Skip to Main Content

Todd Constable “Networks in brain-behavior modeling”

March 10, 2023
ID
9646

Transcript

  • 00:05Here in the sessions meeting.
  • 00:08And I'm, I'm. I have the luxury
  • 00:10because I'm going last because I
  • 00:13saw some other people's slides.
  • 00:15I've adjusted my talk. But.
  • 00:18So I'm going to talk about
  • 00:20networks and behavior modeling,
  • 00:21and some of this is like, I don't know.
  • 00:26The slides aren't advancing.
  • 00:27Or are they no OK?
  • 00:33After what?
  • 00:37In the middle of what? There we go.
  • 00:40So I I'm introducing some objectives.
  • 00:43Sage and her talk said that
  • 00:44we needed to have, you know,
  • 00:46be clear on what our clinical objectives are.
  • 00:49And so some of the objectives
  • 00:51here in terms of bringing behavior
  • 00:53modeling that I'm interested in
  • 00:55are the clinical applications and
  • 00:57understanding brain disorders.
  • 00:58So what can this tell us about the brain?
  • 01:00And when the brain doesn't work,
  • 01:02the brain alone when we just look
  • 01:04at the brain, doesn't really
  • 01:06provide sufficient information for,
  • 01:08you know, diagnostic categorization.
  • 01:10Let's say mental illness and
  • 01:12behavior alone also doesn't do that.
  • 01:15But the hope is that these brain
  • 01:17behavior models will someday do this,
  • 01:19and they don't do that yet.
  • 01:20But that's the goal.
  • 01:23In Europe we have the IC D2 manual
  • 01:27for classification of diseases.
  • 01:29In the US it's the DSM 5.
  • 01:32Neither of these work very well,
  • 01:34and they've been around for 40
  • 01:36years or 50 years.
  • 01:38But they're not biologically
  • 01:40based and they yield these very
  • 01:42heterogeneous diagnosis with multiple
  • 01:45combinations of symptoms that are
  • 01:48insufficient really to provide to
  • 01:50provide a good diagnosis and or to.
  • 01:53Uh.
  • 01:55To assign the correct treatment
  • 01:57to an individual,
  • 01:59so to make progress in this requires
  • 02:01more transdiagnostic clinical data.
  • 02:03We're collecting such data now.
  • 02:06And there's not a lot of
  • 02:09this data available just yet.
  • 02:11But the ultimate measure of success,
  • 02:12if we're going to categorize patients,
  • 02:14the ultimate measure of success is
  • 02:16do you assign the right patient to
  • 02:17the right treatment or the right
  • 02:19treatment to the right patient.
  • 02:21And that's going to take some time to get to.
  • 02:23So unlike maybe Simon and Sage,
  • 02:27I haven't given up on the potential
  • 02:29of this to someday be there.
  • 02:31And I don't think we have the answer
  • 02:33yet because we don't have all the data.
  • 02:36So we'll discuss this in the
  • 02:38context of the NIH research domain
  • 02:40criteria when we get to that point.
  • 02:42There's also brain objectives.
  • 02:44I'd like to understand the brain
  • 02:46a little bit better and I think
  • 02:48you know functional connectivity
  • 02:49and functional MRI in general are
  • 02:51potential avenues at understanding the brain.
  • 02:53So there's practical problems
  • 02:55of no definitions.
  • 02:56We saw some of that from Rudy's work and or
  • 02:59edge definitions as we just saw from Rick,
  • 03:02there's no consensus on which
  • 03:04Atlas in my mind,
  • 03:05there's no consensus.
  • 03:06I think you know a lot of groups
  • 03:08have their own consensus on
  • 03:09what what the ideal Atlas is,
  • 03:11but some of the results in
  • 03:13terms of these modeling methods
  • 03:15are actually Atlas dependent.
  • 03:17And Wendy here is has a poster
  • 03:20on consensus driven analysis.
  • 03:23So this says that rather than choose a a
  • 03:27random Atlas from your favorite group,
  • 03:30we we actually choose thousands
  • 03:34of random atlases.
  • 03:37That are then that we choose
  • 03:40thousands of C points and make
  • 03:43atlases out of each of those,
  • 03:45and then we find overlapping voxels
  • 03:48that correspond to behavior.
  • 03:51So what she's doing in this
  • 03:52work is actually using
  • 03:53the behavior. This is kind of.
  • 03:55Randy, you touched on this a little bit,
  • 03:56I'm going to come back to it at the end.
  • 03:58But this turns around the problem and
  • 04:00says we're going to be modeling behavior.
  • 04:02What's what are the voxels that are searching
  • 04:05with that behavior and not so much.
  • 04:06The nodes and their associated with
  • 04:08that behavior so kind of reverses.
  • 04:10It uses the behavior to
  • 04:12inform the choice of Atlas.
  • 04:14So if you want to learn more about this,
  • 04:15Wendy will be presenting
  • 04:17this tonight over drinks.
  • 04:20So there's a practical
  • 04:21problem of no definition.
  • 04:22I'm not going to talk about that.
  • 04:25We can also figure out what
  • 04:27data is most important.
  • 04:28And by data, again the earlier talks touched
  • 04:31on this data could mean brain state.
  • 04:33So if we talk about temporal
  • 04:36fluctuations or dynamics,
  • 04:37brain state could be important.
  • 04:39And this is some of our data.
  • 04:42There's 16 behavioral measures across
  • 04:44here collected under different activation
  • 04:47conditions and the red box is highlight
  • 04:51which which task runs best predicted.
  • 04:55Which behaviors?
  • 04:56And you can see that there's
  • 04:57no clear winner there,
  • 04:59although the movie condition does very well,
  • 05:02as does the eyes,
  • 05:04the emotional eyes task.
  • 05:06I used to think when we started on this
  • 05:09trajectory of green state manipulation
  • 05:11in order to build better models.
  • 05:14I used to think that this is
  • 05:15like a cardiac stress test.
  • 05:16So if you want to map attention,
  • 05:19you give an attention task and that
  • 05:22should help differentiate attention.
  • 05:23That doesn't seem to be panning out.
  • 05:26The attention task, for example,
  • 05:28doesn't necessarily give you
  • 05:29the best model for attention.
  • 05:31The other is that this is just
  • 05:33a synchronization,
  • 05:34so you're removing a lot of
  • 05:36temporal variance.
  • 05:37It's just adds minimum wage.
  • 05:40By synchronizing everybody,
  • 05:41you're kind of removing a lot of
  • 05:43variance and then you can get to the
  • 05:46intrinsic connectivity that you want.
  • 05:47But that's clearly not the case either,
  • 05:49because if it was just synchronization,
  • 05:51one of these tasks would win.
  • 05:53And I was thinking that the movies
  • 05:55are good at the synchronization,
  • 05:56but they should win on all
  • 05:58aspects there and they don't.
  • 05:59So it's not simply synchronization either.
  • 06:02And so this is a very large space
  • 06:03and we're still exploring this
  • 06:05and I'm not actually going to
  • 06:06talk about that today either.
  • 06:08Another type of data and and and
  • 06:10I think Sage touched on this
  • 06:13with you know we're expanding our
  • 06:15datasets and actually a lot of these
  • 06:17things can be compounds,
  • 06:18but we're getting better and better at
  • 06:20measuring things and we're learning
  • 06:21more and more about how these things
  • 06:24interact with the data that we have.
  • 06:25And so I think as we move forward
  • 06:28we'll start to understand these a
  • 06:30little bit better and then maybe
  • 06:32we will give up as Simon suggests.
  • 06:34Yeah, but one of the,
  • 06:36what I am going to talk about
  • 06:38today is can we learn more from the
  • 06:40connectome and I'll talk about that.
  • 06:42And where I'm going to really talk about
  • 06:44this is the sense of can we break up
  • 06:46the connectome into networks and and
  • 06:48Thomas who is left for a ski lesson.
  • 06:52Let's see how it has the yellow
  • 06:54networks that we all use and.
  • 06:56I told him he could watch
  • 06:58recording and says OK.
  • 07:00Yeah, but there are other ways
  • 07:02to break up the Alice and or the
  • 07:05connectome into networks, and I'm
  • 07:07going to talk about that a little bit.
  • 07:09And then there's this feed forward.
  • 07:11And I get this is where we
  • 07:12go back in any expression.
  • 07:13Again, there's this feed forward
  • 07:15feedback sort of thing where,
  • 07:17you know, we're using behavior to
  • 07:18learn something about the brain,
  • 07:20but we can also use the brain to
  • 07:22learn something about behavior.
  • 07:23And I'm going to talk about that.
  • 07:24So those are my 3 objectives
  • 07:27in the brief time that I have.
  • 07:30So the clinical applications,
  • 07:31how do we learn more about the
  • 07:34connectome and what do we learn about
  • 07:36behavior from this sort of modeling?
  • 07:38And so basically we all kind
  • 07:40of know the method or I think a
  • 07:42lot of people will methods.
  • 07:44So you start with a sample,
  • 07:45let's say 100 subjects,
  • 07:471000 subjects.
  • 07:48You have some behavioral test scores,
  • 07:50you have resting state or task data,
  • 07:52you build the connectome and then maybe
  • 07:54you specify some networks and you look
  • 07:57at those components of those networks.
  • 07:58But the whole idea, you know,
  • 08:00we talked about predictive performance a lot.
  • 08:02I mean the goal of this work, right,
  • 08:03is not to predict fluid intelligence
  • 08:05because you can do that with a test, right.
  • 08:07The goal of this is to use.
  • 08:09We have to verify that the circuits
  • 08:11you're identifying that various
  • 08:12function of that behavior are the
  • 08:14circuits that you're interested in.
  • 08:15And so that's the chief thing is to
  • 08:18identify these specific circuits,
  • 08:20develop normative models and the
  • 08:22Donders group is you know publishing
  • 08:25a bunch of stuff on this allow
  • 08:28identification of particular subtypes.
  • 08:30So we're particularly,
  • 08:30you know when it comes to mental illness,
  • 08:33we're really interested in what
  • 08:34if we can do brain behavior,
  • 08:35phenotyping and one of the things
  • 08:38that comes up and it came up.
  • 08:40Any questions is can you identify
  • 08:42other strategies?
  • 08:43So are people using different circuits,
  • 08:45there's an assumption here that
  • 08:46everybody is using the same circuits
  • 08:48and there are ways to kind of get
  • 08:50at that question I think and that's
  • 08:52related to the normative model.
  • 08:53And then count pounds as as Simon
  • 08:56already expressed and we can not
  • 08:58only are there countdowns that
  • 09:00screw up this modeling,
  • 09:02but we can identify through the brain
  • 09:04there are countdowns in the behavioral
  • 09:07studies that we're using as our gold.
  • 09:10Standards build build these models,
  • 09:12and in fact this is where the brain
  • 09:14can tell us about the behavior
  • 09:16or the behavioral models.
  • 09:18And this can therefore provide
  • 09:20insight into these systems,
  • 09:22the behavioral measures you're tapping.
  • 09:23So this is kind of the overarching frame.
  • 09:25So we built models.
  • 09:27I'm going to quickly go through how we
  • 09:30do the our connectome predictive model.
  • 09:32I think most people know this,
  • 09:34but this is very general audience.
  • 09:37So we have a connectivity
  • 09:38matrix from each individual,
  • 09:40we have a behavioral score.
  • 09:41From each individual,
  • 09:42we correlate those,
  • 09:44we find the edges that vary positively,
  • 09:46negatively.
  • 09:47Uh, with that behavior and that yields
  • 09:50the sort of network or set of nodes
  • 09:53or edges that vary or that support
  • 09:57performance and that behavioral measure.
  • 10:00And we don't stop there
  • 10:01because as Merrick showed up,
  • 10:03showed these,
  • 10:03this is a simple association study.
  • 10:06It's not reproducible.
  • 10:07But if we can build a
  • 10:09predictive model out of this,
  • 10:11then that tells us that it is predicted,
  • 10:13that it is meaningful and predictive
  • 10:15modeling tends to be much more.
  • 10:17Reliable then if if we
  • 10:19just stop at this point,
  • 10:20so how do we build a predictive model,
  • 10:22we take these edges,
  • 10:24we calculate some network score,
  • 10:27we build a model that fits the network
  • 10:29score to the observed performance
  • 10:31in a group of subjects and then we
  • 10:34test that in an individual that we
  • 10:37bring in and we can look at how well
  • 10:39we do and observed versus predicted.
  • 10:41And if we do really well then
  • 10:43we're happy with the model and
  • 10:44we can kind of cycle through.
  • 10:46We first did this with leave one out now.
  • 10:47We cabled it's best to do completely
  • 10:50independent samples if you have the data.
  • 10:52And you can do this with a positive
  • 10:54edges or the negative edges and
  • 10:56kind of doesn't matter whether
  • 10:57you do both or one or the other.
  • 10:59And then we can measure model performances,
  • 11:01mean square error or the R-squared
  • 11:05between the predicted and observed.
  • 11:08So this yields, you know,
  • 11:09these complex networks that
  • 11:11we're all interested in that tell
  • 11:14us something about the brain.
  • 11:16Most standard task based MRI people
  • 11:19hated these when these came out
  • 11:22because they're crazily complicated.
  • 11:24But the brain is kind of complicated.
  • 11:25So I don't actually have a problem
  • 11:28with our representation of the
  • 11:29brain being complicated.
  • 11:31And this is a small subset of the
  • 11:33number of available edges, right?
  • 11:34So this is a very small fraction
  • 11:36of the connections.
  • 11:37So the complexity likely reflects the
  • 11:39true complexity of the brain at the scale.
  • 11:42And to be honest, we're doing,
  • 11:44you know, in our predictive models.
  • 11:46Our meeting everybody,
  • 11:48not just us.
  • 11:50People that are doing this predictive
  • 11:52modeling are getting some of the best
  • 11:54pretty good models that are in the
  • 11:55field and you know most of the other
  • 11:58methods don't can't predict at this level.
  • 12:00So what's what I find fascinating,
  • 12:02really cool is that you know this
  • 12:04is this is a 268 by 268 matrix.
  • 12:06So that's what the actual
  • 12:08connectome looks like.
  • 12:09But it's really cool that
  • 12:11there's information on tons of
  • 12:12different things in here right.
  • 12:14And we're just trying to what we're
  • 12:16really doing is learning how to extract
  • 12:18that information and figure out what
  • 12:19this piece.
  • 12:20Connections tell us about the brain,
  • 12:22and so that's kind of fascinating.
  • 12:26What we've been doing is we've
  • 12:28been collecting data.
  • 12:29We have a behavioral battery that we
  • 12:31do outside the magnets about two hours.
  • 12:33We're doing a transdiagnostic sample.
  • 12:36We have a bunch of clinical scores as well,
  • 12:40which I don't have on here,
  • 12:43but I'm going to focus on these 16
  • 12:46behavioral measures and we can extract,
  • 12:48we can build models for each of
  • 12:50these measures and this is the
  • 12:52performance of those models.
  • 12:53So these are the 16 behavioral
  • 12:55measures and this is the.
  • 12:56R-squared performance of these are
  • 12:59our values starting on our squared the
  • 13:02R values of these across the sample,
  • 13:05and this is in a K fold analysis
  • 13:09and talking about count fans.
  • 13:12You know some of these things.
  • 13:13If you regress out certain compounds,
  • 13:15the model does better.
  • 13:16Sometimes you regress out things
  • 13:18in the model disappears,
  • 13:19but it's important to check
  • 13:21for various compounds.
  • 13:22But the point here is that the connectivity
  • 13:24matrix contains information on all of these.
  • 13:26Trades.
  • 13:27There's also,
  • 13:27in terms of dynamics that are state
  • 13:30based changes in the connectome as well,
  • 13:33which you can identify and
  • 13:35those are super interesting too.
  • 13:37So, oh, there's the clinical measures,
  • 13:39clinical tests we have as well.
  • 13:42So we can build models for all
  • 13:43of these things.
  • 13:44And then in Group analysis,
  • 13:45we can identify the circuits,
  • 13:47we can develop normative models
  • 13:48for what those circuits look like
  • 13:49as a function of score and that
  • 13:51may look something like this.
  • 13:52So we have that edges that are
  • 13:54involved and we have the performance
  • 13:56of the model or the relationship
  • 13:59between the behavioral score and the
  • 14:01network score and we can start to
  • 14:03look at who look for whom the model fails,
  • 14:06for example and.
  • 14:07Bringing individuals.
  • 14:09So I think in addition to the group data,
  • 14:11it's really interesting to look
  • 14:13at individuals and we have this
  • 14:15nature paper last summer where we
  • 14:17looked at the individuals outside
  • 14:18of these distributions and we show
  • 14:21that in fact those individuals
  • 14:22were failing the model,
  • 14:24not because of something to
  • 14:26do with those individuals,
  • 14:27but because the behavioral test was
  • 14:29biased against certain populations and
  • 14:31the brain was actually telling us that.
  • 14:33And I think that's actually really cool.
  • 14:35So there is a feedback feedforward
  • 14:37sort of loop.
  • 14:38Between what the our models tell
  • 14:40us about the brain and what they
  • 14:42also tell us about behavior.
  • 14:44And there hasn't really been a lot
  • 14:47of feedback on behavioral measures.
  • 14:48And I think that we have a an
  • 14:51opportunity here to contribute to the
  • 14:54knowledge about these behavioral tests.
  • 14:56So the open questions are what does
  • 14:58this tell us about behavioral measures
  • 15:00besides identifying biases and the measures?
  • 15:03I think Ruby showed the green and red dots,
  • 15:08the green dots.
  • 15:09They're from tests.
  • 15:09And the red dots,
  • 15:10you know the other way around,
  • 15:11the red dots are from tests and
  • 15:12the green dots were subjective,
  • 15:14you know self report and things.
  • 15:16There is a aspect of behavior
  • 15:18that you know for example,
  • 15:20we can't model anxiety.
  • 15:21We've tried you know dozens of
  • 15:22different measures of anxiety and
  • 15:24we don't get any performance,
  • 15:26we cannot seem to model anxiety in in
  • 15:28different populations and things like that.
  • 15:30And anxiety is kind of a subjective measure,
  • 15:33right, where it's not a test score
  • 15:35on a reaction time or performance
  • 15:37accuracy and it turns out that.
  • 15:39A lot of subjective measures are
  • 15:41self report measures are difficult
  • 15:42to model and what that means is they
  • 15:45don't have a direct brain for that.
  • 15:47So what you think about yourself
  • 15:49doesn't necessarily isn't necessarily
  • 15:51reflect reflected in your brain.
  • 15:53Which is a problem,
  • 15:55because psychiatry generally
  • 15:56relies on self report.
  • 15:58They don't rely on reaction times or,
  • 16:00you know, accuracy on some little quiz.
  • 16:04They rely on these sort of
  • 16:07subjective measures of brain state,
  • 16:09which it turns out we are poor at modeling,
  • 16:12but maybe in a sort of feedforward
  • 16:15feedback loop we can get better at the
  • 16:18questions we ask and improve that.
  • 16:20I mentioned that this doesn't
  • 16:22have to be resting state.
  • 16:23I already gave you the answer that,
  • 16:25you know, we can actually do better.
  • 16:27Ruby showed nice data,
  • 16:29but kind of for predictive power
  • 16:31because she's using resting state and
  • 16:34I think that if that was combined
  • 16:36with I know she went skiing too.
  • 16:38So I can say this.
  • 16:40That combined with task data might
  • 16:43actually do a lot better actually.
  • 16:45The other thing about Rudy's study is
  • 16:48that she's using HP behavioral measures.
  • 16:51They would like 58 behavioral measures,
  • 16:52but a lot of those measures are at
  • 16:54ceiling and then you need a good
  • 16:56distribution of behavior in order to
  • 16:58get good models and that's partly why
  • 17:00her performance measures were kind of low.
  • 17:03So the, again, the,
  • 17:04the type of data that we're relying
  • 17:06on to build our models is important,
  • 17:08but we can do, you know,
  • 17:10so I just want to throw in this thing.
  • 17:11We can do all these different tasks.
  • 17:14These tasks were designed to
  • 17:16tap different systems.
  • 17:17You know,
  • 17:18the emotional system.
  • 17:19Grad CPT is an intention task and
  • 17:22back is working memory tasks.
  • 17:24So these are designed to task,
  • 17:26to tap different components of cognition
  • 17:29and see how those components map on to
  • 17:33the different behavioral tests that we have.
  • 17:36And what was the result.
  • 17:37I showed you the beginning.
  • 17:38It's kind of all over the place.
  • 17:39So we're still unpacking this.
  • 17:42But the.
  • 17:45The cool thing is that.
  • 17:49We can take another step in this one.
  • 17:51I'm going to talk about now this
  • 17:53breaking the connectome model
  • 17:54into components and measure the
  • 17:57contribution of these components to
  • 17:59the system and so this is trying
  • 18:00to be responsive to NIH.
  • 18:04Desires for this sort of getting away
  • 18:06from the DSM until they get a more
  • 18:10biologically based diagnostic system.
  • 18:11We they introduced this rdoc
  • 18:13and this is actually 14
  • 18:15years ago or something they introduced this,
  • 18:17but we're now just getting to the
  • 18:19point where people have data on this
  • 18:21and can actually start testing this.
  • 18:22But the assumptions of our dog is that mental
  • 18:25illness is the circuit disorder problem.
  • 18:27Cognitive constructs should reflect
  • 18:29some of those disordered circuits.
  • 18:31And so if we can identify these
  • 18:33disordered circuits, the circuit with.
  • 18:34Commission maybe that is going to tell us
  • 18:37something about what's wrong in the brains
  • 18:40of people suffering from mental illness.
  • 18:42And they the aspect here,
  • 18:44the assumption here is that is this
  • 18:46trans diagnostic assumption that
  • 18:47we're all on the spectrum, right?
  • 18:49So you know,
  • 18:50we all have some level of paranoia and
  • 18:53we all have any symptom that you have.
  • 18:56You know,
  • 18:57there's somebody along the spectrum there
  • 18:59and the idea is that you can look at a
  • 19:03transdiagnostic population and extract.
  • 19:05Uh, components that go
  • 19:07across everybody and that.
  • 19:09I find that kind of an attractive theory.
  • 19:11So they define the they got a bunch
  • 19:13of psychologists together and they
  • 19:16defined these six cognitive constructs,
  • 19:18attention, perception, memory, language,
  • 19:20working memory and cognitive control.
  • 19:23And what we did was we went and
  • 19:26built networks for each of these,
  • 19:29and these networks then become
  • 19:31components in our predictive modeling.
  • 19:34And so we can.
  • 19:36Specifically look then at what these
  • 19:39components contribute to each task.
  • 19:42So,
  • 19:43so far we've been to,
  • 19:44we've considered this data-driven
  • 19:45approach where we take the whole
  • 19:47connectome and we build our models.
  • 19:49What we're going to do now is going
  • 19:50to break up these connections.
  • 19:51We're going to take components of them and
  • 19:53rather than just getting a single slope here,
  • 19:56we're going to build a model that
  • 19:58looks at multiple components.
  • 19:59And so here we can look then at the
  • 20:01contribution of each of these or we
  • 20:02could do this separately where we
  • 20:04just take each of these components.
  • 20:05Individually and build a model like that.
  • 20:08And so we've done that and it's kind
  • 20:11of cool that you can actually look at.
  • 20:14So if you look at the memory,
  • 20:15working memory construct,
  • 20:16you can then look at, well,
  • 20:19what's the,
  • 20:20how,
  • 20:20what's the contribution of that
  • 20:22network to this behavior.
  • 20:24And and across these 16 behaviors,
  • 20:26we can rank them according to how
  • 20:28much they rely on the working
  • 20:30memory system for performance.
  • 20:32And I don't think this has really
  • 20:34been done in evaluating these
  • 20:36behavioral measures previously.
  • 20:38And so we can get this rank and
  • 20:40there are basic assumptions around
  • 20:41what these things are measuring.
  • 20:43But this is now a way for us to test if
  • 20:46in fact those basic assumptions are true.
  • 20:48And if you really want to task that
  • 20:50it's going to have working memory,
  • 20:51you would look at the verbal fluency 2 test,
  • 20:54I guess in this case.
  • 20:55And we can repeat this for each
  • 20:58of the constructs.
  • 20:59And so I'm emphasizing again the
  • 21:01speed forward feedforward thing is
  • 21:03that we can identify the circuitry,
  • 21:05the feedback, so in in a population,
  • 21:07let's say feedback.
  • 21:08Thing is, we could be a priority,
  • 21:10define the system and then measure
  • 21:13the components of that system.
  • 21:16And so we've done this for the
  • 21:19language that kind of control
  • 21:21the working memory circuits and
  • 21:23we've done it for the other three.
  • 21:25The attention,
  • 21:26the perception of declarative
  • 21:27memory and the rankings on along
  • 21:30the Y axis here change with
  • 21:31each of these constructs and we
  • 21:33can look at the distribution.
  • 21:37And the reason I was telling
  • 21:39Thomas he shouldn't go skiing is
  • 21:41because I wanted to throw this in.
  • 21:43But there's the seven canonical yo networks
  • 21:46and you can define it this way as well.
  • 21:50You can look at the contribution
  • 21:52of each component of that network
  • 21:54and get the same sort of results.
  • 21:57So if we do this in terms
  • 21:59of a normative framework,
  • 22:01we have a distribution of scores,
  • 22:03we get a model coefficients for each term.
  • 22:06So we get distributions of those
  • 22:07and we can get distributions that
  • 22:09we have single components and
  • 22:11distributions of the typical network
  • 22:13associated with each behavioral score.
  • 22:15And then we can bring in a new
  • 22:17subject in contrast to their score.
  • 22:19And if we have this these sorts
  • 22:21of normative models,
  • 22:22we can see potentially are they
  • 22:24who are the same performance,
  • 22:26they may be enhancing working.
  • 22:28OK.
  • 22:28They may be relying on working memory
  • 22:30more to achieve that performance and
  • 22:32that's something that we can start
  • 22:34to break up with this approach.
  • 22:35And so here's the summary of
  • 22:37the whole system.
  • 22:38So we can build the system,
  • 22:39we have the circuits,
  • 22:40we can divide,
  • 22:41we can get innovative models
  • 22:43for each behavior,
  • 22:45we can see who's outside of that framework,
  • 22:47and we have the predicted and observed.
  • 22:51We can do this for all of
  • 22:53the behavioral measures.
  • 22:53So this is really a high dimensional
  • 22:55space that we have 16 measures or more.
  • 22:58Actually Rudy's talk there were 58 measures.
  • 23:01So it's a high dimensional
  • 23:02space that we can look at,
  • 23:04but we can bring all this data
  • 23:06together and look at these components.
  • 23:08So we're currently increasing
  • 23:10our sample size where we're
  • 23:12finding these network definitions.
  • 23:14So we defined like the working
  • 23:17memory definition network we
  • 23:19define using neurosystem.
  • 23:21And there are other ways obviously of
  • 23:24defining these networks and I think
  • 23:26there's going to be some work around
  • 23:28that how to segregate these networks,
  • 23:31but getting.
  • 23:32Getting back to Randy's question.
  • 23:37This slide.
  • 23:39It was ready.
  • 23:41Getting back to Randy's question,
  • 23:45there's a couple of ways you could do this.
  • 23:47So you could start with these 16 networks.
  • 23:49I mean they they're 6, they're not.
  • 23:51Let's say that was sixteen.
  • 23:53We could actually,
  • 23:54we're starting to do this where we pull up,
  • 23:57we look at the,
  • 23:58you know,
  • 23:58maybe there's six principal
  • 23:59components in these networks that
  • 24:01we can reverse engineer and we can
  • 24:04define these networks in a different
  • 24:06way and then see if those networks
  • 24:08outperform the systems and there's
  • 24:10yet another way that we could.
  • 24:12Just this around which is the whole
  • 24:14you know in psychology and ontology
  • 24:16is the concept of working memory
  • 24:19and attention and things like that.
  • 24:22Those are ontologies that we use
  • 24:23in psychology that define features.
  • 24:25But you can do a data-driven sort
  • 24:28of analysis where you define the
  • 24:30way you define the components of
  • 24:32cognition that span some behavioral
  • 24:34space and then build models across
  • 24:37that and see if those models
  • 24:39do better than the
  • 24:40the classical ontologies.
  • 24:42Working memory and attention
  • 24:43and stuff like that.
  • 24:44So that's kind of where
  • 24:45we're going with this.
  • 24:46It may all be, you know, compounds,
  • 24:48but I think this is potentially
  • 24:51interesting and it it kind of uses.
  • 24:54The data and keep forward feedback
  • 24:56networks where the behavior
  • 24:57drives some of the imaging data
  • 24:59and the imaging data can drive
  • 25:01the behavior and I think that's
  • 25:03a super useful approach to to do.
  • 25:06So. I'm going to wrap up,
  • 25:09we're interested in this clinical objective.
  • 25:11We want to learn more about the
  • 25:13connectome and how it relates to
  • 25:15behavior and but the connectome
  • 25:16can also tell us about behavior
  • 25:18and I think that's really powerful
  • 25:20and really kind of exciting.
  • 25:22So thank you.
  • 25:30We'll start with Bandy first bandy.
  • 25:34I'd like to read it all up. Publication.
  • 25:47My question.
  • 25:51Yes. Yeah. Subject.
  • 25:58Yeah, on both sides of the intersection.
  • 26:03Yeah. No, we're very interested in this.
  • 26:05And you know that was a creep representation,
  • 26:08OK, but we are doing that that sort of thing.
  • 26:12Todd was next and then I'll go to Julia and.
  • 26:17It was bizarre to me the way you
  • 26:20came up with the networks for
  • 26:22those cognitive funds drugs.
  • 26:24Do you think neuroscientists bizarre?
  • 26:27Just because it's, you know,
  • 26:30looking at you could walk,
  • 26:31we do analyze the last base
  • 26:34and all of those tasks that
  • 26:37are mentioned there already,
  • 26:38you can actually analyze those tasks
  • 26:40and they're summarized in neuroscience.
  • 26:45No. Yeah. Yeah. Yes. So that's true.
  • 26:47Networks that you know,
  • 26:48turns out that I know that this is not in
  • 26:52any way except for the popular opinion.
  • 26:54But it turns out that there is a
  • 26:56set of networks that I said many
  • 26:58times here before that spans pretty
  • 26:59much all past that are available.
  • 27:01So if you take,
  • 27:02you know all the working memory,
  • 27:04massive publicly available
  • 27:05or shared between people,
  • 27:07cognitive control tasks, attention pass,
  • 27:10whatever other types of constructs,
  • 27:12you're fine.
  • 27:15Across those paths and then a,
  • 27:18you have the networks and B,
  • 27:20that cognitive function that may not
  • 27:22match the ones that have been decreased
  • 27:25by cognitive psychology or clinical
  • 27:27nurse oncology to us might start to
  • 27:29emerge by observing the way those networks.
  • 27:31So yeah,
  • 27:32I agree fully and and in my defense,
  • 27:35let me say that we're super excited
  • 27:37about the thinking about bringing
  • 27:38this way and I think there's a lot
  • 27:40of details still to be worked out on
  • 27:42how we define these networks and.
  • 27:44You know,
  • 27:44these six cognitive constructs
  • 27:46are the ones the NIH came up with.
  • 27:48You know, that may not also be the best,
  • 27:50but the whole data-driven approach
  • 27:52to it I think may actually be better.
  • 27:54But Julie was next.
  • 27:56Now, but I was just wondering,
  • 27:59you said that you know these big
  • 28:02contracts with disabilities and
  • 28:03I could argue that pension is.
  • 28:07Everywhere.
  • 28:10Yeah.
  • 28:13Yeah. Well, that's why you do the
  • 28:17data-driven approach and maybe we
  • 28:19can discover better ways of dividing
  • 28:21that data rather than relying on
  • 28:24these kind of old formalisms, right,
  • 28:26that that if we can do better in
  • 28:28terms of predictive models and then
  • 28:30feed forward feedback then then I
  • 28:32would say that's a good argument for
  • 28:34using a different circle anthology.
  • 28:38Sorry, you were next.
  • 28:42OK, so.
  • 28:45So I I guess kind of related to when
  • 28:49this poster if it would take too long.
  • 28:53Like you can define the passports
  • 28:56with behavior. It's like,
  • 28:58well I guess it's kind of running to do that,
  • 29:00but like if you define the networks with
  • 29:03the behavior and let's say that all,
  • 29:05I predict my behavior better even
  • 29:07though it is like cross medication,
  • 29:09wouldn't that be kind of silly? I see.
  • 29:12So the question was if you're at the
  • 29:14beginning I I mentioned helping the
  • 29:17behavior to define a parcellation so
  • 29:19we don't have to rely on a particular
  • 29:21Atlas and so the question is,
  • 29:23is that circular?
  • 29:24I don't think it's that circular if
  • 29:27they're an independent populations,
  • 29:29but we are and I think actually what
  • 29:32we're getting at there is that,
  • 29:34so I I'm a I know this is maybe heresy
  • 29:37and some of the some of the items here,
  • 29:40But I'm not as strong.
  • 29:42Believer in uh,
  • 29:43an aerial area that you know doesn't
  • 29:45function and has, you know,
  • 29:47clearly defined by the site of architecture.
  • 29:50I, you know,
  • 29:51there's tons of evidence that these
  • 29:54functional regions engineering
  • 29:55spoke about this a little bit,
  • 29:58that these functional regions,
  • 29:59you know,
  • 30:00changing more and stuff depending
  • 30:01on brain state and their tasks.
  • 30:03And one of the cool things about
  • 30:06what Wendy's doing is that when
  • 30:08you when you define these nodes in
  • 30:10this kind of Atlas free framework.
  • 30:12Um,
  • 30:12you'll get overlapping nodes and and Thomas.
  • 30:15You'll have this in an ontology
  • 30:17paperback and cerebral cortex where
  • 30:20he showed overlapping brain regions
  • 30:23responsible for different tasks or
  • 30:25essentially different behaviors.
  • 30:27As to the circularity,
  • 30:28I don't know whether people think
  • 30:30if you define this in a in a
  • 30:32separate group do is that circular
  • 30:34is a completely circular?
  • 30:35I think the the brain is the
  • 30:38behavior is helping us zero in on
  • 30:42the voxels that are consistent.
  • 30:45And and then we choose those
  • 30:47voxels and I think that's,
  • 30:49I think that's legit.
  • 30:52Yes, David. I would have a question.
  • 30:58Basically we use.
  • 31:0330.
  • 31:06Stop so.
  • 31:09What is the difference?
  • 31:12These things. Well, again, it's,
  • 31:15you know, these things are.
  • 31:16So yeah which you know how
  • 31:18does what's the physiological
  • 31:20manifestation of depression?
  • 31:22I mean I think the idea is the
  • 31:24assumption is that that there's
  • 31:25disordered brain circuits and they
  • 31:27may be reflected in somebody's
  • 31:28behavioral measures that we've seen.
  • 31:30So it's not necessarily saying
  • 31:33here's the depression circuit,
  • 31:34but here's a disrupted circuit and
  • 31:36maybe that's a target for therapy or
  • 31:38something like that or maybe that's
  • 31:40a a metric for measuring response
  • 31:41to therapy and biological basis for.
  • 31:45That makes sense.
  • 31:48Ohh, there's a I OK, I gotta read.
  • 31:51There's an online question,
  • 31:52then I did somebody else have a
  • 31:54question while I look that up.
  • 31:56Ring of going back to the six domains
  • 31:58if you saw that it wasn't a paper,
  • 32:00but it was like a working group thing
  • 32:04from 2014, about 6 different domains.
  • 32:09Yes. How much reliability has been how,
  • 32:12what is the state of reliability and
  • 32:14validity of these? Oh, yeah. Yeah.
  • 32:15So summarize all of them. Yeah.
  • 32:18Well, we're actually looking into that.
  • 32:19I have some stuff to bring on that right now.
  • 32:22You know the we're using the
  • 32:23behavioral measures is like a gold
  • 32:25standard and we're building our
  • 32:26models on these behavioral measures.
  • 32:28But others have already published
  • 32:30papers showing that really reliability
  • 32:32of many of these measures is poor.
  • 32:34Again, that's a fee for feedback thing.
  • 32:36I mean if you put garbage in,
  • 32:37you're not going to get model and
  • 32:39I think some of the variations.
  • 32:41You see and the ability to model things
  • 32:43might be directly related to that.
  • 32:44So yeah, we can't, you know,
  • 32:47we don't have any other,
  • 32:48nothing else to do, right.
  • 32:50I mean we have to use what we can,
  • 32:52but I think we need to be aware of
  • 32:54that reliability of those things that.
  • 32:56Ohh, there's online question here.
  • 32:58Can you guys read that?
  • 32:59But doesn't show up for you, OK?
  • 33:03I am Brandon from Beck Todd's lab at UBC.
  • 33:07What might be the benefits,
  • 33:08disadvantages of evaluating the
  • 33:09contributions of networks and normative
  • 33:11distributions during different cognitive
  • 33:13tasks compared to looking at normative
  • 33:15versus normative representations,
  • 33:16emotionally relevant stimuli and specific
  • 33:18regions of interest during these tests?
  • 33:20Well, looking at specific
  • 33:21regions of interest during,
  • 33:23you know,
  • 33:24tasks doesn't seem to be
  • 33:26very predictive of stuff.
  • 33:27I mean that's the bottom line.
  • 33:29You kind of have to these,
  • 33:32as I said at the beginning.
  • 33:34These kind of connection based models
  • 33:36tend to be much more predictive
  • 33:38of performance or or traits than
  • 33:41F MRI based activation as I think
  • 33:43that's kind of what the question is.
  • 33:49Any other comments? One more so.
  • 33:53People confuse on the brain.
  • 33:57Is that like there's obviously
  • 33:59like variation from college trial,
  • 34:00like investigators reaction time like?
  • 34:03You know, sometimes it's easier, sure.
  • 34:07And then there's obviously some
  • 34:09sort of average dynamics, you know,
  • 34:11and like how could these people like
  • 34:13because the input change the connectivity,
  • 34:15so like yes, yes and no.
  • 34:18So when you're speaking
  • 34:19your face is changing,
  • 34:21but I can still recognize you.
  • 34:23And I think, you know when we're doing stuff,
  • 34:26our connectome is there.
  • 34:27It changes like a little bit.
  • 34:30I think in our original fingerprinting
  • 34:31paper we showed that we could still
  • 34:33identify people by their connectome
  • 34:35under different task conditions.
  • 34:37See you tweak the connect them a little bit,
  • 34:39but you don't change it completely
  • 34:41and you still look the same.
  • 34:42And so that's the whole state
  • 34:44versus trade thing there.
  • 34:46You can do state manipulation and change
  • 34:48various edges and in the connectome,
  • 34:50but you still mostly look the same
  • 34:53and hopefully you're getting trades
  • 34:54out and that's we're advocates for
  • 34:56like 20-40 minutes of data so that you
  • 34:59actually average out I think a lot of
  • 35:02those moments and moment fluctuations,
  • 35:03but I think Joanna is going
  • 35:06to talk about dynamics.
  • 35:07And there's tons of stuff going
  • 35:09on at the moment to moment level
  • 35:11and you can look for that and you
  • 35:13can get things out of that.
  • 35:15That's not to say that there's
  • 35:17not a grand average that we're
  • 35:18dealing with kind of thing so but
  • 35:20does that answer your question?
  • 35:22I'm just confused how you do.
  • 35:25Well, they're both there.
  • 35:26We're ignoring the the moment to
  • 35:27moment fluctuations and we're looking
  • 35:29at the ensemble averages of activity.
  • 35:31So if you wanted to look at moment
  • 35:33to moment fluctuations,
  • 35:35you could some of this still works.
  • 35:38It gets noisier,
  • 35:39you know,
  • 35:39the less data you have,
  • 35:40the more noisy it gets.
  • 35:43But you know,
  • 35:44these things still hold for the most part.
  • 35:48OK.
  • 35:48So I'd like to thank all three
  • 35:50speakers for this session.
  • 35:52Thank you.
  • 35:57And we have.