Skip to Main Content

3/23/22 – Dr. Jeffrey Gruen –The New Yale Program for Learning Disabilities Research

March 24, 2022
  • 00:00Spring.
  • 00:05We have a few announcements,
  • 00:06upcoming grand rounds next week,
  • 00:09March 30th is one of our.
  • 00:11A special lecturers named lecture
  • 00:14and part of our Centennial series,
  • 00:17the C DAF Cook Lectureship.
  • 00:19Those of you who don't remember
  • 00:21that Cook was a former chair of
  • 00:24the Department of Pediatrics.
  • 00:27And Kinari Webb,
  • 00:28who's in family medicine and
  • 00:29founder of health and harmony,
  • 00:31will be speaking on healing
  • 00:33the planet by recognizing the
  • 00:36fundamental sickness of colonialism.
  • 00:38Uh.
  • 00:40Then on April 6th we're going
  • 00:42to be having care rounds,
  • 00:43which are conversations and
  • 00:46reflections experiences in the
  • 00:48Children's Hospital and look forward
  • 00:51to that interactive grand rounds.
  • 00:57Another announcement coming up on
  • 00:59this Monday or monthly pediatric
  • 01:02educational learning community
  • 01:03series will be led by Galloping
  • 01:06Me Soma and Gunjan Tiagra,
  • 01:09both from the section of emergency
  • 01:11medicine on introduction to
  • 01:12qualitative research methods,
  • 01:14exploring factors that influence
  • 01:16the choice of academic pediatricians
  • 01:18by underrepresented minorities.
  • 01:25And also Joe reminder.
  • 01:27Another one of our major
  • 01:29events for a Centennial.
  • 01:30Is the Richard Ehrenkrantz Neonatology
  • 01:33Symposium and this year's topic
  • 01:35is ethical issues in neonatology?
  • 01:37This will be taking place virtually Tuesday,
  • 01:40April 5th from 1:00 to 5:30.
  • 01:44There will be a incredible
  • 01:47lineup of speakers and talking
  • 01:50about these important topics in
  • 01:52ethics related to neonatology,
  • 01:54so please join us.
  • 01:59There are no conflicts and no contact,
  • 02:02no disclosures that need to be made.
  • 02:06This is accredited for see me
  • 02:09by the Yale School of Medicine.
  • 02:11There's the number.
  • 02:12The text to the CME office
  • 02:15and will also put this in the
  • 02:18zoom chat at various intervals
  • 02:20throughout the grand rounds.
  • 02:25And with that I'm going to
  • 02:28turn it over to Mark Mercurio,
  • 02:30to introduce today's speaker.
  • 02:38Thank you Cliff.
  • 02:39It's an honor to introduce my
  • 02:41friend and colleague Jeff Grouin.
  • 02:42Today I'm gonna just share my
  • 02:44screen for a moment if I could.
  • 02:49Let's go with this and share and
  • 02:52are you looking at my slides now?
  • 02:54So this this of course is doctor Jeff Broin
  • 02:57who is known to you all as a professor
  • 02:59of Pediatrics and genetics here at Yale.
  • 03:01He's certainly one of the pillars of of
  • 03:05Yale and neonatology, Yale Pediatrics,
  • 03:08and Yale Science, but of course he
  • 03:10didn't start out as such a big cheese.
  • 03:13He was got his BS and MD at Tulane,
  • 03:16came to Yale where he did his residency
  • 03:18and fellowship and Jeff and I traveled
  • 03:20this journey together ever since.
  • 03:22And so it's been my absolute pleasure
  • 03:24to watch him rise through the ranks.
  • 03:26So let's see if we can get this slide
  • 03:29to advance and apparently we can't.
  • 03:31Let's try that.
  • 03:32So here we go back a little bit
  • 03:34in time and we see this here.
  • 03:35I don't know if you can see my cursor.
  • 03:36We see the second from the left.
  • 03:37Standing up is a young Jeff grew in with his
  • 03:40dog with his wife Susan here and Michelle.
  • 03:42I see everybody.
  • 03:43We had a lot of babies were
  • 03:44born in Pediatrics that year.
  • 03:45This, I think would be the spring
  • 03:47of 85 in my backyard in Hamden.
  • 03:50This is a young Luciano Pavarotti
  • 03:51you see over here at the time.
  • 03:53And this is and and again, Michelle.
  • 03:56Jeff's daughter was not in this picture,
  • 03:58but I've had the the pleasure
  • 04:00of watching Doctor Gruen.
  • 04:02Just rise through the ranks over the
  • 04:04years and grow gradually to be the the
  • 04:07fellow that you all know and love today.
  • 04:10He became a professor of medicine.
  • 04:12Excuse me of Pediatrics.
  • 04:13In Unix in 2010 he's also an
  • 04:16honorary professor at Shenzhen
  • 04:18Tong University in China.
  • 04:20He's internationally recognized as a
  • 04:22scholar and investigator with collaborators
  • 04:24around the collaborators around the world.
  • 04:27He's very, very well published.
  • 04:29He's been very well funded for
  • 04:30a number of years, many,
  • 04:31many years, and at Yale,
  • 04:33he's really a valued mentor and advisor
  • 04:36to medical students to graduate students,
  • 04:38to, to our trainees, to our junior.
  • 04:40Faculty and, and frankly,
  • 04:42to at least one very grateful Chief for
  • 04:45whom he's been a valued advisor for many,
  • 04:48many years.
  • 04:48So it's been my pleasure to watch Jeff
  • 04:51really become such an incredibly,
  • 04:53highly respected scholar and
  • 04:55investigator that he is.
  • 04:56You know,
  • 04:57he studies genetic variants that
  • 04:58underlie common learning disabilities,
  • 05:00and you're going to hear a bit
  • 05:01about that today.
  • 05:02And as such,
  • 05:03the talk should really be of interest
  • 05:04to anybody who's interested in
  • 05:06genetics or studies genetics or
  • 05:07who wants to learn more about it,
  • 05:09or anybody who's who works with
  • 05:11kids with learning disabilities or
  • 05:13wants to learn more about that.
  • 05:14But there's the lecture will also
  • 05:16be of interest to anybody who wants
  • 05:18to be a better teacher,
  • 05:19because as most of you know,
  • 05:21and many of you know,
  • 05:22and you're all about to find out,
  • 05:24our friend Jeff can teach.
  • 05:26He can take difficult subjects
  • 05:27and make them accessible to
  • 05:29to the point where even a bioethicist can
  • 05:32understand he's an extremely gifted teacher,
  • 05:34and he's also, by the way,
  • 05:35a very gifted neonatologist.
  • 05:37He is the triple threat that
  • 05:39people talk about on academics,
  • 05:41and it's really been my
  • 05:42pleasure and honor to.
  • 05:43Have this journey with Jeff for
  • 05:45the last many years.
  • 05:46As you can see from that picture,
  • 05:47you know I think Cliff is still celebrating.
  • 05:49We're all still celebrating 100 years
  • 05:51of Yale Pediatrics and so and so they
  • 05:54wanted a couple of guys today who
  • 05:56actually were here 100 years ago at
  • 05:58Yale Pediatrics and so here we are.
  • 06:01It's an absolute pleasure
  • 06:02to introduce my friend,
  • 06:04Jeff Gruen.
  • 06:14You know if you ever want an emotional
  • 06:18experience, be introduced by somebody
  • 06:20who's been really your friend,
  • 06:22not just a colleague for 35 years, it's.
  • 06:26You can really get choked up about
  • 06:27this stuff. Thank you for doing that.
  • 06:29Right before I have to give a talk,
  • 06:30I really appreciate that Mark. Thank you.
  • 06:34Glad to help, Jeff, thank you.
  • 06:38So I'm going to talk a little
  • 06:40bit about our new program for
  • 06:43learning disabilities research.
  • 06:44I'm going to basically answer the
  • 06:47five basic questions, why, what,
  • 06:49how, where and who very simply.
  • 06:52Why is early identification of
  • 06:54learning disabilities important?
  • 06:56What is the scope of the problem?
  • 06:57How will early identification disrupt and
  • 07:01change the current educational paradigm?
  • 07:03Where can we make a difference
  • 07:05and who are we?
  • 07:07So let's get started and with all our
  • 07:10research we start in the classroom.
  • 07:12And that's because worldwide,
  • 07:14about 15% of students everywhere
  • 07:16struggle with learning disabilities.
  • 07:18These include things you've
  • 07:20heard of and things you haven't.
  • 07:22Most common is dyslexia,
  • 07:24also known as reading disability,
  • 07:26specific language impairment,
  • 07:27which is delayed onset of speech,
  • 07:30verbal trait disorder,
  • 07:31which is impairment of articulation,
  • 07:34dyscalculia,
  • 07:34a math disability and attention disorder,
  • 07:38is better known to most folks.
  • 07:39Is ADHD and ADD dot.
  • 07:42But what I'm talking about
  • 07:43today and every and all the kids
  • 07:45that I'll be talking studies,
  • 07:46I'll be talking about today are students
  • 07:49in regular classes at regular schools.
  • 07:52And with normal or above normal intelligence.
  • 07:56For most children, a durable
  • 07:58intervention can make a huge difference,
  • 08:01and I'm going to show you that
  • 08:02data in a in a few minutes,
  • 08:04but in the meantime there are three
  • 08:06critical things to know about reading.
  • 08:09The first is that dyslexia or
  • 08:11reading disability is really common,
  • 08:14so if I take the 2000 fourth
  • 08:16graders here in New Haven,
  • 08:18and I take them,
  • 08:19I give them any standardized
  • 08:20reading performance assessment.
  • 08:22Let's say comprehension or single
  • 08:24word reading, and we.
  • 08:26We graph them the number of
  • 08:28readers at each point,
  • 08:29low scoring versus high scoring.
  • 08:31Not surprisingly,
  • 08:32you'll get a normal distribution
  • 08:33if you have enough students with
  • 08:35a mean and what we call reading
  • 08:38disability is basically a cut off.
  • 08:39It's a cut off at the tail end.
  • 08:41It's a lower end,
  • 08:42so most places do around the 10th percentile.
  • 08:45Some places do 15, some do 12 percentile.
  • 08:48But in the United States we're talking
  • 08:50about 5 to 10 million US schoolchildren.
  • 08:53Again, these are in regular kids.
  • 08:54These are regular kids in regular schools.
  • 08:58The second thing to know is
  • 09:00that it's genetic, and so again,
  • 09:01if we take our distribution of children,
  • 09:04let's say again 4th graders and we do
  • 09:06say comprehension and we chart them.
  • 09:08So we have a normal distribution.
  • 09:10Not only will it have a mean,
  • 09:11but it'll be a variance around
  • 09:13the mean as well.
  • 09:14And if we ask the factors,
  • 09:15ask which factors can account for that
  • 09:18variance and in fact non genetic factors.
  • 09:21To the surprise of most people actually
  • 09:23account for a very small percent,
  • 09:25probably in the 20 to 30% range.
  • 09:27Ras cumulative genetic factors
  • 09:29account for the vast majority of
  • 09:32reading performance or the variance
  • 09:34around the mean for all of us,
  • 09:36and that's why it's really important
  • 09:37to pick our parents carefully,
  • 09:39because this is what really is
  • 09:41important for our performance
  • 09:42in reading.
  • 09:45So what we say is that genetic factors
  • 09:47account for up to 80% of reading performance,
  • 09:50and we call this concept heritability.
  • 09:52And since genetic factors are mostly
  • 09:55responsible for dyslexia and our
  • 09:57performance and meeting our past
  • 10:00was to discover reading genes.
  • 10:02And so to identify these kids at
  • 10:04risk when intervention works best,
  • 10:06we developed a genetic screening panel.
  • 10:09We collect saliva from saliva.
  • 10:11We extract genomic DNA and we've
  • 10:13done these studies in two forms.
  • 10:15Our first one was about 10 years ago.
  • 10:17Was the grad study of the genes
  • 10:19reading and dyslexia study.
  • 10:20The first study of its kind of a
  • 10:22strictly African American Hispanic
  • 10:24American kids in the United States
  • 10:26using genetics and then more recently
  • 10:28the longitudinal program called
  • 10:30the New Haven Lectionum project.
  • 10:32And these have been informative for kids
  • 10:35that are European American background,
  • 10:37Hispanic American background
  • 10:39and African American background.
  • 10:41And we published these in in
  • 10:43in in peer review journals.
  • 10:45Some pretty high quality peer review
  • 10:47journals and that in a very nice very
  • 10:50nicely that many of them are reproduced
  • 10:52sometimes in other populations,
  • 10:54even in other languages.
  • 10:57So the third thing to know
  • 11:00about reading is that.
  • 11:02Is that dyslexia can be effectively treated,
  • 11:04and so let me share with you data
  • 11:07from my colleague Maureen Lovett
  • 11:09at the University of Toronto and
  • 11:12State Kit and Maureen study.
  • 11:15Does randomized control trials and
  • 11:17kids who struggle with reading and she
  • 11:19does a very intense intervention that
  • 11:21lasts for about a year and what she's
  • 11:24done is she's retested them two years
  • 11:26after completion of the intervention.
  • 11:28So these are outcomes 2 years after
  • 11:30completion of an intervention for
  • 11:32children that were picked up in grade.
  • 11:34One children in grade two children
  • 11:36in grade 3/4, etc.
  • 11:38All the way out to grade 12 and what
  • 11:41you can see here pretty obviously is
  • 11:43that these are the kids that perform
  • 11:45the best that are most responsive.
  • 11:47So about 75% of these kids will
  • 11:50respond to intervention and will be
  • 11:52reading at grade level two years
  • 11:54after completion of the intervention.
  • 11:57Whereas in contrast,
  • 11:57if you look at kids that are
  • 11:59picked up in high school,
  • 12:00which is about 50% or so of kids in
  • 12:02the United States are picked up.
  • 12:04Was reading disability in high school
  • 12:07or later only about 1/4 of those
  • 12:09kids will actually be respond will
  • 12:11be reading it ever brought up to
  • 12:13grade level or be reading at grade
  • 12:16level even two years afterwards,
  • 12:17so early identification is
  • 12:19obviously really important.
  • 12:20That's the key.
  • 12:22The key to effective intervention
  • 12:24is early identification,
  • 12:26but the window of opportunity is narrow.
  • 12:29So let me show you why.
  • 12:31Here's our current approach in
  • 12:32our educational system today,
  • 12:34and that's in private schools,
  • 12:35public schools everywhere we
  • 12:36call it the wait to fail model.
  • 12:38You'll be very familiar with this,
  • 12:40and so this would be typical development of,
  • 12:42say, comprehension.
  • 12:43But again, it could be single word reading,
  • 12:45could be spelling of kids of
  • 12:47typical children,
  • 12:48the United States and around the world.
  • 12:50Frankly, in a pre K all the way up through
  • 12:52grade five you can see it's nearly linear.
  • 12:54The increase in the performance
  • 12:56from year to year.
  • 12:58But there are children who begin to
  • 13:01fall off once they are come face to
  • 13:03face with a challenging curriculum,
  • 13:05so this will usually happen
  • 13:06at the end of grade one,
  • 13:07definitely in grade 2 where
  • 13:09they really begin to fall off.
  • 13:11In the current model,
  • 13:12what we do is we wait until
  • 13:14they're really performing poorly,
  • 13:16and then we do a standard intervention.
  • 13:18And even if the intervention
  • 13:20is intense and even if they're
  • 13:21young enough to respond to it,
  • 13:23they never really catch up,
  • 13:25and so this gap is never really bridged.
  • 13:28What we're proposing is a different model.
  • 13:30What we'd like to do is identify the
  • 13:33kids here early and then track them,
  • 13:35and then when they even fall off,
  • 13:37or even even just mildly,
  • 13:40then we can initiate an early intervention,
  • 13:43and which they would catch up,
  • 13:45or at least attain very nearly
  • 13:46Gray level reading.
  • 13:47And that is then sustainable.
  • 13:49But the key here is the early identification.
  • 13:54So in in order to do that, that's where.
  • 13:57The the identification of genes,
  • 13:59genetic variants and creation of a
  • 14:02genetic screening panel is so important.
  • 14:04So how do we do this?
  • 14:05How do we do this magic of this
  • 14:08early identification?
  • 14:08And so as an example for a minute,
  • 14:10let me just talk to you about
  • 14:12some of the work we've been doing
  • 14:14over the last seven years on
  • 14:15the New Haven Leccinum project.
  • 14:20You know, hey, the goal in New Haven
  • 14:22election project has been to explore
  • 14:24the extent to which genetic variants
  • 14:26correspond with response to intervention
  • 14:28through whole genome sequence analysis,
  • 14:30serial behavioral testing and
  • 14:32serial functional MRI studies.
  • 14:35It's a longitudinal study of
  • 14:37normally developing children.
  • 14:38Grades we would enroll children
  • 14:40in grades one and follow them
  • 14:42all the way through grades five.
  • 14:44We recruited these children from
  • 14:4632 New Haven public schools.
  • 14:47These are the regular schools,
  • 14:49not the schools that have children with
  • 14:52intellectually impaired children in them.
  • 14:53We recruited about 500 and
  • 14:55following about 500 children,
  • 14:57these included a parent questionnaire
  • 14:59based on for past medical history in depth
  • 15:03questions about socioeconomic status,
  • 15:04learning disability histories,
  • 15:06primary language and spoken in the home, etc.
  • 15:09And then for each child we did 30X
  • 15:13whole genome sequencing through saliva.
  • 15:18We also did standard psychometric
  • 15:20batteries that assess reading
  • 15:22language executive function.
  • 15:24Beginning in grade one roughly every
  • 15:26six months, all the way through Grade
  • 15:285 and the people who did the executive.
  • 15:30These assessments were folks from our group
  • 15:32and people who are trained to do this,
  • 15:34so we didn't rely on school testing.
  • 15:36It was our own group who
  • 15:38actually did the testing,
  • 15:39and we used standard batteries.
  • 15:41So just for giving an example for those
  • 15:43of you who are familiar with the field,
  • 15:45these are standard neuro
  • 15:47psychometric testing batteries.
  • 15:48These are not batteries that we
  • 15:49made up or tests that we made up.
  • 15:51These were chosen by skilled
  • 15:53neuropsychologists to be part of
  • 15:56the educational assessment tool.
  • 15:58They include the tower two or the test,
  • 16:00averting reading efficiency subtests
  • 16:02from the Woodcock Johnson and subtests
  • 16:05from the Grey Oil reading test,
  • 16:06and so we actually combined
  • 16:08for the first study.
  • 16:09I'm going to show you to this today is.
  • 16:14It's what we call a composite of five
  • 16:16subtests from these standardized testing,
  • 16:19the first one is a sight word
  • 16:21efficiency from the tower,
  • 16:22where we ask children in a timed manner
  • 16:25to read real words like cat, dog, etc.
  • 16:29And then we score them for accuracy.
  • 16:31They do it for about 45 seconds,
  • 16:33so the next one is a
  • 16:35phonemic decoding efficiency.
  • 16:36It's called and so and so that
  • 16:38memory doesn't play into this.
  • 16:39We actually give them non words.
  • 16:41Words like zoup.
  • 16:42Again, it's timed and we score for accuracy.
  • 16:46The letter word ID subtest Woodcock
  • 16:48Johnson also looks at real words.
  • 16:51It's not timed,
  • 16:52but it's progressively more difficult,
  • 16:54and here we score for accuracy.
  • 16:56The number of words they accurately
  • 16:58get and then the word attack,
  • 16:59which is again back to non words again to
  • 17:01to really look at their ability to decode.
  • 17:04Also non timed and finally to
  • 17:06look at reading fluency you have
  • 17:08them read age appropriate passage.
  • 17:10It's time we score for accuracy
  • 17:13but also comprehension.
  • 17:14So we combine these five subtests.
  • 17:16Into a composite phenotype.
  • 17:19And it's quantitative, that is.
  • 17:21We could score it when we score
  • 17:24it by a number,
  • 17:25so we have high performers
  • 17:27and low performers.
  • 17:28And then the the trick here is
  • 17:31to relate it to the genetics and
  • 17:33we can do that using within the
  • 17:36same children single nucleotide
  • 17:38polymorphisms and just to re familiarize
  • 17:41yourselves with what exactly a
  • 17:43single nucleotide polymorphism is.
  • 17:45I put up this little diagram that is
  • 17:49diagramming a specific single nucleotide.
  • 17:52Polymorphism has this horrific name called
  • 17:58Rs 6935076. It's specifically
  • 18:00located on chromosome 8 and its
  • 18:03position is 24 million, 644,000,
  • 18:07zero and 94 nucleotides from the top
  • 18:12of the short arm of chromosome 8.
  • 18:15And here you can see the sequence
  • 18:17is very specific and because
  • 18:18all of us carry 2 chromosomes,
  • 18:20there are two representations of this area
  • 18:22and of this specific position and person.
  • 18:25One here is inherited from his mother
  • 18:27ASI and from his father he's inherited.
  • 18:30Also, I see and so we would say
  • 18:32that for this snip at this point,
  • 18:34which is which has a location
  • 18:37specifically assigned to it,
  • 18:39this person is homozygous for
  • 18:41the C allele or CC.
  • 18:43This person I a different person actually
  • 18:47carries AT inherited from the mother
  • 18:49and AT inherited from the father.
  • 18:51So therefore this person would be homozygous
  • 18:54for the teal or TT at this specific snip,
  • 18:57and then finally we have the heterozygous
  • 19:00individual who inherited AC from one parent.
  • 19:02And a T from the other parent.
  • 19:03So this person would be called the CT or
  • 19:06the genotype would be a CT designating
  • 19:09that that this person has one of each.
  • 19:12So that's what a snip is.
  • 19:13It's just a single nucleotide difference
  • 19:16at a very specific location for everybody.
  • 19:19So Rs.
  • 19:226935076 that specific location can
  • 19:24be identified in all of us as well
  • 19:27as our 500 children enrolled in
  • 19:29New Haven election on project.
  • 19:31So it does take a look at these
  • 19:33three at these three genotypes.
  • 19:36The CC,
  • 19:37the see T and the TT and what we
  • 19:39can do for any individual child
  • 19:41since we now can determine the
  • 19:43CCC T or TT status of that child
  • 19:46is then related to performance on
  • 19:49the decoding composite phenotype.
  • 19:51And So what you can see here is
  • 19:53that the CC in in in a single
  • 19:56individual might my correspond with
  • 19:58low performance and asked sort of
  • 20:00in the middle performance and AT&A.
  • 20:03Why, in other words,
  • 20:04it would appear here is that the
  • 20:07C allele is detrimental and that
  • 20:09the more see you have,
  • 20:11the lower your performances
  • 20:13in this decoding composite.
  • 20:17So there's a relationship here.
  • 20:18It's actually a linear relationship.
  • 20:21More see the worse you do Morty,
  • 20:24the better you do,
  • 20:25and so if you do this over a lot of
  • 20:28individuals then you can actually
  • 20:30get a coefficient of correlation.
  • 20:32The R-squared over those individuals
  • 20:34and you could assign that value
  • 20:37AP value a confidence value.
  • 20:40And then you can graph that
  • 20:42value for that individual snip.
  • 20:44So here for this particular snip
  • 20:486935076 located where I said 24
  • 20:51million etc on chromosome 8 with a
  • 20:54particular R-squared in 500 children,
  • 20:56you can graph that on a P value
  • 20:59on his P value.
  • 21:00The confidence interval
  • 21:01for that relationship.
  • 21:02In fact,
  • 21:03you can do this for a lot of snips
  • 21:06on chromosome 8. Not just one.
  • 21:10In fact.
  • 21:11Every one of these dots represents
  • 21:13a different snip genotyped in
  • 21:15all 500 of our children.
  • 21:17In fact,
  • 21:18there are many snips on chromosome 8.
  • 21:20There's well over 10,000, and so after.
  • 21:23If you want to present them graphically
  • 21:25again relative to their P value
  • 21:27for their R-squared, you literally
  • 21:29saturate or paint that chromosome.
  • 21:32So represented here is every snip that we
  • 21:35tested against the composite phenotype for
  • 21:38decoding within our cohort of 500 children.
  • 21:42And in fact we did this for every chromosome,
  • 21:45so I just depicted chromosome 8.
  • 21:47But this is chromosome 9,
  • 21:49chromosome 1011, etc.
  • 21:50All the way out to 22 and you'll
  • 21:53notice that one is the widest because
  • 21:56chromosome one is the longest,
  • 21:58so it has the most number of snips
  • 22:01and chromosome 22 is the shortest.
  • 22:03So has the least number of Snips.
  • 22:06This is real data.
  • 22:07This is the real result we call this
  • 22:09for obvious reasons in Manhattan plot.
  • 22:12Right, and here's a peek at
  • 22:13the Manhattan plot.
  • 22:14In fact, for the decoding composite,
  • 22:17we got a peak right here on chromosome 19
  • 22:20by a number of snips in a very small area.
  • 22:24And because of the magic of
  • 22:25the Human Genome Project,
  • 22:26we now can tell since we know precisely what
  • 22:29the nucleotide number in chromosome it is,
  • 22:32we can identify the gene
  • 22:34that this corresponds to,
  • 22:35and it corresponds to a gene that was
  • 22:37not previously implicated in reading,
  • 22:39but which we found to be very strongly
  • 22:41have a very strong effect size called Gary.
  • 22:45One in our cohort of 500 children.
  • 22:48What do I mean by that?
  • 22:49Let's go back into our cohort of 500
  • 22:52kids and let's look at four different
  • 22:54assessments of reading or reading subtests.
  • 22:57The word, letter ID, word attack.
  • 22:59Which word letter ID is timed,
  • 23:02word comprehension, and reading fluency?
  • 23:04That's the.
  • 23:05That's the paragraph passage.
  • 23:07And let's look at kids who have the
  • 23:09Gary run risk allele here in orange,
  • 23:11and you can see is that they enter
  • 23:14first grade behind the kids in the
  • 23:17same cohort at the same schools.
  • 23:19At the same age in the same grade,
  • 23:22at at, at, at, at,
  • 23:24at a significant disadvantage.
  • 23:25And if we follow these same kids
  • 23:27because it's a longitudinal study,
  • 23:29you can see they never catch up.
  • 23:32Let me show you what that looks like.
  • 23:34This is a comprehension,
  • 23:36right,
  • 23:36a fundamental and important things
  • 23:38skill in reading.
  • 23:39If we look at kids in grade at the
  • 23:42start of grade one at the end of Grade
  • 23:441 starter grade two end of grade 2 all
  • 23:47the way up to the start of grade five,
  • 23:49you can see is that children in oak.
  • 23:51Our cohort here at the risk
  • 23:53allele for Gary One,
  • 23:54started off at a disadvantage
  • 23:55to kids who do not carry any
  • 23:57risk alleles for Gary one,
  • 23:59and it's never really.
  • 24:00They never really filled a gap.
  • 24:02Even at the start of the Great Five,
  • 24:04so it's sustained.
  • 24:05It's a sustain effect over this age group.
  • 24:12We can look further into Gary one we didn't
  • 24:14know this so we looked in the text portal,
  • 24:16which gives you expression data in different
  • 24:19parts of the body and specifically
  • 24:21in different parts of the brain.
  • 24:23And to our surprise, Gary went well.
  • 24:25It's not surprising that Gary
  • 24:26wins expressed in the brain,
  • 24:28but its strongest expression is
  • 24:30in the cerebellum, the cerebellum
  • 24:32hemisphere and total cerebellum.
  • 24:35That was a surprise.
  • 24:36But not really that much because what
  • 24:38we know is that the cerebellum is
  • 24:41intimately linked to the cerebral cortex,
  • 24:43the cortical area where there are
  • 24:47specific and specified specialized
  • 24:49reading areas by a circuit or a
  • 24:52cerebellar cerebral cortex loop.
  • 24:55This is well known and in fact
  • 24:57its support of a theory that was
  • 25:00put forward in the 1990s called a
  • 25:03cerebellar theory of reading disability.
  • 25:05Nicholson Faucet observed that reading
  • 25:07disability was also associated with
  • 25:10deficits in cerebellar related functions.
  • 25:12Distonia time estimation skill
  • 25:15optimization and balance,
  • 25:18and so they posited that the cortical
  • 25:21cerebellar circuit shown here has
  • 25:23an important role in reading.
  • 25:24There has now been a whole lot
  • 25:26more published.
  • 25:27On the cerebellar theory of reading.
  • 25:29And sometimes some people are in
  • 25:31favor and some people are not,
  • 25:32but we go where the science is taking us.
  • 25:35And since Gary one is implicated,
  • 25:36certainly by association studies,
  • 25:39we pursue that.
  • 25:41And so I don't really have the time
  • 25:42to go through all the data and
  • 25:44all the projects that we're going,
  • 25:46so let me sort of summarize
  • 25:49those briefly here.
  • 25:50So our New Haven,
  • 25:51Lexington project,
  • 25:52which began to implicate the cerebellum,
  • 25:54actually spurred a number of
  • 25:57interesting projects Haley Dasilva
  • 25:58in our lab has actually worked
  • 26:01on a a program for processing and
  • 26:04preprocessing imaging data that
  • 26:06she calls Neuro Stack specifically
  • 26:08made for a WS to Amazon Web server.
  • 26:11And that's really important because
  • 26:12a lot of the imaging data,
  • 26:14for example from ABCD, is stored,
  • 26:17allocated,
  • 26:17processed and worked on in an
  • 26:20Amazon Web Services up on the web.
  • 26:22So in this is all cloud based work
  • 26:24and Haley actually created this
  • 26:27new tool that is now generally
  • 26:29available as it's free and it's
  • 26:32incredibly useful for neuroscientists.
  • 26:33You know who don't want to get into
  • 26:36the specifics of preprocessing and
  • 26:38processing data. Tremendous work.
  • 26:41We also calcium xerac in our lab who's
  • 26:44been working with Young Frieder is unusual.
  • 26:46Haley also,
  • 26:47as well as Emily Curtain,
  • 26:48has been working in ABCD.
  • 26:50ABCD is a longitudinal study of
  • 26:54typically developing 10,000 US
  • 26:56children from across like 32 sites
  • 26:58in the United States and they're
  • 27:01getting cereal imaging studies
  • 27:02as well as genotyping.
  • 27:04Genotyping,
  • 27:05one serial imaging studies and and
  • 27:07as and some cognitive assessments
  • 27:10to the NH Toolbox.
  • 27:11That data is publicly available and
  • 27:13so with kelson's been doing is he's
  • 27:16been doing genetic correlation studies
  • 27:18between the Leccinum project and ABCD,
  • 27:20and in a way that he's been
  • 27:22using these to create sort of to
  • 27:24leverage our small data set with
  • 27:27a larger data set. This is a theme
  • 27:29that that goes on and on with us.
  • 27:33Seeing Wang has been working by doing
  • 27:36our due osis one I just showed you as
  • 27:40well as T Ross is Tiwa Sizarr transcript
  • 27:43transcript own wide association studies
  • 27:45and So what she does is she goes back
  • 27:49into GTX that expression data data
  • 27:51data set in the brain either in the
  • 27:53celebra ballimore the whole brain
  • 27:55and she uses that information to
  • 27:57wait the snips for the for the guasa.
  • 28:00Therefore it's called that
  • 28:02waser transcriptome.
  • 28:03Wide Association study and she's and and
  • 28:06we've been working with helping as well.
  • 28:09Helping Zang Kate Connors from the
  • 28:11group with young Frieders has been
  • 28:13doing looking at our math data
  • 28:15because I think initially,
  • 28:17when I explained how the
  • 28:19election project works,
  • 28:20we do executive function which
  • 28:22includes attention as well as math,
  • 28:24math subjects as well.
  • 28:26So Kate's been been really focusing
  • 28:28on math and we'll be hearing some more
  • 28:30data from that in the next couple of years.
  • 28:33And then finally Steven Penny Agra.
  • 28:35His spirit had had a collaboration
  • 28:37with Indian Park over the stem Cell
  • 28:40center and developing neurons human
  • 28:42neurons from human embryonic stem
  • 28:44cells and so the idea here being
  • 28:46is that one of the things that's
  • 28:47really held back.
  • 28:48Translational Neuroscience is access
  • 28:50to brain human brain material,
  • 28:53and so unlike cancer,
  • 28:54when you can get lots of material,
  • 28:57in fact it's would be difficult.
  • 29:00I would say it's impossible to get.
  • 29:02Human brain material from from normal brains.
  • 29:05And so in order to.
  • 29:07If you want to study human neurons
  • 29:09that what you do is you get human
  • 29:12embryonic stem cells and then
  • 29:14you begin to develop you.
  • 29:18You can induce them to develop
  • 29:20into human neurons along the way,
  • 29:22and what Steven has done is
  • 29:24he's knocked down using CRISPR.
  • 29:26The expression of a prominent reading
  • 29:31or dyslexia gene called KA 319 and
  • 29:33what he's shown is that this puts
  • 29:36the cells in a quiessence state,
  • 29:38so these are neuronal progenitors
  • 29:40arrested in a quiescent state.
  • 29:42So to analyze that data which you know
  • 29:45we've been doing a lot of sequencing there,
  • 29:47is you.
  • 29:48You are a few,
  • 29:49I'm just doing a marvelous job
  • 29:51again from Helping's group and one
  • 29:52of our undergraduate students.
  • 29:54Is he Lopez?
  • 29:54So these are things that are
  • 29:56currently ongoing.
  • 29:57Major projects in the lab.
  • 29:59I don't have time to talk about a
  • 30:01whole lot today, but I just wanted
  • 30:03to know that they're going on.
  • 30:05OK,
  • 30:05so the question is or what the
  • 30:08question I posed before I left this
  • 30:10slide was how do we do this magic?
  • 30:13How do we identify these children
  • 30:14early and when I said was we do this
  • 30:17molecular genetic studies and I showed
  • 30:19you a nice example focusing on dyslexia.
  • 30:24But do we really disrupt by doing this?
  • 30:27Do we really disrupt the current
  • 30:29current educational program?
  • 30:30Like how to disrupt the
  • 30:32current educational paradigm?
  • 30:33Is that really the question or is the
  • 30:36question is how do we really disrupt
  • 30:39the current educational paradigm?
  • 30:40And I think that's really,
  • 30:41really an important question for us,
  • 30:43because doing our studies
  • 30:45and publishing them,
  • 30:46even publishing them in peer
  • 30:48reviewed journals has been great.
  • 30:50It's been great. You know,
  • 30:51we've been able to get support for that,
  • 30:53both private foundation.
  • 30:54Age foundation and do
  • 30:55these really cool studies,
  • 30:57but are we really moving
  • 30:59them into the classroom?
  • 31:00And I would say I have to
  • 31:02admit in fact that we were not.
  • 31:03So if we really want to do this,
  • 31:05where can we make these changes
  • 31:07so we can translate our findings?
  • 31:09Our ability to identify kids at
  • 31:11high risk for learning disabilities
  • 31:13at a young age when it's really
  • 31:15important saying kindergarten?
  • 31:16How do we get that adopted in
  • 31:19public schools across America?
  • 31:20Even in New Haven,
  • 31:22across the state of Connecticut or America?
  • 31:25So to do that,
  • 31:27we really need policy level change.
  • 31:30In the last five years,
  • 31:31dyslexia and learning disability
  • 31:33policies at the state and federal
  • 31:36levels have improved significantly,
  • 31:38most notably in the areas of
  • 31:41teacher training and universal
  • 31:42dyslexia screening in grades,
  • 31:44kindergarten through third grade.
  • 31:4841 states today have universal
  • 31:50evidence based screening for dyslexia,
  • 31:53including Connecticut and Massachusetts.
  • 31:56And the feds have clarified that you
  • 31:58can now use this term Once Upon you couldn't.
  • 32:00You could use this term when,
  • 32:03when,
  • 32:04when teachers create the iep's,
  • 32:06the individual educational
  • 32:07programs that are so important in
  • 32:10mandated for each child that has
  • 32:12a disability learning disability,
  • 32:1432 states across the country provide a
  • 32:17list of approved evidence based screeners.
  • 32:20None of them are genetic.
  • 32:23So our goal is to pass legislation
  • 32:26that includes reliable and validated
  • 32:28genetic screening for risk of, say,
  • 32:31dyslexia or any learning disability.
  • 32:34And get it,
  • 32:35get it on the improved list
  • 32:37of evidence based screeners.
  • 32:40This would allow parents to screen
  • 32:42their child as early as even birth,
  • 32:45but definitely pre K for risk of
  • 32:46any of the learning disabilities,
  • 32:48including dyslexia.
  • 32:49It would require school districts
  • 32:51to take those results into account
  • 32:54when when they create the iips,
  • 32:56the individual educational
  • 32:57program plans and the 504 plans.
  • 33:02And this would enable children
  • 33:04finally to receive an evidence based
  • 33:06reading instruction or intervention
  • 33:08method at the very earliest
  • 33:10stages of reading acquisition,
  • 33:12when they are most important
  • 33:14and are most likely to work.
  • 33:17So I've last 20 minutes or so.
  • 33:20I presented a hodgepodge really
  • 33:22of information from a number
  • 33:24of seemingly discordant fields.
  • 33:26I presented some epidemiological data
  • 33:29showing what is the frequency of of.
  • 33:31Well, I presented reading disability,
  • 33:33but in fact of learning disabilities and
  • 33:35it's somewhere in the 10 to 20% range,
  • 33:37I presented an intervention
  • 33:39program from my colleague or or
  • 33:41the results of an intervention
  • 33:43intense intervention program for
  • 33:45my colleague Maureen.
  • 33:46Love it at the University of Toronto
  • 33:48and showed how important it is.
  • 33:50For early intervention and for
  • 33:52it to work and to be and for
  • 33:55its effects to be sustained.
  • 33:57I presented some genetic genetic studies,
  • 33:59both molecular genetic studies
  • 34:00and statistical genetic studies
  • 34:02to show the importance of
  • 34:03genetics and why genetics should,
  • 34:05could and should be used for early
  • 34:07screening to identify kids at high
  • 34:09risk for attention disorders,
  • 34:11reading disabilities,
  • 34:12language disorders, etc.
  • 34:15And I presented an education paradigm
  • 34:17that doesn't work the wait to fail role
  • 34:20model and an alternative to that model.
  • 34:22And finally I I presented some
  • 34:25policy stuff and so the fact is
  • 34:28is that it's a bit of a mess,
  • 34:30so to find a form that accommodates
  • 34:33the entire mess that I've just
  • 34:35presented and to unite these from my
  • 34:38friend Samuel Becker from 1961 and
  • 34:40to unite these seemingly unrelated
  • 34:43initiatives under a single umbrella
  • 34:46this year.
  • 34:48With a lot of help from Gene Shapiro,
  • 34:50Cliff Boag and the Dean,
  • 34:53we created the Yale Program for
  • 34:57Learning Disabilities Research.
  • 34:59Our program is multidisciplinary.
  • 35:01It covers the fields of neuropsychology,
  • 35:04Biostatistics,
  • 35:05education, neuroimaging,
  • 35:06and genetic and the people
  • 35:09who come into this group.
  • 35:12They know their field well,
  • 35:14but neuro psychologists who come in
  • 35:15don't know a whole lot about genetics.
  • 35:18Geneticists that come in don't
  • 35:19know a whole lot about education,
  • 35:22often not a whole lot about neuroimaging,
  • 35:24so it's some very important and
  • 35:26we really stress that we begin to
  • 35:28learn each other's vocabulary that
  • 35:29we really interact personally.
  • 35:31Although this year it's been
  • 35:33difficult to interact personally and
  • 35:35so we can inform each other studies
  • 35:37and help and work collaboratively
  • 35:38collaboratively within the lab.
  • 35:40And I think we've been pretty successful.
  • 35:42I think that's like,
  • 35:43for example,
  • 35:44the study I presented from the
  • 35:45Haven election on project.
  • 35:46You can see that's a combined effort from
  • 35:50neuropsychologist geneticist Biostatistics.
  • 35:52I didn't show you the neuroimaging data,
  • 35:53but it's remarkable how well that works and
  • 35:56now we're moving into education with a PhD,
  • 35:58PhD,
  • 35:59postdoc in education recently joined the lab.
  • 36:01So these are great initiatives
  • 36:03and show how we're really blurring
  • 36:05all the lines amongst all these
  • 36:07different disciplines.
  • 36:08But we're also trying to go cross
  • 36:10campus and reach out and bridge
  • 36:12the gap to other schools to other
  • 36:14students and the other professional
  • 36:16schools in the university.
  • 36:18So I'm especially proud to say that
  • 36:20we've been working with the folks at
  • 36:22the Yale Educational Studies program,
  • 36:24and we'll be offering a A1
  • 36:27semester long course,
  • 36:28mostly for undergraduates
  • 36:29beginning this fall.
  • 36:31This is the first course that they've been.
  • 36:33Offering on learning disabilities and
  • 36:35it's a full I think it's 17 lectures
  • 36:37over the course of the semester,
  • 36:39so this is developed by the
  • 36:41folks in our in our group most.
  • 36:44There's no Lee,
  • 36:46Kim Tsujimoto and John Boston,
  • 36:49and so they're prepared to present this.
  • 36:51It is currently under review, but it's it's.
  • 36:53It looks like it's going to start
  • 36:55in the fall. We work really strongly
  • 36:57with the folks in the computational
  • 36:59biology and bio for Mattix program,
  • 37:01but they have members from those
  • 37:03programs in our lab and plus we
  • 37:05present to them on a regular basis.
  • 37:07This is a really extraordinary new
  • 37:09program here at the School of Management.
  • 37:12It's called the broad center or
  • 37:14the Broad Center for Educational
  • 37:16Leadership and Public Education.
  • 37:18It's a brand new initiative.
  • 37:19I think there are about a dozen broad
  • 37:21Centers for this across the country.
  • 37:23It was just started this past
  • 37:24fall that brings in educational
  • 37:26leaders from around the country.
  • 37:28These are superintendents and school
  • 37:30principals in the largest districts in the
  • 37:32United States and they engage in a yearlong,
  • 37:34transformative leadership
  • 37:35program in public education.
  • 37:38And so we're in contact and we're
  • 37:39working with these folks as well.
  • 37:41And then finally,
  • 37:42of course,
  • 37:42we're based here in the Department
  • 37:44of Pediatrics and Genetics,
  • 37:45where which is our home base and
  • 37:48where we're going to also be reaching
  • 37:51out to residents and hopefully.
  • 37:53Increase the exposure they're
  • 37:54having to learning disabilities
  • 37:56that they're going to encounter
  • 37:58when they enter practice as well,
  • 38:00'cause it's so common.
  • 38:02So this is our Yale program for
  • 38:05Learning Disabilities Research.
  • 38:06Its comprehensive, it's large,
  • 38:08it's ambitious.
  • 38:10We just got started this past September.
  • 38:12These are the different components
  • 38:14I tried to present to you,
  • 38:15and with that I'm happy to take
  • 38:18questions and also a big shout out
  • 38:20to all these wonderful people who
  • 38:21make this sort of research possible,
  • 38:24as well as our funding groups
  • 38:26from private foundations,
  • 38:27them and foundations to the
  • 38:28NIH as well as to individuals.
  • 38:31And of course, thanks to all the children.
  • 38:33Families who participated in
  • 38:34all these studies.
  • 38:36So if there are questions,
  • 38:37I'm happy to take them.
  • 38:44OK, Jeff, thank you so much.
  • 38:46That was amazing.
  • 38:47That was fantastic.
  • 38:48Now we do have questions.
  • 38:49I mean, I invite folks to submit
  • 38:52questions via the chat or the Q&A,
  • 38:54but we've got some questions already.
  • 38:56Doctor Who in one of your
  • 38:58earlier slides indicated that
  • 39:00heritability for dyslexia is 0.80.
  • 39:02How much of that is accounted
  • 39:04for it by the SNP and Gary one?
  • 39:06Is there a polygenic risk score for dyslexia?
  • 39:09If so, how much variance?
  • 39:11Does it account for?
  • 39:12That's a great question.
  • 39:14Thank you for asking so as clearly
  • 39:17the you know Paul knows is that
  • 39:20there's really two types of.
  • 39:23Yeah, of variants that you can account
  • 39:25for heritability that you can account for.
  • 39:27There's broad sense heritability,
  • 39:29and there's narrow sense heritability.
  • 39:31When I say 80%, it's it's broad sense
  • 39:34heritability that is when you look
  • 39:36at twin studies and family studies,
  • 39:38and then you see,
  • 39:39and then you ask the question,
  • 39:40what's the concordance rate between
  • 39:42siblings who are affected, say,
  • 39:44was reading disability in those that aren't,
  • 39:46and you compare them the the classic
  • 39:48example would be identical twins
  • 39:50which share 100% of their genome.
  • 39:53The concordance rate there versus
  • 39:55the concordance rates and non
  • 39:57identical twins which only share
  • 39:58on average about 50% and so if
  • 40:00you do those sorts of those are.
  • 40:02Those are standard heritability studies
  • 40:04in the 1980s and they would say that
  • 40:07the genetic component is about 80%.
  • 40:10There has been a polygenic risk score.
  • 40:14Publication from Robert Plomin's
  • 40:17group from the UK.
  • 40:19He has the largest collection of twins,
  • 40:22I think in probably the largest one is
  • 40:24in Australia but he has the largest
  • 40:26one that was reading assessments.
  • 40:28I think it's 10,000 twins and he
  • 40:30was able to account for all snips
  • 40:32that he looked at and he looked
  • 40:34at a genome wide panel.
  • 40:36I think about 700,000 snips.
  • 40:38He was able to account for
  • 40:40about 8% of the heritability.
  • 40:42This is pretty common
  • 40:44for polygenic disorders.
  • 40:45That is that even if you look at cancer etc
  • 40:48etc etc and just look at common variants,
  • 40:51which is what's represented and a
  • 40:53single nucleotide polymorphism panel,
  • 40:55you generally there's a huge gap
  • 40:57between broad sense heritability
  • 40:58and narrow sense heritability.
  • 41:00Now, to answer you Paul direct
  • 41:02your question directly, Paul,
  • 41:03if you look at any single snip,
  • 41:05you're looking only at a fraction
  • 41:07of the heritability that is Gary.
  • 41:09One explains only a tiny amount,
  • 41:12so all that's why.
  • 41:13And I didn't get into it on this talk,
  • 41:16but in previous.
  • 41:17Oxide, I mentioned it is that is
  • 41:19that dyslexia language acquisition,
  • 41:21language impairment,
  • 41:23verbal trait disorder,
  • 41:25dyscalculia,
  • 41:25or the all these are polygenic disorders.
  • 41:28You have to be lucky enough to
  • 41:30have the smorgasbord of the right
  • 41:32variants all at once for that.
  • 41:34For that to happen,
  • 41:35it is rarely a single gene disorder,
  • 41:38although we have been looking
  • 41:40at rare variants,
  • 41:41and it does occur from time to time.
  • 41:43None of them are in Gary want.
  • 41:47Thank you Jeff. A much more general question.
  • 41:48How did you become involved in this research?
  • 41:52Uh. So. Most people think that you know,
  • 41:58Jeff. He's a pediatrician,
  • 41:59and so he cares about children
  • 42:01and he cares about reading and he
  • 42:03got somehow seduced into genetics.
  • 42:05But actually it's the opposite.
  • 42:08And yes, I'm a pediatrician.
  • 42:10Yes, I care about children.
  • 42:11Yes, I care about reading,
  • 42:12but the way I got in was during
  • 42:15fellowship with with you Mark,
  • 42:17I actually got first.
  • 42:19Initially I was in cell biology and then
  • 42:21I got seduced over to molecular genetics
  • 42:23just at the time of the beginnings.
  • 42:25Of the Human Genome Project,
  • 42:27and I thought, you know,
  • 42:27this is a pretty cool thing,
  • 42:29and so I ended up in a lab that
  • 42:31does that was really pioneering
  • 42:33many of the methods for the human
  • 42:35Genome project and my first.
  • 42:37This was insuring Weismans lab here at Yale,
  • 42:39and at that time what we were doing is
  • 42:42remember there is no human genome project.
  • 42:44It was just starting off the ground.
  • 42:45Nobody really knew how to
  • 42:47clone the human genome,
  • 42:48and so one of my early
  • 42:49projects was the clone.
  • 42:50The short arm of chromosome 6 and then one
  • 42:52day Sherm came up to me and said, Jeff,
  • 42:55why do you clone the hemochromatosis?
  • 42:58And of course, I said sure you know,
  • 43:00I'm a pediatrician,
  • 43:01I don't really know very
  • 43:02much about hemochromatosis.
  • 43:04It sounds like a disease of adults and
  • 43:07Alcoholics, and he goes no, no, no you.
  • 43:08We we've known that it's genetic
  • 43:10for a long time,
  • 43:11and you've already cloned it.
  • 43:13It's one of your one of your 6 or
  • 43:157000 tubes you have in your freezer,
  • 43:17so just go figure out which one it was,
  • 43:19and so that's how it got started.
  • 43:21We got scooped by a private
  • 43:23company several years later,
  • 43:24but I had all these resources
  • 43:26for chromosome 6.
  • 43:27I looked around and there was this locus.
  • 43:29There was this location of a strong
  • 43:32genetic effect on the short arm
  • 43:34of chromosome 6 for which I was,
  • 43:36I think,
  • 43:36at that time probably the only
  • 43:38person in the universe who actually
  • 43:40had complete coverage in new
  • 43:42were all the markers were,
  • 43:43and that's how I got started.
  • 43:45And so the truth is, yes, I'm Peter, Trish.
  • 43:48And yes, I practice medicine.
  • 43:49Yes, I care about children.
  • 43:51I care about reading,
  • 43:52but the reason I really jumped into
  • 43:54this is 'cause I had a strategic,
  • 43:56scientific advantage.
  • 43:57And I took advantage of it.
  • 44:01Thank you. From Steve uptegrove.
  • 44:05He even has, unfortunately a well
  • 44:07known history for a high prevalence
  • 44:08of childhood lead poisoning while
  • 44:10childhood lead exposure is supposed
  • 44:12to be documented on all children PE
  • 44:14forms upon school entry in Connecticut.
  • 44:16This information is not routinely been used
  • 44:18to identify those at high risk for the
  • 44:21same disabilities you were concerned with.
  • 44:23Rather, they come to the light
  • 44:25only by the same weight to fail,
  • 44:27model the wait to fail model you mentioned.
  • 44:29What has been lacking are the
  • 44:31resources in schools for particularly
  • 44:33districts like New Haven, Kubara,
  • 44:35Heavy burden of potentially at risk hits.
  • 44:39Steven, that's a great question.
  • 44:41First of all, I want to tell and just,
  • 44:43you know, reach out to Steve is that
  • 44:45when we first started our studies
  • 44:47here in New Haven Public Schools,
  • 44:49Steven was one of the earliest
  • 44:51persons that I reached out to because
  • 44:54I think at that time Steve you
  • 44:56were either on New Haven School,
  • 44:58you were on the school board or you
  • 45:00add connections to the school board.
  • 45:02And so you were one of the first people
  • 45:04and you were wonderful and I greatly
  • 45:06appreciate all the help that you
  • 45:08offered and all the advice that would.
  • 45:09Proved to be very, very useful.
  • 45:12Second of all is that I'm not
  • 45:15discounting the environmental exposures,
  • 45:17they're huge and and they're a big deal,
  • 45:19and they are certainly a big deal
  • 45:22to different socioeconomic groups.
  • 45:23I can't answer your your your excellent
  • 45:26question directly because that would
  • 45:28that would require is probably you know
  • 45:30more lead testing and more documentation,
  • 45:33but what I can say is that we've
  • 45:35begun as part of the Yale Program
  • 45:37for Learning Disabilities Research.
  • 45:39We've begun a formal program to look
  • 45:42at the electronic medical record we
  • 45:44started looking at by just looking
  • 45:47at coding for learning disabilities.
  • 45:49And we we're doing this project
  • 45:52collaboratively with Emily Power,
  • 45:53so so I think we're just starting to
  • 45:55get the data at interesting enough.
  • 45:58Interestingly enough,
  • 45:58we're probably looking right now
  • 46:00at about 40,000 kids in total,
  • 46:03and amongst the 40,000 kids there.
  • 46:05We're looking for codes that would at
  • 46:07least implicate some learning disability.
  • 46:09From there, we'll go on and that is.
  • 46:11We'll try and peel the onion and
  • 46:13get to be well.
  • 46:14What is the real learning
  • 46:15disabilities that they have could be?
  • 46:16They could? Could they be overcoated?
  • 46:18Could be undercoated,
  • 46:19but one of the things that we
  • 46:21will definitely look look for now
  • 46:23that you've mentioned it.
  • 46:24And thank you for asking is we'll
  • 46:26look at lead levels 'cause we can do
  • 46:28that in the electronic medical record
  • 46:30and we'll try to see if that is a
  • 46:31significant factor in these things.
  • 46:34I suspect it is,
  • 46:35but I also suspect there correlate
  • 46:37strongly with socioeconomic status,
  • 46:39which is something I didn't mention
  • 46:41in the GWAS that I presented.
  • 46:42But in that GWAS it is corrected
  • 46:45for socioeconomic status.
  • 46:46We always do that as well as sex and age,
  • 46:50and so that's an important part.
  • 46:51But I'm going to add lead to the list.
  • 46:53I think that's a great question.
  • 46:57Jeff from Jim Pelligrini Jeff.
  • 46:58Truly amazing.
  • 46:59You mentioned executive functioning.
  • 47:01Any research at the genetic level.
  • 47:02Here schizophrenics can have
  • 47:04excellent verbal and reading skills,
  • 47:06but severely poor executive function.
  • 47:11No, I I've been very careful to
  • 47:13stay away from the psychosis Jim.
  • 47:16That's a that's a well populated
  • 47:19field to be politically correct.
  • 47:22And so I, you know, sort of stay in my
  • 47:25lane and within within with children
  • 47:27and typically and specifically.
  • 47:29And typically developing children
  • 47:31if you're not typically developing.
  • 47:34That is, if you have a neuro
  • 47:36psychosis or even autism.
  • 47:38We've been excluding those
  • 47:39children from our studies.
  • 47:41Really trying to focus on those
  • 47:43pathways and circuits that are
  • 47:45specific for either reading
  • 47:47language or executive function.
  • 47:49Having said that,
  • 47:51we have extensive executive function
  • 47:53assessments on all 500 of our kids
  • 47:56in our longitudinal study as well as
  • 47:59the 1300 kids in our initial cross
  • 48:01sectional study called the Grad study,
  • 48:03and so we've actually performed
  • 48:05our first juos and tiwas on that
  • 48:07data specifically on attention,
  • 48:09and I can tell you that our
  • 48:11preliminary results.
  • 48:12Look really,
  • 48:12really good,
  • 48:13so I think yes we will have some
  • 48:16jeans and genetic variants that
  • 48:18will correspond beautifully and and
  • 48:20will identify risk for attention.
  • 48:22I can tell you is that that there
  • 48:24are a lot of good people in this
  • 48:26field and a number of handful
  • 48:27of genes for us specifically for
  • 48:29attention have been identified,
  • 48:31and so I think this is a very
  • 48:33cool area of interest.
  • 48:35We continue to pursue it
  • 48:37and I'll keep you posted I.
  • 48:39I think we'll have something solid.
  • 48:42I'm hoping the manuscript will
  • 48:43go out before June.
  • 48:47Thanks Jeff from Julia Rosenberg.
  • 48:49Thank you for a great talk.
  • 48:51I'd love to learn if English language
  • 48:53learners have been included in these
  • 48:54studies in New Haven schools and if
  • 48:56you have any insight into how some of
  • 48:58these findings may factor in for those
  • 49:00who are English language learners.
  • 49:02Thank you. Hi
  • 49:03Julia, so the short answer is yes,
  • 49:06you know there are.
  • 49:07So if you're going to be in New Haven
  • 49:09public schools, you're going to be.
  • 49:11You're going to have English
  • 49:13language learners. So the.
  • 49:15We for our initial study of the grad study,
  • 49:19we required that so that
  • 49:21was an older age group,
  • 49:23so those kids were nine,
  • 49:26roughly 9 years old,
  • 49:27and we required that they have been at
  • 49:30least three years in instruction in the
  • 49:32United States to be in our studies.
  • 49:34Remember that study?
  • 49:35The grad study was the 1st,
  • 49:37and remains the only study of
  • 49:40underrepresented minorities of
  • 49:41genetics and reading disability effort,
  • 49:44and so, and we identified variants.
  • 49:46Do that and publish variance through
  • 49:48that and so that was for the grad
  • 49:50studies in the haven next rental project.
  • 49:53We didn't have much.
  • 49:55Basically they could get by
  • 49:57with a cursory understanding.
  • 49:59In English we didn't.
  • 50:02We didn't offer in the initial
  • 50:04study the grad study we offered
  • 50:06Spanish language testing.
  • 50:08We actually, interestingly enough,
  • 50:09we really see a difference between
  • 50:12English and testing them in English
  • 50:14or Spanish and the New Haven next.
  • 50:16Project just about all the kids
  • 50:18are pretty proficient in English,
  • 50:20but it wasn't an exclusionary criteria.
  • 50:24Thanks Jeff. Question from Susan Blvd.
  • 50:28Do you know why New York and
  • 50:30California were two of the states that
  • 50:32were not doing dyslexia screening
  • 50:33and any concerns that prenatal
  • 50:35screening might be used to terminate.
  • 50:37Quote non ideal End Quote babies so
  • 50:41I can't tell you about California New York.
  • 50:43Sorry but I do know that there that
  • 50:46they haven't passed legislation
  • 50:47yet for universal screening.
  • 50:49I suspect it has a lot to do with
  • 50:51money and so you know these are
  • 50:53expensive things, but I don't.
  • 50:55I don't know the specifics.
  • 50:56Regarding prenatal diagnosis,
  • 50:57I get this question a lot.
  • 50:59People find me, they call me and
  • 51:01this is what I tell them and it,
  • 51:03you know I I don't work
  • 51:06with children with dyslexia,
  • 51:08I'm a neonatologist,
  • 51:08but I my group does and and and
  • 51:11certainly I interact with people
  • 51:13from around the world that work with
  • 51:16children with reading disability
  • 51:18and they tell me two things.
  • 51:19Yes, they struggle.
  • 51:21And they also tell me that a
  • 51:23significant number of them have
  • 51:25other really significant talents.
  • 51:26For example,
  • 51:27there's a group that has told
  • 51:28me that about 20% this is,
  • 51:30and I've I've heard this from multiple
  • 51:32neuropsychologists that there's
  • 51:33probably somewhere between 10 and 20%
  • 51:35of kids with really severe dyslexia.
  • 51:37Have this special ability to be able to
  • 51:40see things in three dimensional space.
  • 51:43It's really a marvel of ability to do,
  • 51:46and so if you if you look at all these
  • 51:48examples of successes and people
  • 51:49who have had pretty severe dyslexia.
  • 51:52Done well business and academia
  • 51:53etc and they they have been tested
  • 51:56and they really do have dyslexia.
  • 51:58The question is why would you
  • 52:00want to exclude them?
  • 52:01Why would you want to
  • 52:02do an early termination?
  • 52:03So I do my very best to when people
  • 52:06call and I think I've been pretty
  • 52:09convincing is that this would
  • 52:10not be something that he won.
  • 52:12I would either test 4 or #2 recommend
  • 52:14that it be test four or certainly
  • 52:17predicated termination on because
  • 52:18it makes no sense whatsoever.
  • 52:20These kids are.
  • 52:22Are phenomenal,
  • 52:23and they turn into phenomenal adults
  • 52:25and incredibly happy and productive.
  • 52:27They work hard, they struggle.
  • 52:29There's no question about it and
  • 52:31their parents struggle as well,
  • 52:32but they do universally, really well.
  • 52:36Well, I think that these kids also may
  • 52:39have an even brighter future based on
  • 52:40some of the work you've been doing.
  • 52:42Jeff, I mean the the ability to
  • 52:45identify these kids early and
  • 52:46really change a lot of lives.
  • 52:49I have a from auto Phoenix.
  • 52:51Ask says this is fascinating and so
  • 52:53so important that I'm thrilled that
  • 52:54you were driving to policy with this
  • 52:57for school aged children earlier than
  • 52:59reading failure is language delay.
  • 53:01Does any of the data point to a way to
  • 53:03screen genetically for this in order
  • 53:06to intervene even before school age?
  • 53:08If we could do newborn screening
  • 53:09for language delay,
  • 53:10we could start early
  • 53:12intervention immediately.
  • 53:13So I should pay you out 'cause that was like,
  • 53:16that's like the question about language
  • 53:18delay or specific language impairment and
  • 53:20reading disability comes up all the time.
  • 53:22Roughly, if you if you if you if
  • 53:26you do a really careful history on
  • 53:28children with reading disability,
  • 53:30about a third of those kids had had
  • 53:34very significant language delay,
  • 53:36that is delay in onset of language again
  • 53:38just to remind everybody I'm talking
  • 53:40about typically developing children.
  • 53:42Children with normal IQ's, but if they're
  • 53:44struggling and they clearly have,
  • 53:46you know, and they're tested,
  • 53:47and they really have a reading
  • 53:48disability if you do a careful history,
  • 53:50you'll find that roughly a third
  • 53:52of those kids.
  • 53:53Well, it had language impairment,
  • 53:54delayed onset and speech.
  • 53:5618 months, 22 months,
  • 53:5724 months even older than that,
  • 53:59there is a significant overlap and when
  • 54:01we look at the genetic association of
  • 54:04jeans that were primarily identified
  • 54:06for reading and then you go around and
  • 54:08you and you use those as candidate
  • 54:10genes for looking at language,
  • 54:11they overlap a great deal.
  • 54:13There are some unique genes specifically
  • 54:15for language impairment and there
  • 54:16are unique genes specifically
  • 54:18for reading disability,
  • 54:19but the overlap is significant
  • 54:20and that can explain why there's
  • 54:23the shared heritability.
  • 54:24If you go forward and you look
  • 54:26at kids who are diagnosed with
  • 54:28specific language impairment,
  • 54:29again,
  • 54:30about 20 to 30% of those kids will
  • 54:32get into trouble by the end of first
  • 54:34grade beginning of 2nd grade and will
  • 54:36have reading disability so those kids
  • 54:38have to be very carefully watched as
  • 54:40far as newborn screening is concerned.
  • 54:43When we ask the question a few years ago,
  • 54:46how many tests in a newborn
  • 54:47screening were done by DNA?
  • 54:49The answer was zero.
  • 54:51That's slowly changing,
  • 54:53and so DNA screening.
  • 54:55Not only is it potentially
  • 54:57incredibly sensitive and specific,
  • 55:00it's scaleable.
  • 55:01That means the cost can be incredibly
  • 55:04low and so that's what the hope is,
  • 55:07and so we're pursuing various ways
  • 55:11to get states to think about this,
  • 55:13or at least to fund this at
  • 55:15least as a pilot project.
  • 55:16But that's exactly right auto.
  • 55:18That's what we're trying to do.
  • 55:21I think we have time for one more question.
  • 55:23Jeff from Joe Abney.
  • 55:24Singer exciting work.
  • 55:25Have you moved the genetic testing
  • 55:27to the point of the test that has
  • 55:29a receiver operating curve that
  • 55:30provides data on sensitivity,
  • 55:32specificity, positive predictive
  • 55:33value and negative predictive value.
  • 55:36That's well, let me finish the question here.
  • 55:39Hold on to that since kids with
  • 55:40dyslexia are a heterogeneous group,
  • 55:43does your work focus on the most common
  • 55:45phenotype of phonological awareness,
  • 55:46or do you have data on the
  • 55:49orthographic variants?
  • 55:51OK, so let me attack the first
  • 55:52part of the question. First.
  • 55:54Joe, it's a great question.
  • 55:55Short answer is no,
  • 55:57and and the reason is because
  • 55:59in or certainly we could do it
  • 56:01retrospectively and when we do
  • 56:03we get a reasonable sensitivity
  • 56:05and incredibly good specificity.
  • 56:06But as far as a receiver operating
  • 56:08curve you really want to do that.
  • 56:10We could do that, you know,
  • 56:13looking backwards,
  • 56:13but the real way to do this would
  • 56:16be a uh as a looking forward that
  • 56:18is your identified or you just.
  • 56:21You take a cohort of children,
  • 56:22follow them for three to five
  • 56:24years and and and do testing,
  • 56:25and that's what the next stage is so we
  • 56:27can do that receiver operating curve.
  • 56:29So we're not there yet,
  • 56:30but I think we're getting
  • 56:32closer to being there.
  • 56:33As far as subtests are concerned,
  • 56:35this is the Holy Grail of neuropsychologists.
  • 56:37I get asked this question all the time.
  • 56:39Can you tell me?
  • 56:40Does the reading disability
  • 56:41that we're seeing in this child?
  • 56:43Is it more to do with orthographic coding?
  • 56:45Is more to do with phonological
  • 56:47awareness and and and so then the
  • 56:49intervention might be a little different
  • 56:51and I can tailor to the child.
  • 56:52We'd love to do that.
  • 56:54The thing to remember is that
  • 56:56there is no orthographic coding,
  • 56:58and for the the the for the non
  • 57:00informed of the group when we're
  • 57:02talking about is really spelling
  • 57:04there is no spelling gene.
  • 57:05There's no grammar gene,
  • 57:07there's no decoding gene.
  • 57:09Remember genes encode proteins,
  • 57:10proteins make up the receptors and
  • 57:13the neurotransmitters or the brain.
  • 57:15They make up the glea, the neurons, etc.
  • 57:17The workhorses of the cell,
  • 57:19and so they worked together
  • 57:21in combination with different
  • 57:22systems in order to enable us.
  • 57:24To do these very complex very human
  • 57:27things called decoding or reading
  • 57:29and spelling and music as well.
  • 57:32And so it it's not.
  • 57:33It's it probably isn't going
  • 57:35to be a single gene,
  • 57:36but it may be a panel of genes
  • 57:39or a panel a panoply perhaps,
  • 57:42or a profile of certain variants
  • 57:44that may be more weighted towards,
  • 57:46say, decoding versus spelling,
  • 57:48but we don't know yet.
  • 57:50The studies aren't large enough yet.
  • 57:53Well Jeff, you know you.
  • 57:54You left plenty of time for
  • 57:56Q&A and our time is up,
  • 57:58but I apologize to the many people.
  • 58:00There's a lot of enthusiasm for
  • 58:02this talk and many questions and
  • 58:03comments which we didn't get to.
  • 58:05So let me just congratulate
  • 58:06you on a marvelous talk and I
  • 58:09think that takes up our hour
  • 58:10and thanks everybody for coming.