55So we predicted that that's what
  • 27:57we would see if we tested this.
  • 27:59So that's what we did experimentally.
  • 28:02So to do these experiments,
  • 28:04we went in and and you can do something
  • 28:05called in utero electropration.
  • 28:07This is where you can take a mouse,
  • 28:09a gravid mom,
  • 28:11you can exteriorize the uterus.
  • 28:13You can go in and check some of
  • 28:16an expression construct into the
  • 28:18ventricle of the into the pup while
  • 28:20it's in the uterus and you put
  • 28:22these electrodes on either side,
  • 28:24you electroplate it just like
  • 28:25you would in a gel.
  • 28:26The DNA goes into the brain and then
  • 28:29you can actually return the grab the
  • 28:32uterus back into the mom sewer up
  • 28:34and those mice can go on and even be
  • 28:36delivered so we can study them later
  • 28:39and what you see is if you do that normally.
  • 28:42The cells migrate out to the cortex.
  • 28:44So this is just with the GFP
  • 28:46electroprated into our ARX Flox mice,
  • 28:48no Cree.
  • 28:49So they're just wild type.
  • 28:52If we introduce a brain
  • 28:54four or this how 3F4 Cree.
  • 28:57What you see when you electroprate in GFP,
  • 29:00now they've lost all of their ARX.
  • 29:03You see that these cells don't migrate.
  • 29:06If we rescue this with a wild type ARX,
  • 29:08you see the cells migrate out to the surface,
  • 29:10just not very nicely.
  • 29:12You have a very nice rescue.
  • 29:14But if you do that with the sorry,
  • 29:17and if you do that with the ARX
  • 29:19that has that expansion mutation,
  • 29:21you also rescue that radial migration.
  • 29:23These cells migrate out to the surface.
  • 29:26So the expansion mutation doesn't seem
  • 29:28to affect these excitatory neurons,
  • 29:30at least their ability to migrate out.
  • 29:36Now contrast that to the inhibitory neurons.
  • 29:38So now what we've done is when you put
  • 29:41it in the wild type, the cells migrate
  • 29:43out very nicely up into the cortex.
  • 29:44So now we've just electroplate
  • 29:46into the ganglionic eminence.
  • 29:48The cells migrate up when you put it in,
  • 29:51when you remove a RX from the whole brain,
  • 29:53you see that these cells
  • 29:54no longer migrate up.
  • 29:55They kind of get stuck down here
  • 29:57in what will become striatum.
  • 30:00If we rescue with the wild type ARX,
  • 30:02you can see that again these cells
  • 30:04are migrating up very nicely.
  • 30:06Yeah, you can see that.
  • 30:08But if we do that with the
  • 30:09alanine tract expansion mutation,
  • 30:11they don't.
  • 30:13So the alanine tract expansion seems
  • 30:16to be differentially affecting
  • 30:19the inhibitory neurons and a whole
  • 30:23series of experiments that we did,
  • 30:25we went on to show that it was due to
  • 30:28the ability of a RX and Progenitor cells
  • 30:31to affect different transcriptional pathways.
  • 30:36So in the inhibitory neurons,
  • 30:38what happens when you have
  • 30:39an alanine tract expansion?
  • 30:40Remember transcription isn't
  • 30:42isolated to 1 transcription factor,
  • 30:44it's a transcriptional complex usually
  • 30:46with 40 to 60 different proteins.
  • 30:49And so this complex usually
  • 30:51has stabilizers and cofactors.
  • 30:53And what happens in the we showed
  • 30:56through Co immunoprecipitation.
  • 30:58This complex falls apart and this T LE1
  • 31:02which is a gaucho like transcriptional
  • 31:05repressor involved in repressing
  • 31:07things like Ed F3 and also that CDKN
  • 31:101C that I mentioned no longer get repressed.
  • 31:14They continue to be overexpressed and that
  • 31:16leads to defects in these inhibitory norms.
  • 31:19However,
  • 31:20the transcriptional complex
  • 31:22is structurally different.
  • 31:24In the excitatory neurons such
  • 31:25that even with that polyne alanine
  • 31:27tract expansion that leads to a
  • 31:30conformational change in this protein,
  • 31:32you still get the structure of this,
  • 31:35the cofactors coming together and
  • 31:37it still can repress molecules
  • 31:40like LMO one which are required for
  • 31:43the excitatory neuron migration.
  • 31:45And so this shows how this one
  • 31:47transcription factor with a
  • 31:49single mutation can have different
  • 31:51functions in different cell,
  • 31:53in different progenitor cells
  • 31:55leading to different phenotypes.
  • 31:57Whereas in in if you remove a RX
  • 32:00it'll affect both of them together
  • 32:02and that's what happens with
  • 32:05the Liz encephaly phenotype.
  • 32:07Now we went on to take a non
  • 32:11unbiased proteomics approach to say.
  • 32:13Wow, this is really interesting.
  • 32:14What are all the proteins
  • 32:15that interact with a RX?
  • 32:17Because we want to understand that whole
  • 32:19transcriptional complex and we actually
  • 32:21uncovered a number of different proteins.
  • 32:23But unfortunately we were really
  • 32:26looking for other transcription factors
  • 32:28that are interacting with a RX,
  • 32:30and I'm going to come back to
  • 32:31that in a minute.
  • 32:31What we did find is a number
  • 32:33of things that are involved in
  • 32:35the when signaling pathway,
  • 32:36which is important for proliferation
  • 32:39in those cortical ventricular cells.
  • 32:40So I'm not going to that's that's
  • 32:43work we published.
  • 32:44I'm not going to talk about it today.
  • 32:46But we also found that it affected
  • 32:49dorsal ventral patterning in the brain.
  • 32:52So if you take the brain and this is
  • 32:55expression of Olig 2, some work we
  • 32:58published just a few years ago here
  • 33:00you can see a very nice border where a
  • 33:03RX is normally sorry where all Olig 2.
  • 33:07Stops being expressed, and you can
  • 33:09see that here in the coronal plane.
  • 33:11And if you look at it in the sagittal plane,
  • 33:15you can see that it stops at the top of
  • 33:19that gang, that lateral ganglionic eminence.
  • 33:21But that's not what happens
  • 33:24in the ARX mutant.
  • 33:25You can see that this olive two,
  • 33:27normally only expressed in the
  • 33:29ventral part of the brain,
  • 33:31is now expressed extensively
  • 33:33across the dorsal cortex.
  • 33:35And you can see it going up beyond where
  • 33:38we would normally see it in the wild type.
  • 33:41So this suggested that in addition
  • 33:44to a proliferation effect,
  • 33:46it was affecting patterning of the brain.
  • 33:49So what's important about that?
  • 33:52Oh, so we went on and showed that
  • 33:54it's not just the wind pathway,
  • 33:55but it's also FGFA.
  • 33:57And Sonic Hedgehog and we basically
  • 33:59were able to separate out all these
  • 34:01pathways and how that was affecting
  • 34:04all of two and downstream components.
  • 34:06Again,
  • 34:06these are not things I'm going to talk
  • 34:08about today in the interest of time.
  • 34:10But what we,
  • 34:11the reason we became interested
  • 34:14in this patterning is because
  • 34:16these mice as I've alluded to have
  • 34:19behavioral defects and we wanted
  • 34:21to understand what's the biologic
  • 34:23basis of these behavioral defects.
  • 34:26So it turns out that and again some studies
  • 34:28we did a few years ago back in 2014,
  • 34:31we showed that these mice
  • 34:33have normal strength.
  • 34:34We had they have a lot of they,
  • 34:36they have normal ability to recognize
  • 34:39things in a remember things in a swim
  • 34:41test when you do Morris water maze.
  • 34:44But they're socially abnormal.
  • 34:46And this is typical of mice that are
  • 34:49believed to be in the human equivalent
  • 34:51of the autism spectrum disorder,
  • 34:53which as I said is part of the ARX phenotype.
  • 34:57And so here the I'm just going to
  • 34:59show you one socialization test
  • 35:00we did where you you take this is
  • 35:03called the novel mouse experiment
  • 35:04and where you what you do is you
  • 35:06put one mouse in this plexiglass
  • 35:09thing with these little holes.
  • 35:11And you put in the other side
  • 35:12of it and either an empty one
  • 35:14of these or an inanimate object,
  • 35:16you can put like a stuffed animal
  • 35:17or something like that in it.
  • 35:19And this is the test mouse right here.
  • 35:20And this mouse has never seen this mouse.
  • 35:23They're social creatures.
  • 35:24They're going to go and interact
  • 35:25with each other.
  • 35:26And you can see that when you
  • 35:27put in these novel mice,
  • 35:29the the blue bar shows you how much time,
  • 35:31the percent of time that they're spending
  • 35:34with that novel with that new mouse.
  • 35:36Whereas the mutants don't do that.
  • 35:39So they've got a socialization defect and
  • 35:40we showed that through several different,
  • 35:42but like I said this is just one of those.
  • 35:45So coming back to this patterning,
  • 35:48this was really interesting to us because
  • 35:50if you look at patterning dorsal ventral
  • 35:52patterning in the spinal cord where it's
  • 35:55been best elucidated work by Tom Jessel
  • 35:58and James Briscoe and others, many others.
  • 36:02What what we now know is that there's
  • 36:04specific cell types that come from different
  • 36:07regions along the progenitor zone,
  • 36:09from dorsal to ventral in the spinal cord,
  • 36:11and that these are defined by Sonic hedgehog
  • 36:15expressed out of the noto cord and then
  • 36:18in the floor plate and on the roof plate,
  • 36:20BMP's and wins,
  • 36:22and these both induce and repress.
  • 36:26Different transcription factors,
  • 36:28setting up these different domains
  • 36:31along the dorsal ventral axis.
  • 36:33And the same thing seems to be happening
  • 36:36in setting up different domains in
  • 36:39the cerebral cortex that define,
  • 36:41as I mentioned before, where the thalamic,
  • 36:44thalamic cortical projections
  • 36:45are going to be.
  • 36:46So you're getting your, you know,
  • 36:48visual cortex back here.
  • 36:50You get your motor cortex,
  • 36:52your sensory cortex.
  • 36:53All of these primary cortices
  • 36:56are coming out of definition,
  • 36:58and that definition starts
  • 37:00with these different gradients,
  • 37:04first identified by Denis O'Leary
  • 37:06and his group,
  • 37:08that you get a gradient of different
  • 37:11factors that turn on different
  • 37:13transcription factors in the brain,
  • 37:15and it's the overlap of these different
  • 37:19patterns of these signaling molecules.
  • 37:21Turning on transcription factors that
  • 37:24define these different regions of the brain,
  • 37:27well,
  • 37:27we had not actually been able to
  • 37:29find any transcription factors.
  • 37:30So we took a new approach,
  • 37:32something called Samoa which
  • 37:34stands for single molecule assay.
  • 37:36And this is a highly sensitive methodology
  • 37:39to be able to look at a single
  • 37:42molecules interacting with each other.
  • 37:44And we had to build a transcription
  • 37:46factor library because one didn't
  • 37:48exist for just the brains,
  • 37:49we took all we actually.
  • 37:51We cloned into a library all the
  • 37:53transcription factors that are
  • 37:54expressed in the brain and we then
  • 37:57asked which ones interact with ARX.
  • 37:59And when we did that
  • 38:02we were able to find a number of
  • 38:05different factor transcription factors.
  • 38:07Now that interacted with arx and
  • 38:08you can see they have overlapping
  • 38:10patterns in different regions,
  • 38:12so some just in the cortex,
  • 38:15others just in the ganglionic eminence.
  • 38:18And others that are both in
  • 38:19the cortex and the ganglionic
  • 38:20eminence in different patterns.
  • 38:22So this one just like ARX stronger
  • 38:24in the more posterior brain,
  • 38:27less so in the anterior.
  • 38:29And when we take and look at these
  • 38:32different transcription factors,
  • 38:33what we see is that the patterning,
  • 38:35the gene expression that define these
  • 38:37different regions in the brain is disrupted.
  • 38:40So you can see here in the
  • 38:43ARX and it's a a loss.
  • 38:45Of your frontal cortex and a
  • 38:48expansion of the ventral cortex.
  • 38:52When we lose a RX and to try and then map,
  • 38:57but we want to try and map,
  • 38:59well, OK,
  • 38:59these these these regions are
  • 39:01abnormal and the behaviors abnormal.
  • 39:03Can we connect those two?
  • 39:05So we wanted to look at the actual
  • 39:08Axon trajectories from these
  • 39:09different regions to strike,
  • 39:10start mapping that out and
  • 39:13so we can go into a mouse.
  • 39:16And we can actually inject these
  • 39:18different tracers that go along
  • 39:20axons are ones that cross axons.
  • 39:22And when we do that,
  • 39:23what you can see here in the sensory,
  • 39:25so this is Ventrilateral,
  • 39:26this is ventral posterior.
  • 39:28You can see the normal mapping of
  • 39:31the ventral lateral from the brain.
  • 39:34But here the ventral posterior despite
  • 39:37you having normal axons coming
  • 39:39through the internal commissure,
  • 39:40sorry, the internal capsule.
  • 39:42They don't go to ventral posterior.
  • 39:45So we can show very specific mapping
  • 39:47abnormalities and then correlate those
  • 39:49with these behavioral abnormalities
  • 39:51that we're seeing in the brain.
  • 39:54So we're going everywhere from the
  • 39:57molecular understanding of how the
  • 39:58cortex is defined to how that sets
  • 40:01up projections and connections and
  • 40:03what that does for the behavior
  • 40:05of the brain giving us a really a
  • 40:07view from behavior to the molecule.
  • 40:10In the brain and in these developmental
  • 40:13disorders.
  • 40:16So that's some ongoing work we're doing.
  • 40:18And and now I want to shift a little
  • 40:21bit in the last 15 minutes or so
  • 40:24and talk about some other relatively
  • 40:26new work we've been doing and that
  • 40:28is related to these two brains.
  • 40:31So I already told you about this or
  • 40:34showed you the brain of the ARX with an
  • 40:38alanine tract expansion where you have an.
  • 40:40A structurally normal brain, but with
  • 40:43epilepsy and intellectual disabilities.
  • 40:46Now these two kids also have epilepsy,
  • 40:50mild intellectual disabilities,
  • 40:52but a structurally normal brain.
  • 40:56Now there may be some signal abnormality.
  • 40:58You can see a little bit of brightness
  • 41:00here in the thalamus and in the
  • 41:02basal ganglia here in this one.
  • 41:04And and that's typical of these disorders.
  • 41:06These are due to mitochondrial disorders.
  • 41:10So these are mitochondrial related
  • 41:12epilepsies and these have always been
  • 41:14thought to be due to a energy problem
  • 41:17that the the might be neurons require
  • 41:19a tremendous amount of of energy and
  • 41:22you just have an energy deficiency.
  • 41:23If we could replace that,
  • 41:25we'd be able to fix their epilepsy.
  • 41:27Turns out it hasn't been that simple
  • 41:28and it it doesn't work that well.
  • 41:30So we we started thinking,
  • 41:32you know,
  • 41:33maybe it's more complicated than that.
  • 41:35And so we first looked at the
  • 41:37literature and it turns out that a
  • 41:39significant percentage of kids that
  • 41:41have mitochondrial disorders have
  • 41:44either abnormal Eegs or frank epilepsy.
  • 41:49And so we started studying this and
  • 41:52we looked at both the excitatory and
  • 41:54inhibitory neurons because our prediction is,
  • 41:56remember the inhibitory when you have
  • 41:58a defect in the excitatory neurons,
  • 42:00the brains are structurally normal,
  • 42:02but with inhibitory neurons you get a.
  • 42:05Seizures and intellectual disabilities.
  • 42:08So on the left side we've labeled
  • 42:12migrating excitatory neurons in green,
  • 42:16and then in red we've labeled
  • 42:18their mitochondria.
  • 42:19And what you can see is it migrates
  • 42:21across is that the mitochondria
  • 42:23stay just in front of the cell body
  • 42:26as it migrates all the way across,
  • 42:28and we've mapped that.
  • 42:29At the top you can see a quantitation
  • 42:31of that.
  • 42:33But if you look at the of the,
  • 42:34sorry,
  • 42:34the one on the right that's in a
  • 42:36migrating inhibitory neuron and
  • 42:38the mitochondria behave very
  • 42:40differently in all of them.
  • 42:41Inhibitory neurons,
  • 42:42they move around through the cells.
  • 42:45Sometimes they go up
  • 42:46into the leading process,
  • 42:47particularly at branch points,
  • 42:48sometimes they're behind the cell body
  • 42:51and sometimes they're in front of it.
  • 42:53So there's this
  • 42:56tremendous dynamic movement in the
  • 42:58inhibitory neurons that you don't
  • 43:00see in the excitatory neurons.
  • 43:01And so to study this further,
  • 43:03so this is just observation,
  • 43:05we actually started to interrogate
  • 43:07that we used a genetic model
  • 43:09where we removed this Ant 1-2,
  • 43:12this is a transporter of a TP.
  • 43:16We also did it pharmacologically
  • 43:18using boncretic acid,
  • 43:19which actually is an inhibitor of this
  • 43:22transporter or we used oligomycin,
  • 43:24which is an inhibitor of the last
  • 43:27step in the oxidative phosphorylation.
  • 43:29And I will tell you,
  • 43:31but I'm not going to show you any data
  • 43:33to ensure that it wasn't just lycolysis.
  • 43:35We blocked lycolysis.
  • 43:36What we were able to show is that
  • 43:39radial migration is completely
  • 43:47whereas the inhibitory neuron
  • 43:48is required for is requires
  • 43:50oxidative phosphorylation.
  • 43:52But I'm not going to talk
  • 43:53about that part again,
  • 43:53this has been published now if
  • 43:56you look, let's see, let's.
  • 44:01How come these movies are not playing?
  • 44:07They were working a second ago. Okay,
  • 44:11I'm not going to be able to show
  • 44:13you these movies on your left is in
  • 44:16in culture looking at inhibitory
  • 44:19neurons and what you'd see is that
  • 44:22that on the top, a normal one.
  • 44:26These, these inhibitory neurons have
  • 44:28tremendous branching as they migrate.
  • 44:30And it it'll just kind of zoom
  • 44:32across the the field at the bottom.
  • 44:34What you'll see is that when you add
  • 44:37oligomycin or when we do it with bancrecic
  • 44:39acid or when we do it genetically,
  • 44:41they don't just migrate across the field.
  • 44:43They go forward and then you see a
  • 44:46leading process coming out of the rear
  • 44:48and the cell actually turning around
  • 44:50and going backwards and going back
  • 44:52and forth and not branching as much.
  • 44:54And then if you look in the genetic
  • 44:56model where we actually are looking
  • 44:58at the cells migrating up,
  • 44:59you'll see a defect in those
  • 45:01cells going up into the cortex,
  • 45:03these inhibitory neurons.
  • 45:06What what we found is that when we do this,
  • 45:11we. So why do cells turn around?
  • 45:13It turns out all epithelial
  • 45:14cells are polarized, right?
  • 45:16And they're polarized.
  • 45:17You have the nucleus and then you have the
  • 45:19MTOC, the microtubule organizing center.
  • 45:22It's also known as the centriole.
  • 45:23In the dividing cell,
  • 45:24it's the same thing.
  • 45:25It's where the microtubules are organized
  • 45:29as cells migrate that that MTOC moves
  • 45:32out into the leading process and then
  • 45:34the nucleus gets pulled behind it.
  • 45:35That's how they migrate out.
  • 45:37But in these cells,
  • 45:39when you lose the mitochondria,
  • 45:40you lose that polarization.
  • 45:42And both in the genetic model
  • 45:45and in the pharmacologic models.
  • 45:47The mitochondria, sorry, the MTOC,
  • 45:49is found over the top of the nucleus
  • 45:51or sometimes even behind the nucleus,
  • 45:53and that's when those cells
  • 45:54are turning around.
  • 45:55So you're losing the polar polarity that
  • 45:58a normal epithelial cell in a neuron has.
  • 46:01And that's what we have found
  • 46:05so more recently,
  • 46:06and that that works all been published.
  • 46:10So more recently we started asking, well,
  • 46:12mitochondria move through the cell, why?
  • 46:14Why are they moving in inhibitory neurons?
  • 46:17And it turns out that mitochondria are
  • 46:20moved along using molecular motors,
  • 46:22moved along microtubules.
  • 46:25And so we started thinking about,
  • 46:27you know,
  • 46:28how could we disrupt that and
  • 46:30the way that the mitochondria
  • 46:32attached these molecular motors,
  • 46:34because dyneins and kinesins move many
  • 46:37types of cargo throughout the cell,
  • 46:39putting things that like Polybee studying,
  • 46:41but the they they attached to
  • 46:44the mitochondria using these.
  • 46:46Adapter proteins,
  • 46:46which are called mirror one and mirror
  • 46:49two and there's another one called track,
  • 46:52Now mirror one and mirror two,
  • 46:54sorry, mirror two is almost not
  • 46:56expressed in the developing brain.
  • 46:57So we just focused on mirror one.
  • 47:00And so mirror one's the main adapter protein.
  • 47:03And if we take a knockout mirror one,
  • 47:05what you see is hearing in
  • 47:07normal mice in radial migration,
  • 47:10you can see that the.
  • 47:12Mitochondria are always out
  • 47:13at the front of that,
  • 47:15just like I showed you before,
  • 47:17are always out at the front
  • 47:18of the leading process.
  • 47:19These cells are migrating up.
  • 47:21If you look at the when
  • 47:23we knock out mirror one,
  • 47:25most of the time the mirror ones at
  • 47:27the back of the nucleus and again
  • 47:30these cells migrate out normally.
  • 47:32So it's not affecting their migration,
  • 47:34it's just affecting the localization
  • 47:35of the mitochondria.
  • 47:36Sometimes it's even in front and in back,
  • 47:38like you see here and here.
  • 47:41In contrast,
  • 47:42when we look at the inhibitory neurons,
  • 47:45we see that the the the mitochondria
  • 47:46all of a sudden just aggregate
  • 47:48at the front of the cell.
  • 47:50So we do something called expansion
  • 47:52microscopy where we can actually
  • 47:53get a better look at these cells.
  • 47:56Looking at the mitochondria we see
  • 47:57not only is are they they stuck,
  • 48:00but they're also abnormal.
  • 48:02They become very small and punctate
  • 48:05as opposed to being nice and
  • 48:07long and sometimes branched.
  • 48:08As you see here,
  • 48:10so this is the defects that we see.
  • 48:13And we also see the same
  • 48:15polarization defects.
  • 48:16So we can look at the direction
  • 48:17these cells are migrating and we
  • 48:19this is just how we calculated it.
  • 48:21Oh, whoops.
  • 48:23Oh, I must have, sorry I took out a slide.
  • 48:25But when we quantitate this,
  • 48:27you can see that there's a
  • 48:28huge number of cells
  • 48:29that are going in the wrong
  • 48:31direction here. They're all supposed
  • 48:32to be going north up this way.
  • 48:37And these mice are also
  • 48:39behaviorally abnormal.
  • 48:40So normally mice,
  • 48:41if you put these little white
  • 48:43pads like this one you see here,
  • 48:46they build a nest out of it and
  • 48:48you can see the wild type mouse
  • 48:50over there building this nice
  • 48:51normal round nest that they use.
  • 48:54But if you look at these
  • 48:55marijuana knockout mice,
  • 48:56they they don't build a nest at all.
  • 48:58You can see some don't do anything,
  • 49:01some tear it up a little bit,
  • 49:02some tear it up a little more,
  • 49:03but they don't make an organized nest.
  • 49:06They also have a interesting
  • 49:09behavioral phenotype where
  • 49:11they they have a high anxiety.
  • 49:14So the way you test anxiety in the
  • 49:17mouse is that normally a mouse if you
  • 49:20put it into a open field they're they
  • 49:24they'll explore the entire box okay.
  • 49:27But if you if they have.
  • 49:30What's thought to be anxiety and
  • 49:32again you know, anthropomorphosizing.
  • 49:34What a mouse does,
  • 49:35if it has high anxiety,
  • 49:37they will tend to stay outside of the
  • 49:39center and stay at the edges of the box.
  • 49:42And here's what our mice with
  • 49:43mirror one mutations look like.
  • 49:45They stay at the outside of the box as
  • 49:48opposed to exploring the middle of the box.
  • 49:51The other thing that's quite interesting
  • 49:53is if you look at a mouse in what's
  • 49:55called an elevated plus maze,
  • 49:57and this is a little more than an
  • 49:59elevated plus maze because the.
  • 50:00The arms that go up and down
  • 50:04are actually enclosed,
  • 50:05whereas the ones that go sideways are open.
  • 50:09And once again,
  • 50:09if you look at a wild type mouse,
  • 50:11it'll explore all of those.
  • 50:13But interestingly, these mice,
  • 50:14and this is a little different than anxiety.
  • 50:17This is thought to be kind of a
  • 50:20claustrophobia type phenotype.
  • 50:21You see that they don't enter
  • 50:23that closed box at all,
  • 50:24They're anxious about entering
  • 50:26the closed arms.
  • 50:28Of this,
  • 50:29whereas the wild type go through
  • 50:31all of the arms of these boxes.
  • 50:33So again,
  • 50:34these behavioral defects and we think
  • 50:39the brains are structurally normal,
  • 50:41the brains of the kids are
  • 50:43structurally normal.
  • 50:44They have these different
  • 50:46behavioral phenotypes,
  • 50:47they have epilepsy just like these mice do.
  • 50:53And so we're now looking to see and
  • 50:55and this is going to be the end of
  • 50:57the story that we're looking to
  • 50:58see what the connectivity is and
  • 51:00what the changes in gene expression
  • 51:02leading us to really think that
  • 51:04this epilepsy phenotype that you
  • 51:06have in the behavioral phenotype
  • 51:08is not just an energy defect.
  • 51:10It's actually a neurodevelopmental
  • 51:12defect from these inhibitory neurons.
  • 51:15And this is just a summation
  • 51:16diagram where you see the,
  • 51:20the excitatory neurons,
  • 51:21they migrate out normally.
  • 51:22We don't know that they function normally
  • 51:24with the with without mirror one,
  • 51:26but we know they migrate normally and
  • 51:28they form a structurally normal brain.
  • 51:29But in contrast, these inhibitory
  • 51:31neurons don't migrate out normally.
  • 51:33The mitochondria stay localized right
  • 51:35at the front as opposed to distributing
  • 51:37in different parts of the cell.
  • 51:39And we actually have been able
  • 51:41to show that this is due to
  • 51:44the the the inhibitory neurons.
  • 51:46They need mitochondria in
  • 51:48the rear of the cell.
  • 51:49Because they require a separate type of.
  • 51:53It's kind of like thinking about
  • 51:55squeezing a toothpaste at the back.
  • 51:57They need that for their migration in
  • 51:59addition to the pulling from the front,
  • 52:02whereas the excitatory neurons don't
  • 52:03seem to need that or as much of that.
  • 52:07So let me end by just doing a
  • 52:11brief summary and then thanking
  • 52:12the people who did the work.
  • 52:14Hopefully I've given you a
  • 52:16little bit of insight into how
  • 52:18inner neurons might be involved.
  • 52:19In these different
  • 52:21neurodevelopmental disabilities,
  • 52:21by by being able to tease out the
  • 52:24specific role of the inhibitory
  • 52:26neurons in the epilepsy versus the
  • 52:29structural defects and now even
  • 52:31the behavioral defects and how
  • 52:33that affects these different mice.
  • 52:35We begin to get new insights in
  • 52:44to how these different defects
  • 52:46led to new studies in terms of.
  • 52:48What, how you might be
  • 52:50treating these patients.
  • 52:51So for example through another set
  • 52:53of experiments I didn't talk about,
  • 52:55we there there was identified a an
  • 53:00estrogen related molecule that's
  • 53:01actually pretty effective for the
  • 53:03alanine tract expansion mouse mutants.
  • 53:05And Jeff Nobles and at Baylor gave this
  • 53:08estrogen to these mice and actually
  • 53:09did a very nice job at improving them
  • 53:11and that went into a clinical trial
  • 53:14which I actually don't know the results of.
  • 53:16It also has led to several different groups
  • 53:20now looking at replacing the inhibitory
  • 53:24neurons using differentiated IPSC cells.
  • 53:26And so you're specifically looking at
  • 53:29not just differentiated inter neurons
  • 53:31but even subtypes which I didn't really
  • 53:34talk about specifically parvalbumin as
  • 53:36opposed to the somatostatin subpopulations.
  • 53:39I also hope that I've given you a
  • 53:41little bit insight how different
  • 53:42mutations in the same gene.
  • 53:45Can result in two different phenotypes.
  • 53:47So when you lose that gene you you affect
  • 53:49both excitatory and inhibitory neurons,
  • 53:51at least for a RX,
  • 53:53whereas the alanine tract
  • 53:54expansion only seem to affect one
  • 53:57population of inhibitory neurons,
  • 53:59giving you different phenotypes
  • 54:00in these different cell types and
  • 54:03therefore different phenotypes.
  • 54:05I've also,
  • 54:05through the mitochondria work,
  • 54:07talked about how metabolic
  • 54:09perturbations in the inhibitory neurons.
  • 54:12Because they require that
  • 54:14oxidated phosphorylation,
  • 54:16they no longer are able to function
  • 54:17and I think this contributes to
  • 54:19the phenotype in these patients
  • 54:21and really should we should start
  • 54:23thinking about new ways for therapy
  • 54:25as opposed to just trying to correct
  • 54:28the energetics in the brain itself.
  • 54:31So let me end there and really thank
  • 54:34the people that have done this.
  • 54:36All the recent work on the marijuana
  • 54:38has been done by a postdoc in my lab,
  • 54:40Abby Myers,
  • 54:41who just now has a job as a as an
  • 54:46assistant professor at Hamilton College.
  • 54:48The work on the Alanine tract
  • 54:51expansion was done by AM DPHD
  • 54:54student in in my lab a while back.
  • 54:57MacLean is Ralia.
  • 54:59All the epilepsy work is done in
  • 55:01collaboration with Eric Marsh,
  • 55:03who was a postdoc in my lab and
  • 55:05we still collaborate together.
  • 55:07And the some of the other work I I
  • 55:11mentioned was done by Erica Lynn Hendel.
  • 55:13She did the early mitochondrial work
  • 55:17and all of this is supported and and
  • 55:20really my labs been largely run I would
  • 55:25say by Ginam Cho and now young Shin Lin.
  • 55:28As they've been with me
  • 55:29for a number of years.
  • 55:31So with that I'll stop and I'm
  • 55:32happy to take any questions.
  • 55:33Thank you very much.
  • 55:41Yes,
  • 55:45didn't show any data,
  • 55:48but what about
  • 55:51this
  • 55:54polarization microphone may actually be
  • 55:59that could affect migration?
  • 56:02Yeah, yeah. So I you're right,
  • 56:04I didn't show we we have done the
  • 56:07the energetics data looking at a
  • 56:09TP localization in the cell and
  • 56:11it's it's kind of interesting.
  • 56:13I'll come to your calcium
  • 56:14question in just a second.
  • 56:15It's actually very interesting.
  • 56:16If you look at adult neurons,
  • 56:18mitochondria actually have to go out
  • 56:21into the into the processes of the
  • 56:24cell mostly the dendrites where new.
  • 56:27Synapses are being formed.
  • 56:29So synaptic plasticity is highly
  • 56:31dependent on mitochondria being
  • 56:32shuttled through this same mechanism
  • 56:35along with molecular motors out to
  • 56:37those sites and then they move back
  • 56:39as those synapses are being formed.
  • 56:41We think
  • 56:47that's a similar defect the cell
  • 56:49and in your it isn't heard.
  • 56:50I haven't I am showing we have done a little
  • 56:52bit of calcium signaling on with this.
  • 56:54We've done it more to look at the epilepsy
  • 56:55and looking at the calcium waves for example
  • 56:58in the hippocampus and in the cortex.
  • 56:59And and there are significant abnormalities.
  • 57:02We have not yet looked at it in terms of
  • 57:04intracellular localization of calcium,
  • 57:07which as you know is is a little bit harder.
  • 57:08But we that that is something I'd like to do,
  • 57:12but we just haven't done that yet.
  • 57:13Yeah. Yeah. So wondering if you
  • 57:18follow that, but the certain fact is that
  • 57:24underlying the sociability and anxiety.
  • 57:27The same circuits affected by epilepsy or
  • 57:33yeah, it's it's hard to disentangle
  • 57:37that we think So at least the one
  • 57:39circuit we have we think correlates
  • 57:41very nicely with some of the one of
  • 57:43the behavioral phenotypes that we have.
  • 57:47When you look at these mice
  • 57:50and the epilepsy we see a.
  • 57:52Simultaneous disorganization of
  • 57:54electrical activity in in both
  • 57:58hemispheres and both hippocampus.
  • 58:00So when we when we do our analysis,
  • 58:03we put two frontal electrodes in
  • 58:06and two hippocampus electrodes in.
  • 58:08So we're recording 4 sites
  • 58:10simultaneously in these mice and
  • 58:13we see disorganization of the
  • 58:14cortex in and the hippocampus
  • 58:16in all areas and seizures can
  • 58:19come from different locations.
  • 58:21So we don't think it's circuit specific.
  • 58:23We think it's a more generalized
  • 58:25disorganization of the of all the circuits.
  • 58:30Yeah,
  • 58:36yeah, absolutely.
  • 58:46Yeah.
  • 58:54Yeah, yeah. So we, we started to look
  • 58:59at both fission and fusion and and
  • 59:01unfortunately those experiments had fizzled.
  • 59:04So I don't have any data to
  • 59:06show you we tried to do that.
  • 59:09We do think that it's a failure of
  • 59:11fission as opposed to a fusion.
  • 59:13We think they can break apart, but.
  • 59:16When we've studied that, we've studied
  • 59:19that in a couple different ways.
  • 59:21The cells get really sick when we
  • 59:24try and disrupt that specifically.
  • 59:26And so I don't think we,
  • 59:28the cells get sick, they die,
  • 59:29they don't move, they don't divide,
  • 59:31they don't do a lot of things.
  • 59:32And so that's been a really
  • 59:33hard thing for us to study,
  • 59:34at least in these neurons.
  • 59:36I know you can study it in other cell types,
  • 59:38but when we try and introduce some
  • 59:40of those genes that are involved
  • 59:42in both fission and fusion or
  • 59:44actually we've disrupted them. It is.
  • 59:46It is not gone very well for us.
  • 59:48So I wish I could answer that.
  • 59:50We've tried.
  • 59:51It's a it's a great question
  • 59:53and I'd like to get an answer,
  • 59:54but that's been a hard thing to study.
  • 59:58Yes Sir,
  • 01:00:01totally something that
  • 01:00:03I'm told it got wrong.
  • 01:00:05So I'm really fascinated to try to
  • 01:00:08connect with 1/3 of the dollars.
  • 01:00:10Seems like there is a rather
  • 01:00:12large gap in there.
  • 01:00:14I don't get any together or
  • 01:00:18just one basic question is
  • 01:00:20that's the thing just seems
  • 01:00:26like there's this behavioral.
  • 01:00:35Yeah, you're right. It's a big
  • 01:00:37jump from molecular to behavioral.
  • 01:00:39But that is what we're trying to piece
  • 01:00:40together one, one part at a time.
  • 01:00:44In terms of the specificity, the behavior
  • 01:00:48is actually becomes very specific.
  • 01:00:52Why can I say that? Well, we do,
  • 01:00:54you do this whole battery of tests,
  • 01:00:56we did smell tests.
  • 01:00:57You do these bearing of different
  • 01:00:59marbles and things like that and
  • 01:01:00look at how fast they can find them.
  • 01:01:02You can do these memory tests you can do.
  • 01:01:08You you have to test gate,
  • 01:01:09you have to test strength because if they,
  • 01:01:11if they're not strong enough
  • 01:01:12or they can't walk, they don't,
  • 01:01:13they can't do some of these things,
  • 01:01:15they won't go over and see that other mouse.
  • 01:01:17So there's a whole battery of tests we
  • 01:01:20do and show that they're all normal.
  • 01:01:23I mean these mice behave normally
  • 01:01:25in in almost all these tests,
  • 01:01:27but it gets very specific for these
  • 01:01:29socialization tests where we've
  • 01:01:30see all the defects. So the the,
  • 01:01:33the specificity of it is very good.
  • 01:01:37Now the next step, though,
  • 01:01:38is what does it mean? I mean,
  • 01:01:40that's the anthropomorphosizing, right?
  • 01:01:42What is an autistic mouse?
  • 01:01:44I mean, you don't really know that, right?
  • 01:01:46So we use these kind of surrogates
  • 01:01:50that the field has, has adopted to use.
  • 01:01:53And and that's, I think, the best we can do.
  • 01:01:56But there is, I think,
  • 01:01:57a high degree of specificity.
  • 01:02:012 questions. First,
  • 01:02:02have you gone beyond the fact you
  • 01:02:05have a transcriptional regulator?
  • 01:02:07And there's migratory in terms of
  • 01:02:11looking the downstream mechanism
  • 01:02:12and what are the gradients
  • 01:02:14there caused in migratory.
  • 01:02:17And I'll give you the second question,
  • 01:02:19if you look at stamping structure,
  • 01:02:22please sample any change there.
  • 01:02:31I don't have it in here. Yeah.
  • 01:02:32So we actually published.
  • 01:02:35Several articles.
  • 01:02:36They're all in J. Neuroscience.
  • 01:02:38Dan Lysco was a graduate student in
  • 01:02:40my lab who was really interested in
  • 01:02:42this and and he started looking at
  • 01:02:44gradients of different molecules
  • 01:02:46and their role in the migration
  • 01:02:48and actually connecting the
  • 01:02:50gradients of different things,
  • 01:02:53particularly in the wind signaling
  • 01:02:55pathway which is expressed
  • 01:02:57both in the meninges and in the
  • 01:03:01that that migratory stream.
  • 01:03:03And show that that went signaling
  • 01:03:06going through the receptors through
  • 01:03:09the frizzled receptors actually
  • 01:03:11connects to the micro to the both
  • 01:03:14actin and microtubule cytoskeleton
  • 01:03:16and affects the ability of that to
  • 01:03:19branch and do things in the migration.
  • 01:03:28So, so that and. And also the
  • 01:03:31the other one actually that he
  • 01:03:35studied even more was SD F4 and so
  • 01:03:42CDCX CR4 and CX CL12 which is the
  • 01:03:45ligand and he was able to show
  • 01:03:47that those are modulated by a RX.
  • 01:03:52We haven't looked at all at synaptic,
  • 01:03:54it would be another area to go,
  • 01:03:55but we have not done that, yeah.
  • 01:04:02Look at the difference that make this
  • 01:04:07thing to run out or in
  • 01:04:14human and so
  • 01:04:18we've kind of we've done descriptive
  • 01:04:21pathology in the human and we've been able
  • 01:04:25to look at 2 humans with a RX mutations.
  • 01:04:28And they do have a deficit in
  • 01:04:30the inhibitory neurons that's
  • 01:04:31greater than their excitatory ones.
  • 01:04:33But the the two brains we've looked at
  • 01:04:35both had the Liz encephalophenotype.
  • 01:04:37So they also had a, a structural defect.
  • 01:04:39So it wasn't so easy to parse that out.
  • 01:04:42And then we, you know,
  • 01:04:44most of the works been in the mouse.
  • 01:04:45We have not been able to,
  • 01:04:46we've not done anything in the humans in
  • 01:04:49terms of trying to study the migration,
  • 01:04:51but we'd like to do that.
  • 01:04:53And just recently, I've gotten 2 patients.
  • 01:04:57They're they're actually brothers
  • 01:04:59that have a RX mutations.
  • 01:05:03The parents have consented and we've
  • 01:05:04gotten and we're in the process of making
  • 01:05:07IPSC's from those ARX mutation patients,
  • 01:05:10which are going to allow us to then
  • 01:05:12look at the migration of those cells,
  • 01:05:15both putting them into like nude mice
  • 01:05:17but also in culture to be able to study
  • 01:05:19how do they behave when we differentiate
  • 01:05:21them to inner neurons or excitatory neurons.
  • 01:05:23So that is the next step to kind of
  • 01:05:26make that transition and understand
  • 01:05:27what's the relationship of these two,
  • 01:05:30the humans.
  • 01:05:30So that's what we're doing right now
  • 01:05:32to do that and I'm excited to have
  • 01:05:35these two patients and grateful there
  • 01:05:41is some evidence. Yeah.
  • 01:05:45So I think that's.
  • 01:05:46So to work that you did a number
  • 01:05:48of years ago that was published in
  • 01:05:50Neuron and then work that Thomas
  • 01:05:52Noakowski at UCSF has recently
  • 01:05:54done doing single cell work really
  • 01:05:57I think defines that there is a
  • 01:06:00subpopulation of the inhibitory
  • 01:06:02neurons that are coming from
  • 01:06:04the cortical ventricular zone.
  • 01:06:08All the studies in mice don't
  • 01:06:10seem to have supported that.
  • 01:06:11That's at least my feeling from it you.
  • 01:06:13Yeah, but I I didn't see yet.
  • 01:06:17Maybe, yes Marks. Yeah. Yeah.
  • 01:06:29Yeah. So I think that's right.
  • 01:06:30And I do think that there are
  • 01:06:32those cells that are coming
  • 01:06:33from there and by studying these
  • 01:06:35ipsc's and differentiating
  • 01:06:36them along different lines,
  • 01:06:38we should be able to get at these answers.
  • 01:06:40But I don't know now
  • 01:06:44any other questions? All right.
  • 01:06:47Well, thank you very much.