Skip to Main Content

Surrogate Decision Making

February 16, 2023

January 18, 2023

Douglas White, MD, MAS

Vice Chair and Professor of Critical Care Medicine

UPMC Endowed Chair for Ethics in Critical Care Medicine

Director, Program on Ethics and Decision Making in Critical Illness
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine

ID
9487

Transcript

  • 00:00Good evening and welcome to the
  • 00:03Yale Program for Biomedical Ethics.
  • 00:05And welcome back to our home home
  • 00:06base here at Cohen.
  • 00:10My name is Mark material. I'm the
  • 00:11director of the program here at Yale
  • 00:13and I'm pleased you're here.
  • 00:15I thank you very much.
  • 00:16We have a great group here,
  • 00:19Cohen Auditorium and plenty
  • 00:20of folks on zoom as well
  • 00:23the the guest. Our guest this
  • 00:25evening is Doctor Doug White,
  • 00:26and I'll introduce him in
  • 00:27one second, but just to
  • 00:29let you know how it's going for those
  • 00:30of you who are new to our format,
  • 00:32I'll give a brief introduction.
  • 00:34Doctor White will speak on the
  • 00:36subject of surrogate decision
  • 00:37making for about 45 minutes.
  • 00:40Plus or minus, after that,
  • 00:41he and I are both going to
  • 00:43have a seat at the table here.
  • 00:44What we'll do is I'll take your questions
  • 00:46from the audience as well as online,
  • 00:48and I'll give them to Doctor White
  • 00:49and make sure they're difficult.
  • 00:50We didn't bring this guy here for easy
  • 00:52questions, but ask him some hard questions.
  • 00:55And and we'll have a conversation.
  • 00:58And that conversation will end.
  • 01:00That clock by the way as
  • 01:01you can see is an hour fast.
  • 01:03So we'll have a conversation
  • 01:04that will end at the at 6:30,
  • 01:07which is 730 and then pay no
  • 01:09attention to that the conversation
  • 01:10will end at 6:30 will have a hard
  • 01:13stop then until our next meeting.
  • 01:15I will tell you just one brief
  • 01:17announcement now for many of you who are
  • 01:19on is that I had sent out a notification.
  • 01:21I'm going to send out another one very soon.
  • 01:23A lot of very special lecture we're having
  • 01:25on the first Thomas Duffy MD Lecture and.
  • 01:28Now medical ethics,
  • 01:30in honor of our recently
  • 01:31deceased great friend Tom Duffy,
  • 01:34who was a stalwart at this meeting,
  • 01:36and in fact help us get this whole
  • 01:38program started now 13 years ago.
  • 01:41So this is going to be an honor of Tom.
  • 01:42And I sent out a notebook saying it was
  • 01:44April 6th, which is the 2nd day of Passover,
  • 01:47which has actually got some difficulty still
  • 01:49for some people. And unfortunately we
  • 01:51were able to change it to April 4th.
  • 01:53So we're going to be at the New Haven
  • 01:54Lawn Club with a reception from 4 to 5,
  • 01:57and I'm going to send you all
  • 01:58this in an e-mail as well.
  • 01:58From four to five at the New Haven Lawn Club.
  • 02:00We just have a reception some past orders
  • 02:03and such and somehow wine and beer.
  • 02:05And then we will we'll have a lecture
  • 02:09by Loris Caljan from University of Iowa.
  • 02:12He'll be speaking.
  • 02:12He'll be our guest speaker for the
  • 02:14first of what it's going to be
  • 02:16an annual event that Thomas Duffy
  • 02:17Lecture in medical ethics.
  • 02:18So that change of date we're we're is
  • 02:20now April 4th and look forward to that.
  • 02:23We're actually doing something.
  • 02:24I was talking to Anna Reisman,
  • 02:25I think you know about this Karen
  • 02:27from the program in Humanities.
  • 02:29Tom was very instrumental and
  • 02:30he was the leader of the program
  • 02:32and humanities for many years,
  • 02:33very involved in both.
  • 02:34So the two programs are going to
  • 02:36do something together next year.
  • 02:37We're going to have a,
  • 02:38we're going to have a program
  • 02:40next fall probably,
  • 02:40and there'll be more about that to come.
  • 02:42But stay tuned for now for tonight,
  • 02:45let's talk about surrogate decision making.
  • 02:47But before we do that,
  • 02:48let me talk about Doctor.
  • 02:49Doug White was kind enough
  • 02:51to come here and join us.
  • 02:52I think the first time we communicated
  • 02:55Doug was probably during the COVID,
  • 02:57the early days of COVID as we
  • 02:59were all trying to figure out.
  • 03:00How we were going to handle this.
  • 03:02And I see several of the guys
  • 03:03in the audience here,
  • 03:04Mark and Sarah and Jack and of course Ben,
  • 03:08who led our group from here and several
  • 03:09others who were involved in this.
  • 03:10And Doug and I think you and I
  • 03:12exchanged some ideas online and
  • 03:13got to know each other a bit.
  • 03:14So I was so pleased when you
  • 03:16agreed to come here.
  • 03:17Doctor White is the vice chair
  • 03:18and professor of critical care
  • 03:20medicine at the University of
  • 03:21Pittsburgh School of Medicine.
  • 03:23He holds the endowed Chair for Ethics
  • 03:25and Critical Care Medicine there and
  • 03:27directs the Program on Ethics and
  • 03:28Decision making and critical illness.
  • 03:30Which is the first program in
  • 03:32the country focused on ethical
  • 03:33issues and critical care medicine.
  • 03:35He graduated summa *** laude from Dartmouth.
  • 03:39He received his medical degree from UCSF,
  • 03:41did his residency in internal
  • 03:43Medicine and fellowship and pulmonary
  • 03:44and critical
  • 03:45Care medicine at UCSF. He also
  • 03:48completed a Masters in epidemiology and
  • 03:51Biostatistics and most importantly,
  • 03:53fellowship in bioethics. His scholarship
  • 03:57focuses on ethical issues that arise
  • 03:59in the care of critically ill patients.
  • 04:01And he is been funded by
  • 04:03Greenwall and other organizations.
  • 04:05He's widely recognized.
  • 04:06He's a fellow at the Hastings
  • 04:08Center and I could go on.
  • 04:10But suffice it to say, he's clearly
  • 04:11one of the leaders in medical ethics
  • 04:14in our country and particularly
  • 04:17in medical ethics as it relates
  • 04:18to care of critically ill patients. So
  • 04:21I want to welcome Doctor White.
  • 04:22Please come on up. I'm going to turn
  • 04:23it over to you. Thank you, Doug.
  • 04:29All right.
  • 04:34641.
  • 04:3636145 what is 36145?
  • 04:40OK. And can you put that on the channel?
  • 04:43CMU is 36145. What number do we call?
  • 04:514429435. It's also going to be available
  • 04:54in the back here. I'm going to
  • 04:55write on a big piece of paper and we'll
  • 04:56have both those pieces of Commission
  • 04:57for you then. Thanks, Karen. OK, wonderful.
  • 05:01It's nice to see a big crowd in person.
  • 05:05Mark, thanks for the Nice introduction.
  • 05:06And it's really wonderful
  • 05:07to be here in person.
  • 05:09This is one of the first in person
  • 05:11talks I've given since we've kind of
  • 05:14begun to emerge from the pandemic, so.
  • 05:17Here we are. All right,
  • 05:20let's let's talk title today
  • 05:22is surrogate decision making.
  • 05:23These are my disclosures
  • 05:25of financial disclosures,
  • 05:27none of which pose a conflict with
  • 05:29what I'll be presenting today.
  • 05:31And it looks like.
  • 05:32Is there a way to move the zoom stuff off
  • 05:35the top so people can see the titles?
  • 05:50Oh, right. Fantastic.
  • 05:52OK, we're getting there.
  • 05:58Maybe.
  • 06:02So I should just,
  • 06:03should I use the the mouse?
  • 06:05No. OK all right.
  • 06:07So I have three main objectives for today.
  • 06:11First, to summarize, several
  • 06:12important normative debates regarding
  • 06:14surrogate decision making in medical care.
  • 06:172nd, to address the implications of
  • 06:20several negative trials of interventions
  • 06:22to improve surrogate decision making and
  • 06:24ICU's and and I'm an adult intensivist,
  • 06:26so I'll be focusing.
  • 06:28Largely on the adult context,
  • 06:29but we'll do my best,
  • 06:31especially since we're in a child center to
  • 06:34talk somewhat about the pediatric context.
  • 06:36And then third,
  • 06:37to make the case that the way we've been
  • 06:40thinking about trying to improve surrogate
  • 06:42decision making in the acute care context is.
  • 06:45Perhaps not misguided,
  • 06:47but insufficiently multidimensional.
  • 06:51Some things that I won't be addressing fully,
  • 06:53we'll we'll touch on each of them a bit.
  • 06:55Legal aspects of surrogate decision making.
  • 06:57I'm not a lawyer.
  • 06:58I want to.
  • 06:59I put up here a great review by
  • 07:00FAD Pope on the legal aspects
  • 07:02of surrogate decision making,
  • 07:04responding to requests for futile or
  • 07:06potentially inappropriate treatment,
  • 07:07and making decisions for
  • 07:09unrepresented patients.
  • 07:09These are both topics
  • 07:10near and dear to my heart,
  • 07:11and I know Ben and others here
  • 07:14have been working on revising
  • 07:15Yale's policy for requesting,
  • 07:17responding to such requests.
  • 07:19Perhaps we can address these
  • 07:20in the discussion.
  • 07:22Section.
  • 07:24OK,
  • 07:24so let's start with the normative
  • 07:26aspects of surrogate decision making.
  • 07:29Sometimes when I'm attending in the ICU,
  • 07:33I hear rumblings from clinical staff
  • 07:37or trainees along these lines.
  • 07:39You know, families that don't,
  • 07:41they don't really know what the patient
  • 07:43would choose and they don't really
  • 07:45understand what's going on with them.
  • 07:47So they really shouldn't
  • 07:48be the decision maker.
  • 07:49And if they're really, you know,
  • 07:51if these are sort of high level trainees,
  • 07:54maybe internal medicine residents,
  • 07:55they might bring up paper to that
  • 07:57effect and one of the common ones that.
  • 07:59That is bandied about on rounds is
  • 08:01out of David Wendler's group at NIH,
  • 08:03David Shalowitz Levitt.
  • 08:05It was a systematic review in which
  • 08:08they looked at all the studies of.
  • 08:11Decisions that patients made and their
  • 08:13appointed proxies or their next of
  • 08:16kin and compared them and what they
  • 08:18found is that surrogates did not
  • 08:20accurately predict what the patient
  • 08:22would choose about 1/3 of the time.
  • 08:24So they use this as evidence that
  • 08:28these individuals that patients,
  • 08:30families should not be the surrogate
  • 08:32decision makers or the way we have
  • 08:34it all structured is misguided.
  • 08:35So I'm going to stop here just to sort
  • 08:37of gauge people's thoughts about that,
  • 08:39how first of all have show up.
  • 08:41Chance is anyone? Had such murmurings.
  • 08:44When you're clinically on service,
  • 08:45have you heard people say such things?
  • 08:47Yeah, OK.
  • 08:48It looks like about half of the room.
  • 08:50I don't know how many are clinicians
  • 08:52and and how does it strike you?
  • 08:54Does it seem like a?
  • 08:56Reasonable conclusion?
  • 09:00Yeah, well.
  • 09:04Some of the time. OK,
  • 09:07from the time and in some way.
  • 09:11All right. Sarah,
  • 09:14I would argue, I mean at least
  • 09:16the way it's framed here,
  • 09:17it's kind of problematic
  • 09:19because they're the first part,
  • 09:21families don't know what
  • 09:22a patient would choose.
  • 09:23The the the justification for
  • 09:25that is that they don't understand
  • 09:26these complex medical issues.
  • 09:28But that actually is really underscoring
  • 09:30the lack of understanding of the medical
  • 09:32issues and not the patients wishes.
  • 09:34And by that argument you would almost say,
  • 09:36well patients shouldn't
  • 09:37choose for themselves.
  • 09:38And I think we have a pretty robust
  • 09:40consensus that patients should have the
  • 09:42autonomy to choose and in fact this.
  • 09:44Is more from my perspective of failure
  • 09:46of medical teams to break down those
  • 09:48complex medical issues in a way that
  • 09:51families actually can process them.
  • 09:53Others, yeah.
  • 09:56See partment perspective where I work.
  • 10:00It seems as though that the patients
  • 10:02families don't always understand the
  • 10:04complex medical issues for two reasons.
  • 10:06One because it hasn't been
  • 10:08explained by the oncologist or the,
  • 10:10you know, the heart failure doctors
  • 10:12or the liver physicians.
  • 10:14Umm.
  • 10:16Or things are different today
  • 10:17because that's why the patients
  • 10:19in the emergency department.
  • 10:20So they may be our job to explain
  • 10:22that things are different now,
  • 10:24but sometimes have a hard conversation
  • 10:26when you've just met the family issue.
  • 10:28Yeah, so I'm hearing two
  • 10:29different issues here. One that.
  • 10:33Yes, this is true and it may be
  • 10:35an issue that that patients can't,
  • 10:38that surrogates can't fully and reliably
  • 10:40predict what a patient would choose.
  • 10:42And sometimes that's grounded in
  • 10:44just lack of understanding of the
  • 10:46facts and but for that they might
  • 10:48actually be able to to make the the
  • 10:50same predictions the patient would.
  • 10:51However, I I think it's worth really
  • 10:53sort of stepping back and asking
  • 10:55what's the moral basis for families
  • 10:57as surrogate decision makers?
  • 10:58And is it this narrow because if so,
  • 11:00it's it's easily defeated, yeah.
  • 11:03If you just have these empirical
  • 11:04data that surrogates make decisions
  • 11:05different from the ones that the patients
  • 11:07would choose and the moral basis for
  • 11:09the family's role, is that then?
  • 11:12It's defeated,
  • 11:13but let's let's look at it.
  • 11:14OK, so Dan Brock,
  • 11:18we'll talk about some of Dan's
  • 11:20work later at other work.
  • 11:22But Dan I wrote what I think
  • 11:24is a lovely piece Now 20,
  • 11:26almost 30 years ago articulating 5 grounds,
  • 11:31moral grounds for the the family
  • 11:32of surrogate decision maker.
  • 11:34And he divided these into patient
  • 11:36regarding considerations,
  • 11:37non patient regarding considerations
  • 11:38and what he called democratic grounds.
  • 11:41Only one of these has to do with accuracy,
  • 11:44OK,
  • 11:44and that's the the second bullet
  • 11:46under patient regarding rounds.
  • 11:47The other four are reasons that
  • 11:49really don't hinge on family's
  • 11:51ability to accurately predict 1st.
  • 11:53And let me also say that these
  • 11:57justifications don't apply to all cases
  • 12:00or all decisions in individual cases.
  • 12:03And yet they provide what I think
  • 12:04is a broader justification for why
  • 12:06we often default to surrogates
  • 12:08when when patient or default to
  • 12:09families when patients haven't
  • 12:11appointed the surrogate first if
  • 12:13the patient has appointed a surrogate.
  • 12:18A family member is a surrogate.
  • 12:19We respect the surrogates.
  • 12:21Are the patients right to self-determination
  • 12:24by going with who they identified?
  • 12:27Second, there is some thought that the
  • 12:29patients family knows them best and
  • 12:31is best able to speak to their values.
  • 12:34I would just parenthetically add there
  • 12:36are no studies that show that doctors
  • 12:38are more accurate than families, OK.
  • 12:39So if your claim is that,
  • 12:41well families just aren't accurate enough,
  • 12:44therefore doctors should make
  • 12:45the decisions then that would.
  • 12:47Require data that doctors actually
  • 12:49do predict patients preferences
  • 12:50better than families doing,
  • 12:52and that data does not exist
  • 12:54or those data don't exist.
  • 12:55So that's the the second category is
  • 12:57the non patient regarding grounds and
  • 12:59so he argued that distributive justice
  • 13:02requires consideration of the effects
  • 13:04of decisions on others particularly
  • 13:05the family who are likely to bear the
  • 13:07the greatest consequences of decisions
  • 13:09other than the patient themselves.
  • 13:11And one example of this and this is
  • 13:13actually one we've been dealing with
  • 13:15is a family recently is consider
  • 13:16the the case of a of a patient with
  • 13:18advanced dementia and the decision
  • 13:19is coming up whether to keep them at
  • 13:21home or put them in an assisted living
  • 13:23facility or a memory care center.
  • 13:26It is the people who will be caring
  • 13:28for that patient at home whose lives
  • 13:30will be substantially altered by
  • 13:31being a 36 hour a day caregiver.
  • 13:33And so the argument here would be that
  • 13:36those individuals have some claim
  • 13:38to those decisions and some claim to
  • 13:40involvement because of the impact on them.
  • 13:433rd or 4th.
  • 13:47Using families of decision makers
  • 13:50reinforces the importance in Society of the
  • 13:53family unit that much of society hinges
  • 13:55on the work and the role of families.
  • 13:57And this is something that society
  • 13:58has an interest in fostering.
  • 14:00And to do so we need to have a
  • 14:02allow some space for families
  • 14:04to have autonomy as a unit.
  • 14:06So so goes the argument.
  • 14:07And then finally the the the 5th
  • 14:10ground that Dan laid out was simply.
  • 14:12To adhere all of the things being equal
  • 14:14to the the norms established by democratic
  • 14:17political process in the United States.
  • 14:19That democratic political process
  • 14:21has said that families should be the
  • 14:24surrogate unless someone else has been
  • 14:26appointed directed by the patient.
  • 14:29All right,
  • 14:29so I I want to point out though that
  • 14:32family as surrogate is not a universal
  • 14:34norm and not even a norm in Western,
  • 14:37not even a universal norm
  • 14:38in in Western countries.
  • 14:40And so this is a list put together by
  • 14:44Alexander Letrent and France of who has
  • 14:47decisional authority when a patient
  • 14:49is hospitalized and incapacitated in France,
  • 14:52England, Germany,
  • 14:53Netherlands and Switzerland.
  • 14:55In two situations, one,
  • 14:56when the patient appointed
  • 14:57a surrogate and two,
  • 14:58when the patient did not.
  • 14:59And what I think is really
  • 15:01interesting here is that France
  • 15:03and this is I actually confirmed
  • 15:05this last week because it's still,
  • 15:07I still find it hard to believe,
  • 15:09but the physician is this the
  • 15:11surrogate of record even when the
  • 15:13patient has appointed someone else.
  • 15:15The final decision authority in
  • 15:17France is the physician in the
  • 15:19in the acute care context,
  • 15:21OK,
  • 15:21that's if the patient has appointed
  • 15:23a surrogate and if the patient
  • 15:25did not appoint a surrogate.
  • 15:26Also in England,
  • 15:27Germany and the Netherlands,
  • 15:28if the patient did not authorize using a,
  • 15:31you know,
  • 15:32a some legal form that each country
  • 15:35uses who the surrogate would be,
  • 15:38then it again defaults to the physician.
  • 15:40They need to consult with the family,
  • 15:42but the final decision authority is
  • 15:44the physicians and then Switzerland has
  • 15:46norms that approximate in United States.
  • 15:52A second normative, sort of thorny
  • 15:54normative issue is these legally designated
  • 15:57default hierarchies of surrogates.
  • 16:00And so if I understand Connecticut law
  • 16:02correctly, and please please correct me
  • 16:04if I'm if I'm getting it wrong, if the
  • 16:06patient has not doesn't need a surrogate,
  • 16:08then this is the default hierarchy.
  • 16:10The spouse would be the surrogate.
  • 16:11If there's no spouse, the adult child,
  • 16:13and on down the list parent,
  • 16:14adult sibling, and grandparent.
  • 16:15And I I the the most recent version of
  • 16:18Connecticut law I looked at did not have.
  • 16:20Depends on that list.
  • 16:21Is that still the case?
  • 16:22Friends are not on the the legal hierarchy.
  • 16:25OK, so really important and 35
  • 16:28states have a hierarchy that
  • 16:30looks something like this 15 go.
  • 16:33I trained in California.
  • 16:34We did not have a default list of surrogates.
  • 16:37So when a patient had not appointed
  • 16:40someone that the work of the clinical
  • 16:42team was to get consensus among
  • 16:44all family members who were there.
  • 16:46And so this is a really,
  • 16:47I think an important issue.
  • 16:49I want to flag that I recognize that it
  • 16:52having a default list is is pragmatic,
  • 16:55it's sufficient,
  • 16:56it allows the hospital to to run quickly
  • 16:58in in cases to go more smoothly.
  • 17:00But there are some real downsides
  • 17:02to it and I I experienced or or
  • 17:05saw the potential harms that could
  • 17:08have come from from this.
  • 17:10In my work at San Francisco
  • 17:11General during the AIDS epidemic.
  • 17:13And one of those risks is that there is
  • 17:17no guarantee that the person highest
  • 17:19on the list they have the closest
  • 17:21love relationship with the patient or
  • 17:23the OR will be best able to represent
  • 17:26their deepest values and preferences.
  • 17:28And this came out most clearly with gay men
  • 17:31who were being admitted to the ICU with
  • 17:33advanced new assistance printing anemonia.
  • 17:35You know,
  • 17:36this is at the time essentially
  • 17:39lethal they had committed.
  • 17:40Partners who?
  • 17:41This is before same sex
  • 17:42marriage in California.
  • 17:43So those partners were considered
  • 17:47friends and I would note in Connecticut
  • 17:50they would be the the lowest on
  • 17:52the hierarchy and not infrequently.
  • 17:54These young men's parents would have
  • 17:57very different value systems from
  • 17:59their patients and would would say
  • 18:01things like because our son is gay,
  • 18:03he's going to hell. We love our son.
  • 18:06We don't want him to go to hell.
  • 18:07So continue life support so that we can
  • 18:10keep him from going to hell and that is,
  • 18:13you know obviously not a decision
  • 18:15that is presumably grounded in the
  • 18:18patients values and preferences.
  • 18:20So tricky issue,
  • 18:21what to do with these hierarchies but
  • 18:23I just want to flag coming from a.
  • 18:25They that did not have these hierarchies
  • 18:27and now practicing in one that does that.
  • 18:29I think there are real
  • 18:30concerns with hierarchies,
  • 18:31to the extent that they make assumptions
  • 18:33that are often true but not always true,
  • 18:35about who's best position to
  • 18:37advocate for a patient and represent
  • 18:39their values and preferences.
  • 18:43All right, so moving from who should
  • 18:45be making the decisions to how
  • 18:46should they make the the decisions?
  • 18:48What evidentiary standards
  • 18:49should be used for surrogates?
  • 18:51Should, should surrogates
  • 18:52used to make decisions.
  • 18:53And Dan Brock and Alan Buchanan
  • 18:56have have done some of the the
  • 18:58most seminal work on this.
  • 19:00They're their recommendations in this book,
  • 19:02deciding for others,
  • 19:03form the the basis for the AMA Code
  • 19:06of Ethics on surrogate decision
  • 19:07making and many state laws.
  • 19:09Now I would if anyone is really
  • 19:11interested in surrogate decision making.
  • 19:13Just deciding for others is a must read book.
  • 19:17So they articulated at what what is
  • 19:19called the tripartite evidentiary
  • 19:21standard lexically ordered.
  • 19:23So stated wishes substituted judgment
  • 19:25and the best interest standard is this.
  • 19:27Have folks heard of this before?
  • 19:29Yeah most OK let me just for those
  • 19:31of you who haven't let me just
  • 19:32walk you through it.
  • 19:33The idea here is that you you should use
  • 19:35the highest standard that is feasible
  • 19:38given the information available.
  • 19:39So the the stated wishes standard is that
  • 19:42you should enact the treatment preferences.
  • 19:44That were previously formulated and
  • 19:46somehow communicated by the patient,
  • 19:48preferably in writing,
  • 19:49but verbal or oral directives are
  • 19:52accepted in many states as well.
  • 19:53And the idea here is that
  • 19:55the preference it's not,
  • 19:57it can't just be a vague broad preference,
  • 19:59it needs to be specific to the
  • 20:01actual clinical decision at hand.
  • 20:03And so,
  • 20:03so the work done is just the
  • 20:05surrogate stating what the patient
  • 20:07had said would be their preference
  • 20:10in the circumstance they're in.
  • 20:12If that information is not available,
  • 20:13if the patient did not articulate
  • 20:15clear preferences,
  • 20:15then the next standard is what's called
  • 20:17the substituted judgment standard.
  • 20:19And here are the work of the surrogate
  • 20:21is to beechman shoulders call it donning
  • 20:23the mental mantle of the patient,
  • 20:25taking on the persona of the patient
  • 20:27to try to make the the decision that
  • 20:30patient would have made had they
  • 20:32actually articulated a decision.
  • 20:34And then if, if the surrogate says,
  • 20:36listen, that's just not that's,
  • 20:38I can't do that.
  • 20:39I don't know the patient well enough,
  • 20:41then the the third standard and
  • 20:43the one that that the sorry would
  • 20:45be asked to use is what's called
  • 20:46the best interest standard.
  • 20:48And this is where in the absence of
  • 20:50clear knowledge of what the patient
  • 20:51would have chosen to make the decision
  • 20:53that best promotes the patient's interests.
  • 20:55And we'll come back to this in a moment.
  • 20:58And as I said,
  • 20:59this is the the evidentiary standard
  • 21:01and the the norms in the United States.
  • 21:06And so I I briefly touched on this,
  • 21:08but I want to come back to this.
  • 21:10Pointing out that the work that we
  • 21:12ask surrogates to do according to each
  • 21:14of these standards is quite different.
  • 21:16When the work that we asked
  • 21:19surrogates to do when it is stated
  • 21:22wishes standard is simply to parrot
  • 21:24and report what the patient said.
  • 21:27So there's not,
  • 21:28it's not really a judgment role,
  • 21:30that's a reporting role,
  • 21:32arguably much easier and morally.
  • 21:35That's burdensome to just say, well,
  • 21:37here's what he said that he would
  • 21:40want in this clinical circumstance.
  • 21:42But the substituted judgment standard
  • 21:43in the best interest standard,
  • 21:45both rely on the family exercising
  • 21:47judgment about either what would the
  • 21:50patient choose or what's in their best
  • 21:52interest since they didn't choose.
  • 21:54And so these are, you know,
  • 21:56this is really different cognitive work.
  • 21:58On the one hand,
  • 21:59they're just a mouthpiece or a
  • 22:01communicator of preferences,
  • 22:02and the other,
  • 22:03they are actually exercising a good deal
  • 22:05of judgment about morally weighty decisions.
  • 22:08And this is where I think empirical evidence
  • 22:10is actually quite important to figure out.
  • 22:12You know,
  • 22:12what do we need to be able to do to help
  • 22:16surrogates really function in the role, so.
  • 22:20If you look at sort of empirically
  • 22:22how many adults have made have
  • 22:24completed an advanced directive,
  • 22:25only a third.
  • 22:26So that means 2/3 of patients at
  • 22:28least haven't created it and have no
  • 22:31chance of having a written document
  • 22:34that satisfies the stated wishes.
  • 22:36Criterion or or standard.
  • 22:39In addition,
  • 22:40those who articulate or or
  • 22:42complete advanced directives,
  • 22:44remember most advanced directives
  • 22:45are these boilerplate directives that
  • 22:47really only apply in the clinical
  • 22:49circumstances of a terminal illness
  • 22:51or a persistent vegetative state.
  • 22:52And most patients who wind up in the
  • 22:54hospital are not in one of those two states.
  • 22:56So most advanced directives are
  • 22:58not dispositive for what treatment
  • 23:00decisions should be made.
  • 23:02OK and then we we also looked at this,
  • 23:05we have a we.
  • 23:07A multicenter study about 10 years
  • 23:09ago in which we audio recorded
  • 23:11250 ICU family conversations about
  • 23:13goals of care for the interest and
  • 23:15the focus was really looking at how
  • 23:17doctors are talking about prognosis
  • 23:19and what families understand.
  • 23:21But I had a trainee who did a
  • 23:24secondary analysis because he was
  • 23:26really interested in answering this
  • 23:28question of what often are the the.
  • 23:31Or how often is the information offered
  • 23:33by the family in these meetings
  • 23:36clearly dispositive for the decision at hand,
  • 23:38which is to say there is no
  • 23:40interpretation or judgment required
  • 23:41by the surrogate or the doctrine.
  • 23:43The family just have to say,
  • 23:45well, that, you know,
  • 23:46dad's in the ICU with an acute
  • 23:48stroke on a ventilator, hemiparetic,
  • 23:51very likely to have substantial neurologic
  • 23:54deficits and and hemiplegia and oh,
  • 23:56by the way,
  • 23:57he has an advanced directive that
  • 23:59says if he's in an ICU with a stroke.
  • 24:01Can we predict do not continue life
  • 24:03support and so how often did that
  • 24:05happen less than 1% of the time.
  • 24:07There were actually a couple decisions
  • 24:09in this in this 250 patient cohort
  • 24:11that where it really did seem that
  • 24:13the information from the family that
  • 24:15that the patient had reported to them
  • 24:17was was spot on with the clinical decision.
  • 24:20But 99% of the time not the case.
  • 24:22And Joan Tino in the support trial
  • 24:24that we'll talk about in a couple
  • 24:26of minutes also looked at this
  • 24:27and found that about 3% of decision, 3% of.
  • 24:31Patients advanced directives.
  • 24:34Actually had this positive
  • 24:35treatment information in the
  • 24:375000 patient support cohort.
  • 24:39So again, really, really low numbers.
  • 24:4197% of the time we're going to be asking
  • 24:44surrogates to be exercising judgment
  • 24:47about really morally weighty decisions.
  • 24:49And again this is all the adult context.
  • 24:51I want to flag that the only relevant
  • 24:54criterion for the pediatric context
  • 24:56is the best interest standard, right,
  • 24:58pediatric patients with the, you know,
  • 25:01carving out the exception for minors or
  • 25:04for sorry for adolescents that patients?
  • 25:07Generally, are not able to form values
  • 25:09and preferences and therefore the stated
  • 25:11wishes standard doesn't really apply.
  • 25:13Similarly, since they have not
  • 25:16really formulated preferences,
  • 25:18the the substituted judging
  • 25:20standard also would not apply.
  • 25:22All right, so.
  • 25:23What about these these standards we'll
  • 25:25we'll set aside the stated wishes standard.
  • 25:28There are a host of empirical
  • 25:30problems with that standard.
  • 25:31Whether the the stated preferences
  • 25:33are are actually well informed,
  • 25:35whether they're contemporary.
  • 25:36You know the advance directive was 20
  • 25:38years ago and now the decision is at hand.
  • 25:40But let's just focus on the the
  • 25:42three main normative critiques of
  • 25:44the substituted judgment standard
  • 25:45and the best interest standard.
  • 25:47And these are they.
  • 25:48So the first is that the SJ,
  • 25:51the substituted judgment standard,
  • 25:52is a cruel charade and.
  • 25:54Yeah,
  • 25:55dancel Macy and Dan Brudney
  • 25:56have both written,
  • 25:58I think they were both at
  • 25:59Chicago at the same time.
  • 26:00And both have written, I think,
  • 26:01really nicely about this.
  • 26:03And they argued that asking what
  • 26:05a patient would choose when the
  • 26:07patient has not actually chosen may
  • 26:09distress surrogates and mistakenly
  • 26:11focuses on promoting autonomy,
  • 26:13which is not possible when the
  • 26:15patient hasn't chosen something.
  • 26:16You can't promote self-determination
  • 26:18or autonomy if there's been
  • 26:19no autonomous choice.
  • 26:20And instead they recommend
  • 26:22that clinicians should seek.
  • 26:24Not to unearth the patients
  • 26:27unknown treatment preferences,
  • 26:29but instead they're to unearth
  • 26:30their broader values and to make
  • 26:32right and for the clinicians then
  • 26:34to make recommendations that
  • 26:36promote authentic choices,
  • 26:37those that really represent the
  • 26:40patients commitments and values.
  • 26:42So that's the that's the first critique
  • 26:45of the substituted judgment standard.
  • 26:47And then there are two critiques
  • 26:48of the best interest standard,
  • 26:49the 1st and Doug Dekama has has,
  • 26:52I think most eloquently put forward this
  • 26:54issue is that the best interest standard,
  • 26:56taken literally,
  • 26:57is unfairly demanding,
  • 26:59which is to say,
  • 27:01must parents,
  • 27:02let's just take a pediatric example,
  • 27:03must parents actually do what is
  • 27:05literally in the best interest of
  • 27:07their child when making decisions?
  • 27:09And he gives the example of.
  • 27:12Imagine a parent with multiple
  • 27:13children and what is best for one
  • 27:16child actually creates a problem
  • 27:17for another child such that you
  • 27:19can't simultaneously promote the
  • 27:21best interest of both children.
  • 27:23It's an impossible standard and
  • 27:25in some circumstances and and
  • 27:27Doug Diekema argued that instead
  • 27:29we instead of literally trying to
  • 27:31achieve what's best for for children,
  • 27:34that the threshold that we should
  • 27:35use for when we
  • 27:37question or seek to override parents
  • 27:39is a harm threshold when the
  • 27:41decisions that they're articulating.
  • 27:43Create a substantial risk
  • 27:44of harm for the patient,
  • 27:45not is this the the absolute
  • 27:48best decision for the patient?
  • 27:50And then finally,
  • 27:51Zika Emanuel has articulated an argument
  • 27:54against the best interest standard,
  • 27:56which is essentially that it's
  • 27:57vacuous or empty. That it's it's,
  • 27:59it's a there's there's nothing there.
  • 28:01And it's highly problematic in
  • 28:03a in a pluralistic society.
  • 28:04The idea here is that the best
  • 28:07interest standard to be meaningful,
  • 28:08one has to infuse it with some
  • 28:11conception of the good that is widely
  • 28:13held by citizens in a society.
  • 28:15And when you live in a pluralistic society,
  • 28:17that's actually not possible so.
  • 28:20That one way Zeke did this
  • 28:21that I thought was really nice,
  • 28:22is he laid out different conceptions
  • 28:24of the good and he talked about of
  • 28:26patients who were vitalists and talked
  • 28:27about patients who are rugged individualists.
  • 28:30Vitalist believes that that
  • 28:32life should be continued.
  • 28:35If you can continue it for a millisecond,
  • 28:37it should be continued because the
  • 28:39value of life is merely being alive.
  • 28:41That that's all that is required
  • 28:43for a meaningful life.
  • 28:44The rugged individualist.
  • 28:45And that's, yeah,
  • 28:46I think he used a different name.
  • 28:47They are the autonomist believes
  • 28:49that the value in life.
  • 28:50Hinges on on being able to make
  • 28:52one's own decisions and be an
  • 28:54autonomous agent in the world.
  • 28:56And once that's lost,
  • 28:58then the good in that life is also lost.
  • 29:01Those two individuals that their
  • 29:02best interest when when they're
  • 29:04facing medical decisions would
  • 29:05be quite different according to
  • 29:07their very different values.
  • 29:09And so seeks claim,
  • 29:11is that we can't really formulate
  • 29:13anything substantive about a best interest
  • 29:16standard and in a pluralistic society.
  • 29:20OK. Stepping back from the tripartite
  • 29:24standards, I also again want to
  • 29:26point out that these standards,
  • 29:27this is the the these are the
  • 29:30norms in the United States.
  • 29:32They are not the norms elsewhere.
  • 29:34And. One place in particular
  • 29:36where I think this has come across
  • 29:39clearest is in the United Kingdom.
  • 29:42This is a document that the British
  • 29:44Medical Association released in 2019
  • 29:46and it's titled Best Interest Decision
  • 29:48making for adults who lack capacity.
  • 29:50Their framework is all about best interests,
  • 29:53though they have a broad
  • 29:54conception of best interests.
  • 29:55Which is to say, well,
  • 29:57I'll talk about it a second.
  • 29:58But they they have a 2 standard framework
  • 30:00for how surrogates should make decisions.
  • 30:02One is the stated preferences standard,
  • 30:04akin to the stated wishes.
  • 30:05Standards of Brock and Buchanan
  • 30:07and if that's not present,
  • 30:09if they're if there are no
  • 30:10directives that are that clear,
  • 30:12then it's a best interest standard,
  • 30:13and they articulate this
  • 30:14in the following way.
  • 30:16Clinicians,
  • 30:16when making the best,
  • 30:17when making best interest judgments,
  • 30:19must consider the person's welfare in
  • 30:22the pot and the widest possible sense,
  • 30:23and consider the individual's broader
  • 30:25wishes and feelings and values and beliefs.
  • 30:27All decisions should follow careful
  • 30:29consideration of the individual
  • 30:31circumstances of the patient and
  • 30:32focus on reaching the decision
  • 30:34that is right for that person.
  • 30:35Not what is best for for those around them,
  • 30:38other regarding considerations or
  • 30:40what the reasonable person would want.
  • 30:43So this is the the UK approach.
  • 30:46I think it's just valuable to see that
  • 30:48they've entirely done away with the
  • 30:50substituted judgment standard or or
  • 30:52have just chosen not to include it.
  • 30:54Although interestingly the
  • 30:55substituted judgment standard arose
  • 30:57out of English case law,
  • 31:00so came from England,
  • 31:01they no longer use it and yet we have
  • 31:03we have decided to continue to use it.
  • 31:07OK, that I'm going to stop there
  • 31:09with the normative aspects of this
  • 31:11and and make an empirical turn.
  • 31:12I I would hope that we can come back to
  • 31:14some of these narrative issues that are
  • 31:16quite thorny in the discussion section.
  • 31:18But I want to talk now about work
  • 31:20that has been done to try to improve
  • 31:23the process of surrogate decision
  • 31:25making for seriously ill adults.
  • 31:27Because I think there are quite a few
  • 31:29lessons learned, lessons to be learned,
  • 31:30not only about how to design
  • 31:32better interventions,
  • 31:33but also how to think about training
  • 31:36the next generation of clinicians.
  • 31:38So the first trial that I want to
  • 31:40talk about is the support trial
  • 31:42and this was a really, you know,
  • 31:45you often hear the support trial
  • 31:47comma a landmark trial.
  • 31:48It really was a landmark trial.
  • 31:50It was a cluster randomized trial done
  • 31:52before people even knew what a cluster
  • 31:54randomized trial was of about 5000
  • 31:56seriously ill patients with an end
  • 31:58stage condition admitted to the hospital.
  • 32:00And the goal was to try to to try to
  • 32:02improve the end of life care and the
  • 32:04decisions made by those patient surrogates,
  • 32:06what the intervention entailed.
  • 32:07Was a what was called the support
  • 32:10Nurse giving every day or every every
  • 32:12several days giving physicians in
  • 32:14the intervention arm estimates of the
  • 32:17patient's likelihood of six months survival,
  • 32:19they're likely survival from
  • 32:20CPR and then their likelihood of
  • 32:22functional disability at two months
  • 32:24using validated models that have
  • 32:26been developed in the cohort.
  • 32:27And then the support nurse also went
  • 32:29to the the patients families and worked
  • 32:32to elicit patients preferences around
  • 32:34use of life support and code status
  • 32:36and then share this information.
  • 32:38Of the clinical team.
  • 32:39So really trying to get at let's
  • 32:41make sure the doctors have good
  • 32:43medical information about prognosis.
  • 32:45Let's make sure that that we talk
  • 32:47to families about the patients
  • 32:48values and preferences and get this
  • 32:50information back to the doctor.
  • 32:51So I think in many ways a thoughtful
  • 32:54intervention that was entirely negative,
  • 32:57no effect on end of life care or cost.
  • 32:59These are just.
  • 33:01The main outcome measures they
  • 33:02looked at no difference in the
  • 33:04median time until a DNR order was
  • 33:06written among dying patients.
  • 33:07No difference in DNR agreement,
  • 33:09which is to say did the doctor
  • 33:10know what the patient would want
  • 33:12in terms of their code status.
  • 33:13No difference in the in patients dying,
  • 33:17I think it was after more than seven
  • 33:19days in an intensive care unit.
  • 33:20No difference in the proportion of
  • 33:22patients dying in pain and no difference
  • 33:24in the use in the overall healthcare
  • 33:27costs associated with these patients care.
  • 33:29So a stone cold negative trial.
  • 33:32And what I think is important when you
  • 33:34look at the the conceptual or theoretical
  • 33:36underpinnings of the support trial,
  • 33:38it was explicitly grounded in what's
  • 33:40called expected utility theory or or
  • 33:43traditional decision analytical theory.
  • 33:45An easy way to think about
  • 33:46this is it's essentially
  • 33:48the rational actor model that that says
  • 33:50with good information about outcomes of
  • 33:53of treatment and the values that of that
  • 33:56treatment to patients and their families.
  • 33:59This will reliably lead to good
  • 34:01decisions that will be utility
  • 34:02maximizing that will be logical.
  • 34:04They'll be rational.
  • 34:06And that's was obviously not the case.
  • 34:09I think the, the the clinical model that
  • 34:12this follows or a way to think about this,
  • 34:15the mindset is that really
  • 34:17of informed choice is that.
  • 34:19What a doctor needs to do is simply to
  • 34:21make sure that the patient's family
  • 34:23understands the patient's prognosis,
  • 34:25understands the patient's values,
  • 34:26understands how to apply those values
  • 34:28to the decision at hand and understands
  • 34:30the the available treatment options.
  • 34:31If that's done,
  • 34:32then the family should reliably
  • 34:34come to good decisions.
  • 34:36And I think what we saw here is that at least
  • 34:38in the support trial that was not the case,
  • 34:40that that there there are a number
  • 34:42of assumptions embedded in there
  • 34:43that did not prove to be true.
  • 34:45Now you might say,
  • 34:46listen, that was.
  • 34:4825 years ago called the the norms
  • 34:50of medicine were very different
  • 34:51in terms of end of life care.
  • 34:53So maybe it's not a fair test but I
  • 34:55want to present another trial done
  • 34:56now just in the last five years that
  • 34:59I was part of and Chris Cox led this
  • 35:01was using we developed a very slick
  • 35:04electronic web-based decision aid
  • 35:06for the surrogate decision makers
  • 35:08of patients receiving prolonged
  • 35:10mechanical ventilation and ICU's
  • 35:12again adult patients had been on
  • 35:15the vent for longer than 10 days.
  • 35:17Uh,
  • 35:18this was an RCT in 13 US ICU 277 patients,
  • 35:22and the intervention was a decision
  • 35:26aid that adhered to existing
  • 35:28standards for for decision aids.
  • 35:30And it helped families understand what it
  • 35:33means to have prolonged chemical ventilation.
  • 35:36It helped them understand
  • 35:37what a ventilator is,
  • 35:38what a tracheostomy is it.
  • 35:40It exposed them to the possibility
  • 35:43of different treatment options,
  • 35:45which is to say ongoing intensive
  • 35:47treatment with tracheostomy.
  • 35:48A full comfort focused plan of
  • 35:49care or a time limited trial?
  • 35:51Those were the three options that
  • 35:53were laid out and then it it through a
  • 35:56series of interactive exercises asked
  • 35:58families to sort of think through
  • 35:59what are the patients values and
  • 36:01preferences that would be relevant
  • 36:02to this decision and then give a
  • 36:04number of tips for how to communicate
  • 36:06about this with practical team.
  • 36:08So again,
  • 36:08really grounded in sort of the
  • 36:10rational actor model,
  • 36:12but a much more modern spin on it.
  • 36:14And what we found unfortunately was
  • 36:16again a stone cold negative trial
  • 36:18that the intervention had no effect
  • 36:20on overall healthcare utilization
  • 36:22or decisions to transition to
  • 36:24comfort focused care,
  • 36:26no effect on surrogate psychological
  • 36:28outcomes and problematically no
  • 36:30effect on surrogates perceptions
  • 36:32of the patients prognosis.
  • 36:34What we found is that it surrogates
  • 36:37in the intervention arm remained
  • 36:39overly optimistic about the
  • 36:41patient's likelihood of one
  • 36:43year survival estimating it.
  • 36:45Or reporting a mean 90%
  • 36:46chance of 1 one year survival compared to
  • 36:49the model drive number of 56% chances.
  • 36:51And then the other thing that
  • 36:53was also I think revealing and
  • 36:55concerning and this parenthetically,
  • 36:57This is why it's really important to
  • 36:59do good process evaluations as part
  • 37:02of behavioral health interventions.
  • 37:04We found that surrogates were 43% of
  • 37:08them favored a treatment option that was
  • 37:10more aggressive than what they reported
  • 37:12the patient would choose for him or herself.
  • 37:15So they are systematic.
  • 37:16We ask them, you know,
  • 37:17what do you think the patient would choose
  • 37:19and what do you think we should do here.
  • 37:21And 43% of the time the surrogate was
  • 37:23picking something more aggressive
  • 37:24than what they thought the patient
  • 37:25would choose for themselves.
  • 37:27So that again, that's not a fact issue.
  • 37:29That is there's something about.
  • 37:33Choosing a treatment option
  • 37:34that likely will lead to death,
  • 37:37that is quite aversive for families and
  • 37:39I think we need to keep that in mind
  • 37:41and and we'll come back to this in a moment.
  • 37:44But the bottom line here again,
  • 37:45this trial entirely negative.
  • 37:49So why, why?
  • 37:51Why have these information
  • 37:53focused interventions failed?
  • 37:55What do you what do you think about
  • 37:57a moment here to pause before before
  • 37:59I give you my thoughts? Yeah.
  • 38:01One thing is just that we're focusing on
  • 38:03giving information, but like we're not.
  • 38:06Actually offering recommendations or
  • 38:07like incorporating what the values and
  • 38:10preferences are to then say what we
  • 38:12think morbidity would look like and
  • 38:13what quality of life would look like.
  • 38:15I mean, I do think families have gotten
  • 38:17more sophisticated and reflecting on values
  • 38:20and preferences when asked about that,
  • 38:22but I think we still make it all about.
  • 38:25That in the moment decision instead
  • 38:26of the three month,
  • 38:27six month recovery that it might
  • 38:29take without a guarantee that they're
  • 38:30going to be able to return to the
  • 38:31golf course or pickleball court.
  • 38:33Yeah.
  • 38:33So. So maybe there's there's more
  • 38:35that needs to be done even when
  • 38:37families that they actually truly
  • 38:39understand patients values and
  • 38:41preferences to help make that
  • 38:43directly tied to the implications
  • 38:45for certain treatment choices.
  • 38:46OK Mark what else?
  • 38:48It's still premised on this rational
  • 38:50actor theory and you know I
  • 38:52think that a lot of politicians
  • 38:54know that that's just not.
  • 38:55The way people are thinking,
  • 38:56yeah, the information is important,
  • 38:58but there's so many
  • 38:58other factors that are
  • 39:00going into preferences.
  • 39:01Yeah, yeah. So many other factors
  • 39:03that are going in and and maybe.
  • 39:05Things getting in the way of families
  • 39:08actually making the decisions that.
  • 39:11Possibly in their hearts, they think
  • 39:12is the right decision for the patient.
  • 39:15OK, so let's keep your mind on
  • 39:17what could those things be, yeah?
  • 39:20Yeah, that you know,
  • 39:22traditional economic theory I separating
  • 39:24a little bit from this is is based on the
  • 39:26idea that people are rational actors.
  • 39:29But the field of behavioral economics
  • 39:31has emerged largely as a response to
  • 39:34the fact that in indeed you're not
  • 39:36often rational actors and we tend to
  • 39:39overweight and underweight certain
  • 39:41considerations into your point families.
  • 39:43Surrogates tend to not want to choose
  • 39:45something that they think is more
  • 39:47likely to lead to a sooner death
  • 39:49because there's sort of just default.
  • 39:51Understanding in American culture that
  • 39:52death is bad and to be avoided and
  • 39:54it's a failure and you want to fight.
  • 39:57And you know we have all of this
  • 39:59language around a rescue depots and
  • 40:01fighting and being tough and being
  • 40:04strong and and so perhaps looking
  • 40:06towards behavioral economics to
  • 40:08understand the way choices are
  • 40:10presented and maybe changing the
  • 40:12default might be more effective in
  • 40:15helping to sort of align those decisions.
  • 40:18Yeah. So this notion that there are things.
  • 40:21Other than information that can
  • 40:22substantially shape decisions.
  • 40:24And some of the things could be
  • 40:25have to do with cultural norms
  • 40:27they could have to do with.
  • 40:29Families and we'll talk about this in
  • 40:32a second, but families perception of
  • 40:35moral blame blame worthiness, right?
  • 40:37To what extent does an information focused
  • 40:40intervention attend to the sense of
  • 40:42moral guilt and blame that might come up?
  • 40:46Others, yeah.
  • 40:48I guess I'm wondering like with the
  • 40:52outcomes that like the best decision is.
  • 40:56Transition to comfort or health
  • 40:57care utilization, I I feel like.
  • 41:00That's kind of like an arbitrary,
  • 41:02yeah. What gives here? That doesn't seem
  • 41:04very patient centered, does it? No.
  • 41:08Different way. These interventions
  • 41:12to improve family understanding and
  • 41:13position understanding of patient
  • 41:15values didn't make a difference,
  • 41:17so maybe we already were.
  • 41:19Doing enough of that.
  • 41:21Yeah. So I think, right,
  • 41:23you could purely hypothetically could say,
  • 41:26yeah, listen, there's no problem there.
  • 41:28This is just an intervention trying to
  • 41:30intervene on something that's not broken.
  • 41:33It's not going to show an effect.
  • 41:34There is a host of empirical literature
  • 41:37that there are quite a lot of problems
  • 41:40with how families make decisions for
  • 41:43incapacitated patients as well as a lot
  • 41:46of data showing that the process by
  • 41:49which clinicians engage with families.
  • 41:51Prognosis, values, treatment options,
  • 41:53recommendations is often quite abysmal.
  • 41:55So I think all of these studies were
  • 41:57premised on we all think that there's a
  • 41:59problem with the end of life decision making.
  • 42:01There's good empirical data for it.
  • 42:03Let's see if we can improve it.
  • 42:04But I think the other part of this that's
  • 42:06important is none of these outcomes are
  • 42:08really about goal concordant care, right?
  • 42:10They just.
  • 42:12I think we should come back to
  • 42:13that in the discussion section.
  • 42:14I'll show you a couple of trials that
  • 42:16did have that of an outcome measure,
  • 42:17but that if you know there are multiple
  • 42:20outcomes of interest when we're
  • 42:22studying surrogate decision making,
  • 42:23arguably from a from a a
  • 42:26normative perspective,
  • 42:27that the degree to which the decision
  • 42:29reflects what's important to the patient,
  • 42:31IE is concordant with their values and
  • 42:34preferences is a very important outcome.
  • 42:36Yes. So I agree with that.
  • 42:38Let, let,
  • 42:38let me go on and then we'll have some
  • 42:40more time in the discussion, OK.
  • 42:42So I,
  • 42:43I think you all have,
  • 42:45have largely nailed the reasons why
  • 42:49these informational interventions
  • 42:50were not successful.
  • 42:51I do just want to point out that we
  • 42:54shouldn't leave this room pessimistic
  • 42:55about giving information to patients and
  • 42:57helping them make more informed decisions.
  • 42:59This is a Cochrane review of 31,000
  • 43:02participants and more than 100 studies
  • 43:04of different kinds of decision.
  • 43:06It's for a variety of health decisions,
  • 43:08and what they found is that across a
  • 43:10wide variety of decision contexts,
  • 43:12people exposed to decision aids are
  • 43:14better informed, clearer about their values,
  • 43:16and have more accurate risk perceptions.
  • 43:18Growing evidence that decision aids
  • 43:20may improve values congruent choices.
  • 43:22So lots of decisions and medicine,
  • 43:25giving information,
  • 43:26helping people think about their values,
  • 43:28that may well be enough.
  • 43:30And the ICU context it seems to not be.
  • 43:34And so why not?
  • 43:35Well again,
  • 43:36this,
  • 43:36these are data that largely
  • 43:38just validate the kind of things
  • 43:40you all just raised in here.
  • 43:42So a variety of studies that really
  • 43:44explore the the perspectives of
  • 43:46family surrogate decision makers and
  • 43:48adult intensive care units and some
  • 43:50of the overarching themes and this,
  • 43:52this first quote is from the
  • 43:54study that Yale Schenker led when
  • 43:56she first got to Pittsburgh.
  • 43:58Surrogates experienced significant
  • 43:59emotional conflict between the desire to
  • 44:01act in accordance with their loved ones.
  • 44:03Values and one not wanting to feel
  • 44:05responsible for a loved one's death.
  • 44:07This is the the the,
  • 44:08the guilt and blame aspect of things.
  • 44:10And then also a desire to
  • 44:12pursue any chance of
  • 44:13recovery. I think also a linked thing
  • 44:15to the cultural norms around respect,
  • 44:18meaning doing whatever you can
  • 44:20to help the patient survive.
  • 44:21So again, that these are not
  • 44:23informational issues, right?
  • 44:24These are about psychological considerations,
  • 44:27about family relations,
  • 44:29about moral blame.
  • 44:32Related, but I think it's just we
  • 44:35often have these conversations
  • 44:36in a vacuum when we think about
  • 44:38decision making in the ICU,
  • 44:39but we forget about the social context
  • 44:41in which these decisions happen.
  • 44:43We did a study in which we found
  • 44:45that surrogates having previously
  • 44:46felt discriminated against in the
  • 44:48healthcare setting previously,
  • 44:49which is to say not during the the the
  • 44:52index hospitalization that we were studying.
  • 44:54This was strongly associated with
  • 44:56conflict with clinicians about
  • 44:57end of life decisions in the ICU
  • 44:59with an odds ratio of 17 1/2 so.
  • 45:02You know,
  • 45:03keeping in mind that what patients and
  • 45:06families bring into the ICU setting,
  • 45:08whether they're this is the structural
  • 45:10racism or perceptions of ableism,
  • 45:12if their loved one has a disability,
  • 45:14these things also will may potentially
  • 45:17strongly influence the the way
  • 45:18surrogates make decisions and how
  • 45:20they interact with the clinical team.
  • 45:25There's also an enormous literature
  • 45:27about how strong emotions affect
  • 45:30how people reason and deliberate,
  • 45:33and this is largely this comes
  • 45:35from experimental psychologists.
  • 45:37This is a really nice review
  • 45:38article if people are interested
  • 45:39in learning more about it.
  • 45:41But the gist of the findings
  • 45:43is that intensive emotional
  • 45:44states like fear or anxiety,
  • 45:46the kind of things that arguably
  • 45:47families experience quite a lot when
  • 45:49their loved one is acutely ill,
  • 45:51produce deficits in people's
  • 45:53reasoning ability, such as people.
  • 45:55Have lower ability to recall information
  • 45:57and organize this information in memory,
  • 45:59which is to say,
  • 46:00less ability to work with it.
  • 46:02They scan alternatives to decisions
  • 46:05and in a more haphazard fashion,
  • 46:07which is to say they they may not reliably
  • 46:09consider each available treatment option.
  • 46:12And then they select options without,
  • 46:15as I said, select options without
  • 46:16considering every alternative.
  • 46:17And finally,
  • 46:18they process persuasion
  • 46:19arguments less thoroughly.
  • 46:21And what this means the persuasion
  • 46:22argument might have might look something
  • 46:24like a doctor giving a recommendation.
  • 46:26And so.
  • 46:27If people in what I think of as
  • 46:29really hot states you know flooded
  • 46:30states are are not really able to
  • 46:33hear doctors recommendations and
  • 46:34Kathleen that the the thought that
  • 46:37that may be giving recommendations
  • 46:39more will be the fix could be
  • 46:41inhibited or impaired by families
  • 46:42inability to really hear them
  • 46:44because of the flooding that
  • 46:46they're experiencing emotionally.
  • 46:49So what do we do with this?
  • 46:51Well. Nick Deon Odom,
  • 46:54who's a nurse researcher at Alabama,
  • 46:56and I wrote a paper published
  • 46:58last year in which we tried to
  • 47:01reconceptualize how we should
  • 47:02be thinking about interventions
  • 47:04to support surrogate decision
  • 47:05makers of critically ill patients.
  • 47:07And the gist of it is it
  • 47:10is in this window here.
  • 47:11Decision support that focuses on
  • 47:13medical facts alone mistakenly treats
  • 47:15the act of deciding for others
  • 47:17as a purely cognitive exercise,
  • 47:19rather than one that also entails
  • 47:22emotional and psychological dimensions.
  • 47:23So if we're going to improve decision making,
  • 47:25we probably need to be,
  • 47:27at least in some cases and in a tailored way,
  • 47:30attending not only to informational issues,
  • 47:32but also to the the high emotions that
  • 47:33can come up and also the psychological
  • 47:35barriers to making decisions that
  • 47:37are caught concordant or congruent
  • 47:39with patients values and preferences.
  • 47:43This there's also a paper here by
  • 47:47power that that has been around
  • 47:50for 10 or 11 years now that that
  • 47:52addresses what's called the cognitive
  • 47:54emotional decision making framework.
  • 47:55And this is another way of thinking
  • 47:57about the interplay of emotions and
  • 47:59and rationality or or emotion and
  • 48:01cognition and how people make decisions.
  • 48:03And it's a, I think a great read if
  • 48:06folks want to think about this more
  • 48:09from the experimental standpoint.
  • 48:10OK, so let me let me end by
  • 48:12showing two studies that I think,
  • 48:15at least in part embody this multidimensional
  • 48:19approach to surrogate support.
  • 48:22And so the 1st is a trial
  • 48:24done by Randy Curtis.
  • 48:25Randy is at many of you know Randy.
  • 48:27He's at UW. He's a fantastic scholar.
  • 48:30He's a longtime mentor to me.
  • 48:33But Randy conducted a randomized trial
  • 48:36of what he called a communication
  • 48:39communication facilitator for patients
  • 48:41in ICU's at high risk of death.
  • 48:45And essentially the intervention randomized
  • 48:47it to the communication facilitator
  • 48:50or usual care, the communication.
  • 48:52Facilitator was a a nurse or social
  • 48:55worker who underwent it was a week
  • 48:57of training in mediation theory,
  • 48:59theory and how to support families
  • 49:02emotionally and psychologically through
  • 49:03traumatic circumstances in the ICU.
  • 49:05And so basically this boiled down to the
  • 49:08nurse delivering tailored support to the
  • 49:11families based on a coping styles inventory,
  • 49:14facilitating communication with the ICU team,
  • 49:16and then identifying conflict in its
  • 49:17what I would think of as sort of its
  • 49:20nascent stage rather than well formed.
  • 49:22And intervening early to help mediate
  • 49:24that conflict with the clinical team.
  • 49:26So,
  • 49:27you know,
  • 49:27compare that to the support intervention
  • 49:29that was all about prognosis,
  • 49:31communication and patient values and making
  • 49:33sure that that everyone on on the in
  • 49:36dyad or Triad knew about that information.
  • 49:38This is a much broader conception of
  • 49:40what it means to support surrogates.
  • 49:42And lo and behold,
  • 49:43what they found is that the
  • 49:45intervention had positive effects
  • 49:47on a variety of outcome measures.
  • 49:49Unfortunately,
  • 49:50no,
  • 49:51there was no measure of gold concordance
  • 49:52of care in this study written.
  • 49:54Randy's interest was much more on that.
  • 49:56The impact on on surrogate
  • 49:58decision makers of the process,
  • 50:00their psychological distress and
  • 50:01then also healthcare utilization and
  • 50:03what he found is that patients in the
  • 50:06intervention arm had lower symptoms
  • 50:07of depression at six months and also
  • 50:09lower symptoms of PTSD at six months,
  • 50:11two of their main outcome measures.
  • 50:14They also looked at ICU length of stay
  • 50:18among people who died on with the idea that.
  • 50:21Shorter duration of intensive treatment
  • 50:23prior to death is probably a good thing
  • 50:25as long as the mortality rate is not higher.
  • 50:27The mortality rate was not
  • 50:28higher in the intervention arm.
  • 50:30What they found is that among decedents,
  • 50:32the ICU length of stay was substantially
  • 50:35shorter in the intervention
  • 50:36arm as were the costs.
  • 50:38So big changes in healthcare utilization
  • 50:41with this kind of intervention.
  • 50:44And then the last,
  • 50:46the last study that I want to
  • 50:48present is a trial that we did at
  • 50:51the University of Pittsburgh and UPMC
  • 50:53Health System and published in 2018.
  • 50:56And this was rather than.
  • 50:59Having an external person,
  • 51:01a communication facilitator,
  • 51:03joined the ICU team.
  • 51:04We hypothesized that it would be
  • 51:07possible to train up people on the
  • 51:09existing ICU team and restructure
  • 51:11family support processes in the ICU
  • 51:14in order to better support families
  • 51:16without having to to add yet another
  • 51:18person to the already complex ICU team.
  • 51:21And so we we conducted a step
  • 51:23wedge cluster randomized trial
  • 51:24comparing usual care to what we
  • 51:26call the partner intervention,
  • 51:27which is a protocolized
  • 51:28family support intervention.
  • 51:29Delivered by the existing
  • 51:31interprofessional team,
  • 51:32it was actually a nurse,
  • 51:33nurse and social worker LED intervention,
  • 51:36but involved nurses,
  • 51:38social workers,
  • 51:39palliative care if they were consulted
  • 51:41and that the ICU clinical team
  • 51:44we randomized 14120 incapacitated
  • 51:45critically ill patients at high
  • 51:47risk of death or severe disability.
  • 51:49And we looked at a variety of
  • 51:51outcome measures, surrogates,
  • 51:52long term psychological distress,
  • 51:54a measure of patient centeredness
  • 51:55of care which begins to get at this
  • 51:58notion is that are the care decisions more.
  • 52:00Do they seem more consistent with
  • 52:02the patients values and preferences
  • 52:03or were they were these issues more
  • 52:06attended to in the decision making
  • 52:08process a measure of the quality of
  • 52:10clinician family communication, ICU,
  • 52:11length of state and healthcare costs?
  • 52:15And so as I said, this was a,
  • 52:16this was a multicomponent intervention that
  • 52:18was overseen by nurse leaders in each ICU.
  • 52:20Each of these nurses underwent 16
  • 52:23hours of intensive communication
  • 52:25skills training about how to support
  • 52:27families both emotionally and again,
  • 52:29regarding the psychological
  • 52:31complexity of being a surrogate.
  • 52:33And then the each ICU implemented a care
  • 52:36pathway and I'll show you in a moment of
  • 52:39family support pathway that every single
  • 52:41patient in the intervention arm got.
  • 52:43And then there was a lot because this is a.
  • 52:45You know, a complex behavioral intervention.
  • 52:47There was a lot of on the ground
  • 52:50support to help this new model of
  • 52:53care actually take root in the ICU.
  • 52:56And so this is too small for you all to see,
  • 52:57but basically on the X axis is days,
  • 53:00and on and in the green boxes is what
  • 53:03the partner intervention entailed.
  • 53:06The gist of it is that the partner
  • 53:09intervention entailed a formal
  • 53:10interprofessional family meeting
  • 53:11within 48 hours,
  • 53:13and then every five to seven days thereafter
  • 53:15that the partner nurses organized.
  • 53:17The partner nurses also immediately upon
  • 53:20the patient being entered into the trial,
  • 53:23met with the family to establish Rapport.
  • 53:25To get a sense for who they are,
  • 53:27who the patient is as a person,
  • 53:29and make sure they know that their
  • 53:31contact person for the whole ICU
  • 53:33stay is the partner nurse or one
  • 53:35of his or her colleagues.
  • 53:36Then, before each family meeting,
  • 53:39the partner nurse sat down with the
  • 53:41family and walked them through,
  • 53:42essentially a protocolized way to begin
  • 53:44to think through the patient's values that
  • 53:47their main questions for the clinical team,
  • 53:49what concerns they most had,
  • 53:51what things that they thought the
  • 53:53patient would most be afraid of or
  • 53:55most worried about in the context.
  • 53:56Of the of this current illness
  • 53:58and then the the partner nurse
  • 53:59was there at the family meetings
  • 54:01with the interprofessional team.
  • 54:03These meetings were led by the docs,
  • 54:05but the nurses were trained to really
  • 54:07be monitoring the families for sort of
  • 54:09emotional overwhelm or or monitoring
  • 54:11for situations that seemed like
  • 54:13the family was not understanding.
  • 54:14And we trained them to have ways to sort
  • 54:16of slow the conversation down or to,
  • 54:18you know,
  • 54:19offer a a tissue to the family as
  • 54:21a way to get the doctor to see
  • 54:23that what he or she is saying is
  • 54:25quite overwhelming for the family.
  • 54:27So lots of subtle clues or cues to
  • 54:29try to either slow the conversation
  • 54:32down or have key information that
  • 54:34the family may not have quite
  • 54:36understood or or gotten repeated.
  • 54:38And then debriefing with the,
  • 54:41the clinical team after that.
  • 54:42And so this was the cadence that
  • 54:44happened throughout the entire ICU stay.
  • 54:46The family knew every day they'd be
  • 54:48talking to the partner nurse and
  • 54:50sometimes the conversations were 30
  • 54:51seconds and sometimes they were 30 minutes.
  • 54:53But this was,
  • 54:54you know,
  • 54:55meant to be an interprofessional
  • 54:58team delivered intervention.
  • 55:00And what we found is that it did not at
  • 55:02all impact surrogate psychological
  • 55:04distress at six months,
  • 55:06no difference in their symptoms
  • 55:07of anxiety or depression and
  • 55:09no difference in their PTSD.
  • 55:10Symptoms at all really,
  • 55:11really no, no effect on these
  • 55:14long term psychological outcomes.
  • 55:16But on every other measure that we studied
  • 55:18the intervention had a positive effect.
  • 55:20So on measures of the extent to which
  • 55:22families felt that the communication
  • 55:24quality with the clinical team was good,
  • 55:27there was a significant improvement
  • 55:28between the control and intervention
  • 55:30arm and and for me as someone who cares
  • 55:32quite a lot about patient centered care,
  • 55:35we also administered to surrogates at
  • 55:38three months a validated inventory of.
  • 55:41Their perceptions of the degree to
  • 55:43which the care was centered on the
  • 55:44patient as a person and what we found
  • 55:46is a significant increase in the
  • 55:48proportion of families that said yes,
  • 55:50that the care was highly consistent with
  • 55:52my loved ones values and preferences,
  • 55:53so a 15% increase,
  • 55:55which is a pretty big effect size
  • 55:58for this kind of intervention.
  • 56:01And then in terms of healthcare
  • 56:03utilization and cost,
  • 56:03similar findings to what Randy found,
  • 56:05Randy Curtis found with his communicator,
  • 56:08his communication facilitator
  • 56:09study decreased length of stay
  • 56:12among the patients who died but no
  • 56:15change among survivors,
  • 56:16decreased hospital length of stay
  • 56:18and decreased total hospitalization
  • 56:20costs and direct variable costs.
  • 56:22And for those of you who are interested
  • 56:24in the the economic side of this,
  • 56:27we also calculated how much it cost
  • 56:28to deploy the intervention per patient
  • 56:30which was inclusive of the nurses.
  • 56:32Time the training costs and the
  • 56:34and the cost to support the
  • 56:36ICU's in this longitudinally and
  • 56:38it was about $170 per patient.
  • 56:40So the economic argument was very
  • 56:42strong for this and as well as the
  • 56:45ethical argument and I think this was
  • 56:47you know even before these results
  • 56:49were published in the New England
  • 56:51Journal the UPMC health system said
  • 56:52we're we're going to spread this
  • 56:54across all forty of our hospitals
  • 56:55and the health system and they have
  • 56:57done that to this day with a a large
  • 57:00investment of resources to train.
  • 57:02People yearly in this in this intervention.
  • 57:06OK,
  • 57:06so those are two different interventions,
  • 57:08both positive on important outcomes,
  • 57:10but not all outcomes grounded in in
  • 57:12a much more broad conceptualization
  • 57:13of what it means to support
  • 57:16surrogate decision makers.
  • 57:17And the last study that I
  • 57:19just want to flag for you,
  • 57:20we have a trial ongoing now where
  • 57:22we're testing actually whether
  • 57:24early integration of palliative
  • 57:26care specialists into the care of
  • 57:28patients with advanced critical
  • 57:30illness improves outcomes.
  • 57:31Again,
  • 57:32the same kind of outcomes
  • 57:33that that I discussed.
  • 57:35This is again,
  • 57:36yet a different way to deliver an
  • 57:39intervention that attends to not
  • 57:42only the informational concerns,
  • 57:43but also the psychological concerns
  • 57:45and the emotional concerns that
  • 57:47come up for surrogates in the ICU.
  • 57:48Space we the target is 500 patients.
  • 57:51We're about halfway through enrollment.
  • 57:52So we expect that within the
  • 57:54next year to two
  • 57:55years, we should have results of this trial.
  • 57:57So stay tuned for this one.
  • 58:00And then my last slide is this.
  • 58:04I I think it's really important to think
  • 58:07about if we agree that to support families
  • 58:10is going to require more than information.
  • 58:13I think there are big implications for how
  • 58:15we train the next generation of clinicians.
  • 58:17We already struggle to train clinicians
  • 58:19in how to have a good informed consent
  • 58:22conversation right and how to just talk
  • 58:24about prognosis in clear ways or even
  • 58:26just how to elicit values and preferences.
  • 58:29But I think what these studies.
  • 58:30Suggest is that it's going to
  • 58:32take even more than that to help
  • 58:33surrogates really make good decisions
  • 58:35and feel supported in the process.
  • 58:37And so we,
  • 58:37I think we need to refocus or or
  • 58:40expand the focus of our educational
  • 58:42interventions to really focus on
  • 58:43framing clinicians on how to support
  • 58:45families in traumatic circumstances.
  • 58:47And there's a whole literature on
  • 58:49trauma informed care that I think
  • 58:51is really promising in this regard.
  • 58:52And then also.
  • 58:53Considering and and pursue developing
  • 58:55ways to train clinicians and how
  • 58:57to support families through the the
  • 59:00psychological complexity of of potentially
  • 59:02withdrawing life support on a loved one.
  • 59:05You know how to respond when it's
  • 59:07clear that blameworthiness is coming
  • 59:09up or there are family dynamics and
  • 59:11and the family does not that family
  • 59:13who's the main surrogate decision maker
  • 59:14feels that they will be ostracized
  • 59:16from the family of for a particular
  • 59:18decision that they need to make.
  • 59:19You know these are when you think
  • 59:20about it that sort of university
  • 59:22classroom level these are not 101.
  • 59:24Introductory level skills.
  • 59:25These are very high level skills,
  • 59:27and so in addition to thinking about
  • 59:29let's let's develop good interventions
  • 59:30to to try to improve these outcomes.
  • 59:32I think we also need to leave open
  • 59:35the possibility that these skills are
  • 59:37too complex to expect most clinicians
  • 59:39to do them well and within again,
  • 59:41thinking about whether specialists like
  • 59:44palliative care specialists might be
  • 59:46for select cases of a better approach.
  • 59:48OK. I'm going to end there.
  • 59:50It looks like we have my right that we
  • 59:52have about 1/2 an hour for questions.
  • 59:54OK, thanks very much.
  • 59:55We're going to set up.
  • 01:00:02We're going to set up up
  • 01:00:03here for the questions.
  • 01:00:03Are you going to bring me
  • 01:00:04that laptop? OK.
  • 01:00:09And and that the mics, the ceiling
  • 01:00:10work well enough such that such
  • 01:00:12that you guys can ask questions,
  • 01:00:13we'll we'll we'll do this from here.
  • 01:00:16I said here, you're the one for you.
  • 01:00:20I'll just, I'll take,
  • 01:00:21I'll just set it down.
  • 01:00:22I'm just speaking.
  • 01:00:25Grab a chair.
  • 01:00:32And in in a minute or two,
  • 01:00:34for those who are on the zoom,
  • 01:00:35I'll have the computer for me and
  • 01:00:36be able to see your questions,
  • 01:00:38send them in through the Q
  • 01:00:39and a portion of the of the
  • 01:00:41zoom call rather than the chat.
  • 01:00:44And in the meantime, Ben Tulchin
  • 01:00:46has a question for us.
  • 01:00:49That was a wonderful talk and I
  • 01:00:51really appreciate your point that
  • 01:00:53there there is more needed than.
  • 01:00:57In information and that and that.
  • 01:01:01Psychological stresses and.
  • 01:01:06Technology of of the interaction is
  • 01:01:08at least as important, if not more.
  • 01:01:11Information available for
  • 01:01:12the family of clinicians.
  • 01:01:16Your intervention was was
  • 01:01:19so remarkably successful,
  • 01:01:20one thing that jumped out.
  • 01:01:23About it was that you said that the
  • 01:01:26nurse communicator was attached to the
  • 01:01:29family throughout the course of their
  • 01:01:31ICU stay and attended all the family
  • 01:01:34meetings and something that I have.
  • 01:01:36Clinical experience and again and again is,
  • 01:01:38is, is the negative impact of.
  • 01:01:43Rapid transitions of care and
  • 01:01:47a lot consistent care and you
  • 01:01:48know a lot of the study basis,
  • 01:01:51but on a normal basis we have had a lot
  • 01:01:54of success when we have implemented
  • 01:01:56more consistent communication across
  • 01:01:59family meetings like we did this
  • 01:02:01regularly in the CIC with patients
  • 01:02:04on ECMO during during COVID.
  • 01:02:06And admitted I'm curious what the
  • 01:02:10degree to which you think the.
  • 01:02:13Impact of your intervention was
  • 01:02:15the consistency of care as opposed
  • 01:02:19to the communication skill.
  • 01:02:20Is that possible to put apart?
  • 01:02:25Uh, the folks, Karen,
  • 01:02:27did folks online hear that?
  • 01:02:30You know, you getting any feedback?
  • 01:02:32Did they hear Ben's question?
  • 01:02:33She doesn't have the feedback there now,
  • 01:02:34so why don't you just summarize it briefly,
  • 01:02:36Karen, I could also use you up
  • 01:02:38here for a question with this.
  • 01:02:40This is your question that I heard was to,
  • 01:02:42to what extent was the positive
  • 01:02:44effect of the intervention
  • 01:02:46attributable to continuity of
  • 01:02:48communication versus any particular?
  • 01:02:52Augmentation or improvement in the
  • 01:02:54in the individual conversations,
  • 01:02:56I I don't know. You know,
  • 01:02:59we didn't really to answer
  • 01:03:00that question you'd have to,
  • 01:03:02you'd have to do it experimentally
  • 01:03:05and and randomize people.
  • 01:03:06That's part of the challenge of
  • 01:03:09multicomponent interventions.
  • 01:03:10Unless you're going to do a
  • 01:03:12factorial design in the first round,
  • 01:03:14you won't be able to know whether
  • 01:03:16one intervention component was
  • 01:03:18quite helpful and other intervention
  • 01:03:20components either were not or.
  • 01:03:22Potentially, potentially even more harmful.
  • 01:03:23And so that that is I think just
  • 01:03:25from a research standpoint and an
  • 01:03:27important consideration to say,
  • 01:03:28you know.
  • 01:03:29Keeping in mind that in for a multicomponent
  • 01:03:31intervention at the end of the trial,
  • 01:03:33you'll often be left with open questions
  • 01:03:36about which element was most effective.
  • 01:03:39We did not do an embedded ethnographic
  • 01:03:43qualitative evaluation of the
  • 01:03:45intervention for this trial.
  • 01:03:47We are doing those now for the for
  • 01:03:49the palliative care trial and for
  • 01:03:50several others we have ongoing.
  • 01:03:52So we'll,
  • 01:03:52I think we'll be able to begin to
  • 01:03:55discern these kinds of issues,
  • 01:03:57these kinds of issues in the future.
  • 01:03:59What I will say is anecdotally we have the,
  • 01:04:02the nurses and the docs both both of
  • 01:04:06those groups generally reported that knowing.
  • 01:04:10And having clear information about what
  • 01:04:11had been said to the family before,
  • 01:04:13what the narrative was that that
  • 01:04:15that the new attending coming on
  • 01:04:18needed to sort of appraise for
  • 01:04:19herself and then continue if she
  • 01:04:21agreed with it was quite helpful.
  • 01:04:26Many years ago Terry Free,
  • 01:04:28one of our Jerry Jerry I
  • 01:04:30didn't stack would be here,
  • 01:04:31did a study in which she interviewed
  • 01:04:33families of people who who had
  • 01:04:36loved ones who have
  • 01:04:37died. And I I think it
  • 01:04:38was only people who died,
  • 01:04:39but they. She asked them.
  • 01:04:42What it was if they felt
  • 01:04:45supported, if they felt, you know,
  • 01:04:47what made the experience worthwhile.
  • 01:04:50And in her study, none of the family
  • 01:04:53reported that the doctors were
  • 01:04:55important in that support. Has
  • 01:04:59your have these
  • 01:05:00more recent studies shown
  • 01:05:03that the doctors are doing a better
  • 01:05:06job of relating and supporting?
  • 01:05:09About that our intervention,
  • 01:05:11that the partner intervention and
  • 01:05:13Randy Curtis's intervention both
  • 01:05:15essentially bypassed trying to
  • 01:05:17intervene directly on the doctors.
  • 01:05:19The the You know, the logic there is that.
  • 01:05:24And the nicest way to say
  • 01:05:25it is the docks are are.
  • 01:05:27It's hard to change behavior.
  • 01:05:32And yet these interventions were successful.
  • 01:05:34We, I think we both had a lot of concern at
  • 01:05:37the outset that if the the most important
  • 01:05:39active ingredient is the doctor and
  • 01:05:40we're not intervening directly on them,
  • 01:05:42are these interventions doomed to fail?
  • 01:05:44But we did train the nurses on how to sort
  • 01:05:48of gently get the doctors to do things
  • 01:05:52that would be supportive for the family
  • 01:05:54to to restate the patients prognosis,
  • 01:05:56to slow down the conversation to
  • 01:05:59maybe be empathic, right, you know?
  • 01:06:01What these nurses often reported is you know,
  • 01:06:04when when I offered the the family
  • 01:06:07a a tissue.
  • 01:06:08Suddenly this neurosurgeon who never,
  • 01:06:11never says anything empathic to families,
  • 01:06:13sat back and said, I'm, I'm really sorry.
  • 01:06:15This is, I see how hard this is.
  • 01:06:17But it's it's these kind of cues.
  • 01:06:20You know, another interprofessional
  • 01:06:22team member queuing to just nudge the,
  • 01:06:25the physician to do something that
  • 01:06:27he or she might be able to do with
  • 01:06:30the right circumstances.
  • 01:06:31Positions are potentially educable
  • 01:06:32or at least they can be reminded.
  • 01:06:35Yeah. So couple things here on this.
  • 01:06:38First of all,
  • 01:06:39there's a question
  • 01:06:40whether this event will be recorded.
  • 01:06:42And yes, it will.
  • 01:06:42And it'll only be available if
  • 01:06:44Doug tells us it's available.
  • 01:06:45Don't tell us. OK, fine.
  • 01:06:46And put you on the spot there.
  • 01:06:47Karen told me we were going to do that,
  • 01:06:49and thanks to all of you for
  • 01:06:50telling me that you can hear us.
  • 01:06:51The next question was
  • 01:06:52someone in the back, please.
  • 01:06:54Question harkening back to one of
  • 01:06:56the first points you made about the
  • 01:06:58patient living with HIV in California,
  • 01:07:00speak up a little bit and I was wondering
  • 01:07:03have you have you read any literature?
  • 01:07:06I can certainly appreciate that
  • 01:07:07the overwhelming majority of
  • 01:07:09states focuses on this familial
  • 01:07:11hierarchy of choices for surrogates,
  • 01:07:13but I was wondering on non familial
  • 01:07:15surrogates, are there any,
  • 01:07:17is there any evidence or literature
  • 01:07:19that you've seen possibly from those
  • 01:07:21other 15 states or just focusing on
  • 01:07:24the interventions you presented today?
  • 01:07:26All of them focus on the familial unit,
  • 01:07:29so I'm wondering about, not.
  • 01:07:32Is your question in the states
  • 01:07:34without a hierarchy do we know
  • 01:07:36what the decision making processes
  • 01:07:38are like or how it's different?
  • 01:07:40Is it better or is it worse?
  • 01:07:41Or specifically for interventions where
  • 01:07:44there isn't there are non familial surrogates
  • 01:07:48so the the key distinction is not between
  • 01:07:51familial versus non familial surrogates.
  • 01:07:53In these states the key is in 35 of 50 U.S.
  • 01:07:57states there is a default
  • 01:07:59list of of the order.
  • 01:08:02Of genealogical relationships and
  • 01:08:04and the families always have it's
  • 01:08:05not yes spouse it's it's different
  • 01:08:08family members all the way down
  • 01:08:11and sometimes including friends in
  • 01:08:12California and the other states that
  • 01:08:14they vary from state to state but it's
  • 01:08:16not that it's that the decision makers
  • 01:08:18are not family it's that there's no
  • 01:08:21hierarchy of who which family member
  • 01:08:23gets to decide and instead it's the
  • 01:08:25clinicians job either to in some
  • 01:08:27states they do it let the physician
  • 01:08:29select who she thinks is the the most.
  • 01:08:32Appropriate surrogate California.
  • 01:08:34It's consensus.
  • 01:08:36So
  • 01:08:36I mean and and would it be safe to say
  • 01:08:38also that for the overwhelming majority
  • 01:08:41of these decisions the surrogate decision
  • 01:08:44maker is is for the overall majority is
  • 01:08:46a member of the family friends. Friends.
  • 01:08:48Doing this is pretty pretty rare.
  • 01:08:50Yeah. So. In.
  • 01:08:55I guess it really varies
  • 01:08:56on the on the population.
  • 01:08:58So for example, a lot of my
  • 01:08:59time at San Francisco General,
  • 01:09:01which is the safety in that
  • 01:09:02hospital in San Francisco,
  • 01:09:04many patients were homeless.
  • 01:09:05Many had psychological or
  • 01:09:06substance abuse issues and were
  • 01:09:08estranged from their families.
  • 01:09:09And so quite often they
  • 01:09:11either were unrepresented,
  • 01:09:12which is to say they didn't have
  • 01:09:14anyone to make decisions or it
  • 01:09:16was friends because they were
  • 01:09:17estranged from their family.
  • 01:09:18And then increasingly we see this with the.
  • 01:09:22The aging generation where people
  • 01:09:24are outliving all of their
  • 01:09:27relatives and ending up either
  • 01:09:29unrepresented or having a close
  • 01:09:31friend who's usually a younger
  • 01:09:33person as their decision maker.
  • 01:09:36Thank you. So I have a question
  • 01:09:37here from the zoom please.
  • 01:09:38Why even assume that there is an
  • 01:09:40in quotes correct answer on quotes.
  • 01:09:42A complex decisions may be path dependent
  • 01:09:45with people giving different
  • 01:09:47answers on different ways.
  • 01:09:48I mean I I suppose well I'll let you
  • 01:09:51one way or the outcomes.
  • 01:09:53Different answers on
  • 01:09:54different ways of elicitation,
  • 01:09:55or different answers on different days?
  • 01:09:57I I think that they're actually
  • 01:09:59different on different days.
  • 01:10:00OK, I think that.
  • 01:10:03That that is a first of all
  • 01:10:06that's a great question what it's
  • 01:10:08talking what the the at the root
  • 01:10:11of that is this I I think it's.
  • 01:10:14It helps us see preferences not as
  • 01:10:18something that already swim around in
  • 01:10:20our heads and we just need to pluck them out,
  • 01:10:23but as things that are
  • 01:10:25constructed in the moment, right?
  • 01:10:26And typically,
  • 01:10:27when you face a new decision,
  • 01:10:28you have to construct your your beliefs,
  • 01:10:32your attitudes,
  • 01:10:33your your decision in the moment,
  • 01:10:35which option is the best among things that
  • 01:10:37you may not have thought about before.
  • 01:10:38You don't have a preference
  • 01:10:39swimming around in your head,
  • 01:10:41and you just need to find it,
  • 01:10:41you need to construct it,
  • 01:10:43and there's quite a lot of.
  • 01:10:44Research from the behavioral
  • 01:10:45sciences that the way you elicit
  • 01:10:47people's preferences can shape
  • 01:10:49the the preferences themselves.
  • 01:10:51And so that's why it's there's a
  • 01:10:53there's actually a wonderful book
  • 01:10:54on this called the construction of
  • 01:10:56preference that speaks just to this issue.
  • 01:10:58So I think the clinical
  • 01:11:00implication though is,
  • 01:11:01is not that there are no right answers,
  • 01:11:04but that especially when patients have
  • 01:11:07not articulated a narrow preference,
  • 01:11:09there may be a range of treatment
  • 01:11:12decisions in the ICU context or in the,
  • 01:11:14you know,
  • 01:11:15inpatient or outpatient context that
  • 01:11:17are reasonably consistent with the
  • 01:11:19patients values and preferences.
  • 01:11:20And that's I think uncomfortable
  • 01:11:22for some people and yet I think
  • 01:11:24that that reflects the reality of it
  • 01:11:26when patients have not articulated
  • 01:11:27a preference when they haven't.
  • 01:11:29Really thought it through,
  • 01:11:30there may be several treatment options
  • 01:11:32each that are quite different that
  • 01:11:33could still be consistent with the
  • 01:11:35patient's values and preferences.
  • 01:11:37Thank you, Sarah.
  • 01:11:39I'm. Troubled but not surprised by some of
  • 01:11:43the earlier comments on how positions have,
  • 01:11:46let's say less, less than optimal
  • 01:11:49communication with patients.
  • 01:11:50Sorry, it's in in many of these cases.
  • 01:11:53And I absolutely think that that more
  • 01:11:56dedicated training in some of the
  • 01:11:58psychological official domains is needed,
  • 01:12:00not just the cognitive communication domains,
  • 01:12:02but I also think,
  • 01:12:03and that mark and I have actually
  • 01:12:05done work in this together,
  • 01:12:07I think another issue that often is sort
  • 01:12:09of swept under the rug and not addressed.
  • 01:12:11Because it's.
  • 01:12:11So difficult and so daunting to tackle
  • 01:12:14is the concept of clinician time famine
  • 01:12:17and particularly position time famine.
  • 01:12:19And the fact that there's actually
  • 01:12:21pretty good data to suggest that when
  • 01:12:23people are crunched for time and
  • 01:12:24physicians are not an exception to
  • 01:12:26this they're less empathic and they
  • 01:12:28they're they're not going to be as
  • 01:12:31compassionate and and and also simply
  • 01:12:33that that having a really thoughtful
  • 01:12:36empathic sensitive discussion like this
  • 01:12:39takes time whereas I think too often in.
  • 01:12:42Medicine,
  • 01:12:42there's this idea that if you're really
  • 01:12:44good at something you should be able
  • 01:12:45to do it quickly and that that's just,
  • 01:12:47you know,
  • 01:12:47we sort of hold that as as almost
  • 01:12:50an inviolate standard.
  • 01:12:51And I've had people say that to
  • 01:12:52me before like if you're good you
  • 01:12:53should be able to do it in less time.
  • 01:12:55And often my responses with certain things,
  • 01:12:57I'm good at it because I take the time and I.
  • 01:13:00But I think that in an era where more
  • 01:13:02and more we focus on productivity,
  • 01:13:04efficiency,
  • 01:13:04maximizing capacity so that we
  • 01:13:07maximize profits and and and think
  • 01:13:10about what are the main drivers
  • 01:13:12of how we build our systems.
  • 01:13:14I almost wonder if you know an
  • 01:13:17intervention looking at changing
  • 01:13:19staffing so it is a physician who's
  • 01:13:22responsible for 50% fewer patients.
  • 01:13:24And has this you know communication
  • 01:13:26intervention let's say does does that
  • 01:13:29affect the level of communication
  • 01:13:30because there is data to suggest
  • 01:13:32that that being crunched for time
  • 01:13:35not only leads to burnout which
  • 01:13:36leads to poor communication but
  • 01:13:38made just even taking burnout out of
  • 01:13:40the picture can also understandably
  • 01:13:42lead to worse communication.
  • 01:13:44And and I I've definitely felt that
  • 01:13:46anecdotally where you know when
  • 01:13:47when I'm being pulled in multiple
  • 01:13:49directions it's a lot harder for me
  • 01:13:51to sit down and really like kind of
  • 01:13:52take the time and and often I if it's.
  • 01:13:54Possible.
  • 01:13:55So if someone else can take my
  • 01:13:57pager or phone or because giving
  • 01:13:59undivided attention for more than
  • 01:14:0110 to 15 minutes is often a luxury
  • 01:14:04that we're sort of tacitly not
  • 01:14:05permitted to have when in fact that's
  • 01:14:08absolutely necessary
  • 01:14:09for a really thoughtful discussion.
  • 01:14:11It's a great point.
  • 01:14:12I think there is we have to think really
  • 01:14:14carefully about the staffing models
  • 01:14:16in our ICU and in our hospital wards.
  • 01:14:19I think they're.
  • 01:14:21If clinicians are not at the table when
  • 01:14:23the staffing models are developed,
  • 01:14:25there is a risk that they will be
  • 01:14:27penny wise and pound foolish in the
  • 01:14:29sense that the ICU attending will be
  • 01:14:31seeing 20 patients a day and that may
  • 01:14:34economically on on one Ledger look like
  • 01:14:36quite a good efficient money saving approach.
  • 01:14:40But if that dog doesn't have time to,
  • 01:14:41as you said, sit,
  • 01:14:42take the time to sit down and
  • 01:14:44talk to the families,
  • 01:14:45the length of stay maybe maybe quite
  • 01:14:47a bit longer because patients end up,
  • 01:14:50you know, end of life conversations.
  • 01:14:51They kicked down the road,
  • 01:14:52patients get trigged,
  • 01:14:53pegged that otherwise might not because
  • 01:14:55the doctor didn't have time to really
  • 01:14:57sort of hold the family's hand through.
  • 01:14:59It's terribly hard decision and and so yeah,
  • 01:15:02number one,
  • 01:15:02I think you're you're right to flag that
  • 01:15:05staffing models can really affect this,
  • 01:15:07number 2.
  • 01:15:08So I increasingly think that we
  • 01:15:10need to better leverage the full
  • 01:15:12interprofessional team and the partner
  • 01:15:15intervention really drove home from me
  • 01:15:17that we should be pushing not pushing,
  • 01:15:20we should be allowing.
  • 01:15:21Nurses to function at the the top of
  • 01:15:24their competencies in ways that you know
  • 01:15:26the hierarchies the power hierarchies
  • 01:15:28and ICU don't typically allow all.
  • 01:15:30We have done a lot of interviews with
  • 01:15:32nurses where they say you know I'd like
  • 01:15:34to do these things but the the doctors
  • 01:15:36don't really respect my opinion on
  • 01:15:38matters of appropriate goals of care
  • 01:15:41or values or engaging with families.
  • 01:15:44And so some of them there would need
  • 01:15:46to be some some wholesale changing
  • 01:15:47of the culture and ICU and around
  • 01:15:50rollout applications and what.
  • 01:15:52Doctors are willing to allow nurses to do,
  • 01:15:55and how different professions are
  • 01:15:58willing to collaborate together
  • 01:16:00in the support of families.
  • 01:16:02You know,
  • 01:16:02I think that's really about the two,
  • 01:16:04you know the physicians and nurses
  • 01:16:06and the roles we play in the
  • 01:16:09hierarchy in so many levels on,
  • 01:16:11you know, even on on ethics
  • 01:16:13consultations in an ICU for example.
  • 01:16:14They'll be you'll see why
  • 01:16:16won't we call sooner and.
  • 01:16:17And a young nurse will say,
  • 01:16:18well, I asked him,
  • 01:16:19I was told I couldn't when in
  • 01:16:21fact it is not the case.
  • 01:16:22But that may be the nurse
  • 01:16:24didn't realize that she didn't
  • 01:16:26need somebody's permission
  • 01:16:25to ask for that help. But I want to
  • 01:16:27push back a little bit
  • 01:16:28on the financial piece.
  • 01:16:29I I was interested in your data
  • 01:16:30that showed how much less money is.
  • 01:16:32Perfect. So under the cost but I
  • 01:16:35a little bit of a cynicism of a
  • 01:16:38of a recently stepped down chief
  • 01:16:40he said to the organization when
  • 01:16:42you say well they don't get it
  • 01:16:43pegged not thinking well geez,
  • 01:16:44someones going to build for that peg.
  • 01:16:45They didn't get a peg.
  • 01:16:46You see that saved money.
  • 01:16:47Yeah but someone made money when
  • 01:16:49that Peg was they didn't they
  • 01:16:50spend fewer days in the ICU.
  • 01:16:52Hey somebody made money for
  • 01:16:54each of those ICU
  • 01:16:54or they spend longer in the ICU until
  • 01:16:57the decision for a pay gets made.
  • 01:16:59That's that's the other trade and pay that.
  • 01:17:01That's the other part of this, these.
  • 01:17:02You know, these are hard conversations
  • 01:17:04that often takes three or four
  • 01:17:06meetings and if the doctor is so
  • 01:17:08busy that she pushes them off,
  • 01:17:10then that's just more days going by.
  • 01:17:12Well, no, I don't disagree
  • 01:17:14that that that could happen.
  • 01:17:15What I'm saying is that that
  • 01:17:17when things are more expensive,
  • 01:17:18made more expensive by potentially
  • 01:17:21our suboptimal ability to help the
  • 01:17:24surrogate decision makers and so
  • 01:17:25things are made more expensive.
  • 01:17:27One of the, one of the metrics
  • 01:17:28that you showed in the studies,
  • 01:17:29which was very impressive, I thought,
  • 01:17:31when they're made more expensive.
  • 01:17:33Somebody's actually making that money.
  • 01:17:34Yeah. And sometimes and.
  • 01:17:35And so those are sometimes the same
  • 01:17:38people who we may be appealing to,
  • 01:17:40to say we need more time.
  • 01:17:42So as among other things, we can help.
  • 01:17:45We can coach,
  • 01:17:46we can nurture the surrogate decision makers,
  • 01:17:49not just the patient.
  • 01:17:50Yeah, certainly. I mean,
  • 01:17:51fever service versus capitated payment
  • 01:17:53models could push hospital executives
  • 01:17:56in different directions about
  • 01:17:57whether they think a length of stay
  • 01:18:00that's shorter is better or or not.
  • 01:18:02And ideally. That's
  • 01:18:04not what the size of. Right.
  • 01:18:06I mean ideally there are other
  • 01:18:08considerations that win the day but
  • 01:18:10but it's it's it's I found your
  • 01:18:13financial information was really
  • 01:18:14impressive Mark. So there was a
  • 01:18:17specific domain that I've always
  • 01:18:18been interested in that I didn't
  • 01:18:19see quite coming out tonight yet,
  • 01:18:22which is trust and relationship building.
  • 01:18:25And you know I I feel acutely sensitive
  • 01:18:28to this having gotten COVID because
  • 01:18:31we had one of the most liberal
  • 01:18:33visitation policies and I see you
  • 01:18:36could possibly have that then mean
  • 01:18:38virtually no visitation policy and
  • 01:18:42and we're just crawling out of that.
  • 01:18:44But instead of families on grounds
  • 01:18:47you know we we would talk about
  • 01:18:50everything all the time and
  • 01:18:52sunset that the nature of the.
  • 01:18:55It didn't make the conference
  • 01:18:56was completely observed because
  • 01:18:58suddenly you're dealing with a
  • 01:18:59stranger as opposed to somebody
  • 01:19:01you've had built up relationships
  • 01:19:03and trust them over several days.
  • 01:19:05And so I wonder
  • 01:19:06if there's thoughts about that.
  • 01:19:08I think the the notion of trust or
  • 01:19:13therapeutic alliance is for me,
  • 01:19:15it's embedded when I think about,
  • 01:19:17you know, how could we actually.
  • 01:19:20Actualize. Providing emotional support,
  • 01:19:23providing psychological support,
  • 01:19:25I think that that,
  • 01:19:26that a precondition for
  • 01:19:27being able to do that,
  • 01:19:29to have to have any agency as a
  • 01:19:30clinician to help families in that
  • 01:19:32way is that there's a a trusting
  • 01:19:33relationship and a therapeutic alliance.
  • 01:19:35I think how to do that is an open
  • 01:19:39question particularly in the
  • 01:19:41context of structural racism and
  • 01:19:44or communities that have in other
  • 01:19:46ways been marginalized particularly
  • 01:19:47patients with disabilities.
  • 01:19:48And so you know we have all of
  • 01:19:51these expert recommendations.
  • 01:19:52Here's how.
  • 01:19:53And I, you know,
  • 01:19:54I I have been part of some of the
  • 01:19:56studies that have promulgated them.
  • 01:19:58Here's here are the skills that you
  • 01:19:59should use when engaging with families.
  • 01:20:02Many of them are unobjectionable.
  • 01:20:05But those recommendations come from
  • 01:20:07decision scientists and clinicians
  • 01:20:09and not from people in the communities
  • 01:20:12that have arguably the most at stake in,
  • 01:20:14in how communication plays out.
  • 01:20:16And so I,
  • 01:20:16I think of,
  • 01:20:17you know,
  • 01:20:18a really important gap in the
  • 01:20:19literature and one that one of my
  • 01:20:21postdocs right now is working.
  • 01:20:22One is engaging marginalized
  • 01:20:24communities on their views about how
  • 01:20:27end of life conversations could play
  • 01:20:30out in ways that are less dramatic,
  • 01:20:33less triggering of of past trauma.
  • 01:20:35And so, you know, we'll see that.
  • 01:20:36It may just be confirming or it may.
  • 01:20:39My sense is that that the emphasis
  • 01:20:42on things like spending much more
  • 01:20:44time proving that you're trustworthy
  • 01:20:46may may come out in a stronger way.
  • 01:20:49I would suggest perhaps also
  • 01:20:51just looking at things.
  • 01:20:52I see you dissertation policies.
  • 01:20:54Yeah.
  • 01:20:54And and see
  • 01:20:55how that influences these.
  • 01:20:59Trying to get two more and I
  • 01:21:00have one from here and then one.
  • 01:21:01Can I also make a request
  • 01:21:02I want no, no, I'm sorry.
  • 01:21:05I really do want to hear
  • 01:21:07about that person again.
  • 01:21:09We're in. We're in the place
  • 01:21:11called the Child Study Center.
  • 01:21:12That's right. Well, this is,
  • 01:21:14this is a site childhood site Center,
  • 01:21:17study centers where it's called you bet.
  • 01:21:20How pediatric clinicians think
  • 01:21:23about family decision making,
  • 01:21:25parental autonomy and what they
  • 01:21:28actually are asking families
  • 01:21:31think through when they're making
  • 01:21:32decisions for their critically
  • 01:21:33ill child and to what extent
  • 01:21:35is the best interest standard,
  • 01:21:36one that you find functional in
  • 01:21:39a communication context.
  • 01:21:41So the short answer to this
  • 01:21:42all we really have to say
  • 01:21:44and it gives you a little hit picture to it,
  • 01:21:46many at least some pediatric assist,
  • 01:21:48myself included, I've written
  • 01:21:50there's no such thing as parental.
  • 01:21:51Because autonomy, as you know,
  • 01:21:53well, I mean self rule,
  • 01:21:54you don't have self rule over someone else.
  • 01:21:57There's parental authority which
  • 01:21:58is not really as strong as
  • 01:22:01parental as patient autonomy.
  • 01:22:02You know that you have a right to refuse
  • 01:22:05a life saving treatment for yourself.
  • 01:22:07You don't necessarily have the right to
  • 01:22:08refuse it for your child and there's going
  • 01:22:10to be some threshold of harm, right.
  • 01:22:12And you mentioned Doug Deep was working,
  • 01:22:14but but beyond that just in big picture
  • 01:22:16is going to be some threshold of harm
  • 01:22:18beyond which we won't let parents go.
  • 01:22:20So a big part of the decision making.
  • 01:22:22Obviously, you're quite right that
  • 01:22:23except for at potentially adolescence,
  • 01:22:25but for most of the Pediatrics,
  • 01:22:27a substituted judgment is not an issue,
  • 01:22:29right?
  • 01:22:29So it's about trying to frame,
  • 01:22:31trying to help the parents come to what
  • 01:22:33they think is in the child's best interest.
  • 01:22:36But.
  • 01:22:37So often,
  • 01:22:38and you touching it so often in neonatology,
  • 01:22:41wherever guilted perceived guilt,
  • 01:22:43such a huge part of it, I just couldn't
  • 01:22:45forgive myself up.
  • 01:22:47I didn't try everything. And plus,
  • 01:22:48perhaps it's even my fault that we're
  • 01:22:50in this position because it didn't do a
  • 01:22:52good enough job carrying this Brexit,
  • 01:22:53these things that might not be
  • 01:22:56articulating unless the physician
  • 01:22:57is wise enough to bring them out.
  • 01:22:59Guilt is a huge part.
  • 01:23:02Finding the limits of parental
  • 01:23:04authority is a is A is a very difficult
  • 01:23:07aspect of working with this but most
  • 01:23:09of it comes down to and things that
  • 01:23:10you touched on that Sir touched on
  • 01:23:12is the time is is taking the time
  • 01:23:14to work with the parents but but.
  • 01:23:18Sorting
  • 01:23:19out what do you think would be
  • 01:23:20best for your child? Because it is
  • 01:23:21about what do you think
  • 01:23:21he would have wanted?
  • 01:23:22Right now we need to know that
  • 01:23:23an infant or a small child,
  • 01:23:25what do you think would be best?
  • 01:23:26What are your goals for this? For you?
  • 01:23:28What were your goals for this channel?
  • 01:23:30What do you value?
  • 01:23:31And of course there was.
  • 01:23:33There was a in terms of what
  • 01:23:34families value and and that goes so
  • 01:23:37much into the decision they make.
  • 01:23:39There's a colleague of mine in Annie Javier,
  • 01:23:41who has spoken to this group, I think
  • 01:23:43earlier this year and she wrote a paper.
  • 01:23:45You know, we all talk about what if
  • 01:23:46people value when you're talking to
  • 01:23:47a Yale professor who was a, a newborn
  • 01:23:49who's who's suffered a brain injury,
  • 01:23:51and what this means to that individual.
  • 01:23:53She wrote a paper called Sex and Peace.
  • 01:23:56And essentially the story was it was a
  • 01:23:59case report of dealing with a family,
  • 01:24:01a very happily married young couple.
  • 01:24:03Whose child comma suffered a significant
  • 01:24:05brain injury at the time of birth.
  • 01:24:07They talked about what would
  • 01:24:09this child be like?
  • 01:24:10And they were kind of life be
  • 01:24:11like they were trying to counsel.
  • 01:24:13And ultimately that the parents said,
  • 01:24:14well, you know, the parents ran a pizzeria.
  • 01:24:17They lived a very happy life and their
  • 01:24:19life was working in the pizzeria
  • 01:24:21and they liked having sex.
  • 01:24:22And they said, you know,
  • 01:24:23and I'm sure the other things they
  • 01:24:25like in their life as well.
  • 01:24:25But he's trying to figure out
  • 01:24:27someone had a life worth living.
  • 01:24:28Will he be able to help make the pizza?
  • 01:24:30Sure. Well, will you be able to enjoy sex?
  • 01:24:32Sure. Well, then they for them.
  • 01:24:34Rest for potentially a Yale professor
  • 01:24:36show that might not be good enough.
  • 01:24:39So trying to understand what the
  • 01:24:40value is not of the patient but what
  • 01:24:42the values of the parents are here.
  • 01:24:43It really matters I think surrogate
  • 01:24:46decision making in for adults.
  • 01:24:48I think you're trying to be as
  • 01:24:50patient focused as you can.
  • 01:24:51But the parents values I think matter
  • 01:24:53more with smaller children when
  • 01:24:55the child obviously hasn't formed
  • 01:24:57any values but I talked to them.
  • 01:24:59We'll talk about that more at dinner.
  • 01:25:00I have one question here I promise
  • 01:25:02two more questions just very briefly
  • 01:25:03because this is some.
  • 01:25:04You might want to comment on from
  • 01:25:06my friend Alexander who
  • 01:25:07asks is there a special training program
  • 01:25:09for language mediators and the
  • 01:25:11surrogate decision making process,
  • 01:25:13or should there be?
  • 01:25:15It would include both
  • 01:25:16linguistic and cultural issues.
  • 01:25:18And how could you integrate?
  • 01:25:19Again, that's not a shortened
  • 01:25:21language mediated language media.
  • 01:25:24I don't think, well see,
  • 01:25:25I think what he's referring to is that
  • 01:25:27having both linguistic and cultural issues.
  • 01:25:29So for example someone who
  • 01:25:31not only so that the patient.
  • 01:25:34This comes from country X and
  • 01:25:35the and the language mediator
  • 01:25:37potentially not only speaks the
  • 01:25:39language of culture of country acts,
  • 01:25:41but also is familiar with that culture
  • 01:25:43and how that culture might influence
  • 01:25:45the surrogate decision makers think.
  • 01:25:50We have certain clinically.
  • 01:25:52I have certainly had the experiences.
  • 01:25:55Of individuals who are not from the
  • 01:25:57United States from from very different
  • 01:25:59cultures that decision making and
  • 01:26:01communication improves substantially.
  • 01:26:02When someone from that culture was
  • 01:26:04able to say hey there are a few things
  • 01:26:06to make sure you know when you're
  • 01:26:08engaging with with this family but so
  • 01:26:10yes absolutely that's not is there a
  • 01:26:13resource for that other than the the
  • 01:26:15resources in a particular community no.
  • 01:26:18But but availing oneself of you
  • 01:26:20know often the chaplaincy services
  • 01:26:22within hospitals can say,
  • 01:26:23you know this is either they can do the work.
  • 01:26:25Where they can find someone who is
  • 01:26:27either from that religion or from
  • 01:26:28that culture to speak to these issues.
  • 01:26:30But yes, and it's hugely important.
  • 01:26:32It's a beautiful idea, Alexander,
  • 01:26:34but here and here's Alex.
  • 01:26:35But here's the here's the loss a little bit.
  • 01:26:40That and I over time experience
  • 01:26:42that sometimes with translators
  • 01:26:43when I'm sitting and talking to
  • 01:26:45we're about to go to talk to and Mr.
  • 01:26:46Missus ex about the situation,
  • 01:26:48maybe you'll get some inside.
  • 01:26:50It doesn't usually happen.
  • 01:26:51Maybe we'll get some.
  • 01:26:52But of course most of the
  • 01:26:53time now I'm not actually
  • 01:26:54sitting there talking to translator.
  • 01:26:55I've got someone on a screen bring the
  • 01:26:56poll over with the little screen on it and
  • 01:26:58now that's how we're going
  • 01:26:59to do the translation.
  • 01:27:00So again it's it's not even in terms
  • 01:27:03of trying to a translator being able to
  • 01:27:05understand or or to translate or to.
  • 01:27:10Transmit a certain degree of empathy
  • 01:27:13that perhaps is in the language.
  • 01:27:16We lose a step because we use it
  • 01:27:17with a screen instead of a person.
  • 01:27:18But I get it because I might not have
  • 01:27:20a person here who speaks this very
  • 01:27:22unusual language in the United States.
  • 01:27:23And whereas I can get somebody does it,
  • 01:27:25so I get it.
  • 01:27:26It's the technology helps me,
  • 01:27:28but sometimes it's a piece
  • 01:27:29loss. But I want it. I'm sorry, Doctor
  • 01:27:30Junick, I want you to have the last question.
  • 01:27:32So my question is related to, again,
  • 01:27:35feeling the surrogacy as a burden as many
  • 01:27:38spending many years on our Ethics Committee.
  • 01:27:41In the hospital when we've
  • 01:27:43invoked the nuclear option,
  • 01:27:44the conscientious practice policy,
  • 01:27:46it's not been uncommon where a family
  • 01:27:48that we've dealt with for many,
  • 01:27:50many years, when we took the
  • 01:27:52decision out of their hands,
  • 01:27:54they felt very relieved.
  • 01:27:55And so I'm sort of focusing way back
  • 01:27:57on one of their first slides about how
  • 01:27:59I'll leave about 1% of patients have
  • 01:28:01something that's actionable one way.
  • 01:28:04Obviously an Ed we're we want all
  • 01:28:06of our patients to come in with
  • 01:28:09directives and some of the IO your
  • 01:28:106 year olds who are walking down the
  • 01:28:12street and they have a massive MI.
  • 01:28:13But the vast majority of these
  • 01:28:15patients it is relatively predictable.
  • 01:28:17Our ALS patients are stage four
  • 01:28:20cancer patients that have not
  • 01:28:22responded appropriately to chemo.
  • 01:28:24It's all predictable that this
  • 01:28:25is going to happen and to us and
  • 01:28:27D it's entirely frustrating not
  • 01:28:28that not we're not frustrated the
  • 01:28:30patients we're frustrated at a lot
  • 01:28:32of the other care providers.
  • 01:28:34We're not having these
  • 01:28:35conversations with patients.
  • 01:28:36So I'm wondering if maybe having these
  • 01:28:39more emotional support resources
  • 01:28:41directly directed towards patients
  • 01:28:43when having these conversations.
  • 01:28:46And again it's going to be a process,
  • 01:28:47it's never one conversation as Sarah
  • 01:28:49says with one patient that one day,
  • 01:28:51but over a period of three or six
  • 01:28:53months or three or six years as they
  • 01:28:55go through ALS or something having
  • 01:28:57you know supporting that patient and
  • 01:28:59making their own decisions and that
  • 01:29:01not putting that burden on the on the.
  • 01:29:04Caregivers,
  • 01:29:05which would ruin your whole talk.
  • 01:29:06But
  • 01:29:06yeah, no. So this is this is the
  • 01:29:09whole idea of making surrogate
  • 01:29:11decision making easier by making
  • 01:29:13sure that patients have really had
  • 01:29:16the opportunity and the support to
  • 01:29:18truly formulate their own preferences
  • 01:29:20about the likely care decision.
  • 01:29:22But then you wouldn't have to ask
  • 01:29:24much of anything if it's, you know,
  • 01:29:26again in the alley ALS cases and in
  • 01:29:27advanced cancer patients because it's
  • 01:29:29relatively or advanced liver patients
  • 01:29:31who don't qualify for transplant,
  • 01:29:32it's relatively predictable
  • 01:29:33what's going to happen.
  • 01:29:35Yeah. So that would argue for really
  • 01:29:37somewhat of a disease specific kind of
  • 01:29:39advanced care planning or counseling session.
  • 01:29:41And so Yale Shankar and my colleague at
  • 01:29:44PET is the lead on a trial right now,
  • 01:29:46randomized trial of respecting choices,
  • 01:29:48which is a more interpersonally attuned
  • 01:29:51form of advanced care planning.
  • 01:29:53And she's testing it in patients
  • 01:29:55with advanced cancer who that like
  • 01:29:57just like you said, reliably face
  • 01:29:59certain kinds of milestone decisions.
  • 01:30:01And so, you know we'll see.
  • 01:30:03I think there are so many studies.
  • 01:30:05Have shown that advanced care planning
  • 01:30:08does not particularly affect health
  • 01:30:10utilization outcomes and care decisions.
  • 01:30:13This is, you know,
  • 01:30:15a much more tailored approach to it,
  • 01:30:16a much more interpersonally savvy
  • 01:30:18advanced care planning intervention rather
  • 01:30:20than just a pen and paper directive.
  • 01:30:22I was supporting the patients,
  • 01:30:24but as soon as you see how many
  • 01:30:26countries say, well,
  • 01:30:27let's not leave this to the family
  • 01:30:29anyway that that I wasn't aware
  • 01:30:31of that was we're friends but not
  • 01:30:32the other countries as well.
  • 01:30:33Don't move as we have you know you know
  • 01:30:35you get wonderful things when you come here,
  • 01:30:37dog.
  • 01:30:39You get, you know,
  • 01:30:40you get some Yale paraphernalia.
  • 01:30:41Oh, please join me in thanking Doctor White.
  • 01:30:46Yeah. We got everything we can.
  • 01:30:48We can feel free to wear this
  • 01:30:50right now if you want. Yeah, sure.
  • 01:30:52Oh, yeah. You're looking good.
  • 01:30:54Thank you very much.
  • 01:30:57Stretch out my 17 year old
  • 01:30:58twins who are thinking about
  • 01:31:00colleges now. A couple.
  • 01:31:02There you go. That'll stress them out.
  • 01:31:04Good. Thank you all very much for coming.
  • 01:31:06We'll see you guys again
  • 01:31:07in a couple weeks.