Skip to Main Content

"The Cost of Insufficient Sleep" Janet Mullington (12.09.2020)

December 14, 2020
  • 00:00Alright. So in 2014, the Sleep Research
  • 00:08Society and the American Academy of
  • 00:12Sleep Medicine got together too.
  • 00:16At the encouragement actually of
  • 00:19Michael Query, who is the head
  • 00:22of the Center for Sleep Disorders
  • 00:25Research at Heart, Lung and Blood?
  • 00:29As the director of that? I.
  • 00:35Component of heart, lung and blood.
  • 00:38Michael Query had been asking for.
  • 00:43Position paper on how much sleep is
  • 00:47actually recommended so he he encouraged
  • 00:51the societies and we got together a
  • 00:56number of individuals to participate in.
  • 01:00Uh. A consensus meeting there.
  • 01:05There were many involved in this
  • 01:09panel and the product of that work
  • 01:13was published in Sleep in 2015 and
  • 01:17it was really a landmark consensus
  • 01:20piece and you can see here that there
  • 01:25were several different areas covered.
  • 01:28They reviewed a wealth of literature.
  • 01:32From epidemiological to physiological
  • 01:34data on the effects of insufficient
  • 01:37sleep or hours of sleep on these
  • 01:40different parameters,
  • 01:41including general health,
  • 01:42cardiovascular health, metabolic health,
  • 01:44mental health, immune function,
  • 01:46human performance, breast cancer,
  • 01:48pain and mortality.
  • 01:49Ann on all of these except for breast cancer.
  • 01:54As you can see here,
  • 01:56the hours of recommended sleep or the
  • 01:59hours where there was most agreement.
  • 02:02That that this amount of sleep 7 to 8
  • 02:06hours was the optimal is shown here in green,
  • 02:11so the consensus ended up stating
  • 02:14that a minimum of 7 to 8 hours
  • 02:18of sleep is recommended.
  • 02:20As we can see over here,
  • 02:23there is some disagreement
  • 02:25in the higher range,
  • 02:27but in epidemiological research at least
  • 02:30we have seen that over 10 hours is.
  • 02:35Is thought to be too much.
  • 02:37However,
  • 02:38comorbidities and age are
  • 02:40important to factor in there,
  • 02:42so in terms of the bottom line,
  • 02:45one in three American adults doesn't
  • 02:48get the recommended amount of sleep.
  • 02:50And here you can see a prevalence
  • 02:54map and you can see that.
  • 02:57From 2024.3% all the way to 48.5%
  • 03:01of the population in different
  • 03:04counties across the country have
  • 03:07insufficient sleep less than 7 hours
  • 03:10of sleep on average per night.
  • 03:14This has been monitored by the Center
  • 03:17for Disease Control and there the
  • 03:21Behavioral Risk factor surveillance
  • 03:24questions have got some items on sleep and.
  • 03:28Anybody can actually access those
  • 03:31databases there and report from
  • 03:342016 showed us more about the
  • 03:37economic costs of insufficient
  • 03:39sleep across 5 OECD countries,
  • 03:42and you can see here that the gross.
  • 03:49The GDP is influenced by
  • 03:55insufficient sleep that 2.28%.
  • 04:01Is the amount of GDP that's loss due to
  • 04:05insufficient sleep according to this report?
  • 04:09And this ranges from 1.35 in
  • 04:13Canada to 2.9 in Japan, but.
  • 04:161.2 million working days are lost.
  • 04:20It's estimated each year
  • 04:22in the United States,
  • 04:24so this clearly has an impact on economics.
  • 04:31In in our country and in
  • 04:34other Western nations. She.
  • 04:38Healthy Sleep Awareness program was
  • 04:41actually a program that was developed
  • 04:44in partnership with the Center for
  • 04:47Disease Control and 2013 fourteen.
  • 04:50They put out an announcement for
  • 04:53a competition or excuse me for
  • 04:57Grant to be supporting education,
  • 04:59public education so the Sleep
  • 05:02Research Society and the American
  • 05:06Academy of Sleep Medicine got
  • 05:09together as founding partners.
  • 05:11Developed this education
  • 05:14program with the CDC.
  • 05:17And. This.
  • 05:27This educational group put
  • 05:30together they had a communications.
  • 05:34A communications subcommittee that
  • 05:36worked on these infographics together
  • 05:40with the Academy and you can see here,
  • 05:43from economic costs,
  • 05:45through the importance of drawing of sleep
  • 05:49for drowsy driving for cardiovascular health.
  • 05:53Another one that was put out for sleep
  • 05:57awareness around Halloween and then
  • 06:00another that was developed for kids and.
  • 06:05Particularly adolescents and these,
  • 06:07together with some of the other so
  • 06:11social media led to 4.2 billion clicks.
  • 06:14And so this program was very effective
  • 06:18in helping the educate the public
  • 06:22about the importance of sleep.
  • 06:25I wanted to talk now a little bit about some
  • 06:29of the experimental work that we've done,
  • 06:32so we have tried to do highly
  • 06:35controlled experimental models
  • 06:37looking at the effects of insufficient
  • 06:39sleep and this is Monica hacks work.
  • 06:42Monica is an associate professor
  • 06:44in the Department of Neurology
  • 06:46at our institution at Beth Israel
  • 06:49Deaconess Medical Center,
  • 06:50and she's done a lot of research looking at.
  • 06:56The question as to whether or not
  • 06:58we can adapt to insufficient sleep,
  • 07:01and here you see, you know some plot that
  • 07:04shows what somebody who's working too
  • 07:07much might actually experience this is
  • 07:104 hours of sleep followed by a weekend
  • 07:13where they try and catch up sleep,
  • 07:16and sometimes people can do this
  • 07:18week after, week after week,
  • 07:20and we think that we adapt to this, but.
  • 07:25Monica wanted to look and see
  • 07:27exactly how does this affect
  • 07:29our Physiology in our health.
  • 07:31Health risk parameters,
  • 07:33so we know that that stress affects the
  • 07:38hypothalamic pituitary adrenal system,
  • 07:41the immune system and the
  • 07:45sympathoadrenal system.
  • 07:47We know that cortisol inhibits
  • 07:50monocyte production of aisle 6.
  • 07:53We know that I'll six production when
  • 07:57monocytes are stimulated it increases.
  • 08:00We know that norepinephrine can
  • 08:04also stimulate the production
  • 08:07by a monocytes and I all six.
  • 08:11Can cause a or an immune response.
  • 08:14Causes cortisol to go up.
  • 08:17So Monica was studying week after week.
  • 08:20The effects of insufficient sleep
  • 08:21and what you can see here is that
  • 08:24the end of week one there's actually
  • 08:27two nights of sleep recovery sleep.
  • 08:29What you see is that from from baseline,
  • 08:33which is actually not shown here.
  • 08:35But there's three nights of baseline
  • 08:37before this recording is done,
  • 08:39so they've been sleeping 8 hours a night,
  • 08:42or had that opportunity.
  • 08:44These are normally 8 hours sleepers who
  • 08:47we bring into the clinical Research Center.
  • 08:49Um, then?
  • 08:50What you see here is that
  • 08:53for the first few nights,
  • 08:56there's no significant.
  • 08:58There's a drifting upwards,
  • 08:59but no significant increase in
  • 09:02the aisle 6 positive monocytes.
  • 09:05Percentage and then what you
  • 09:07see is that during the next.
  • 09:10Bout of insufficient sleep this is the aisle.
  • 09:136 positive monocytes are increased
  • 09:15and then after you have that second
  • 09:18that second week you're having
  • 09:20recovery sleep on the weekend.
  • 09:23Those levels are still elevated
  • 09:25and again after the next week they
  • 09:28are still elevated and recovery
  • 09:30sleep is not really doing a good
  • 09:33job returning it back to baseline.
  • 09:35What you note here is that the
  • 09:38cortisol is also increased so.
  • 09:41Quarters all, as I said,
  • 09:43should inhibit the monocyte product
  • 09:47productivity production of the
  • 09:49aisle 6 and it is increasing,
  • 09:52so that's a counterregulatory if
  • 09:55you will immune type response.
  • 09:58And what you see here is that.
  • 10:01The ability of the dexamethasone
  • 10:07to dexamethasone being.
  • 10:10Inhibitory steroid,
  • 10:11the ability of this dexamethasone
  • 10:15to reduce the production of IL.
  • 10:186 drops so the glucocorticoid
  • 10:22sensitivity is increasing in
  • 10:24response to the sleep deprivation.
  • 10:29Anet is not returning to baseline
  • 10:33after a weekend of of sleep.
  • 10:37So cutting back to half half the
  • 10:41amount of sleep you need during
  • 10:43the week and trying to catch up
  • 10:47with just two nights of eight hours
  • 10:50of sleep on the weekend is not
  • 10:53doing it for your immune system.
  • 10:56So Monica and Larissa and heart here,
  • 10:59who is a postdoctoral fellow
  • 11:01working with Monica currently in
  • 11:04our lab has has been investigating
  • 11:07these resolvins and resolvins.
  • 11:08Are an active component of the
  • 11:13immune system that actively tries
  • 11:16to resolve the inflammation
  • 11:19and stop the recruitment of.
  • 11:23Immune.
  • 11:28Immune factors. So here you see that
  • 11:34edema is very quick and then this
  • 11:38inflammation infiltration of white blood
  • 11:41cells occurs within minutes and then
  • 11:46this monocyte macrophage production
  • 11:48of inflammatory signals and counter
  • 11:52regulatory active resolution factors takes
  • 11:55a longer amount of time, hours to days.
  • 12:00These omega-3 fatty acid.
  • 12:03Components are converted to specialized pro,
  • 12:06resolving mediators that you see here.
  • 12:10In these graphs there's RV,
  • 12:13D3, RV D4, RV D5 and 17HD HA.
  • 12:17These are all.
  • 12:20Part of these resolving mediators,
  • 12:23we're exploring them at this point
  • 12:26and I've just selected three that
  • 12:30for rather that are that are showing
  • 12:33quite interesting patterns here
  • 12:35of of stability with the control
  • 12:39sleep and a drop in production.
  • 12:44Associated with insufficient sleep,
  • 12:47and this is showing insufficient
  • 12:50sleep after 14 days of being in a
  • 12:53protocol that involves fragmenting
  • 12:55nocturnal sleep and allowing
  • 12:58interdigitated recovery that that's a
  • 13:013 three days of insufficient sleep,
  • 13:04followed by recovery of one
  • 13:07night and this is repeated.
  • 13:10So this is a partial sleep deprivation model.
  • 13:14That is a recurrent exposure
  • 13:17to the sleep deprivation,
  • 13:18and then when you allow after that sleep.
  • 13:22What you seeing here is that this these
  • 13:25rez resolving mediators are not jumping
  • 13:28right back up to baseline levels,
  • 13:31so it's taking longer for this resolution to
  • 13:34occur in the context of sleep deprivation.
  • 13:40So these models are based on pretty acute.
  • 13:45God changes and we know that
  • 13:48in cardiovascular disease,
  • 13:50risk for cardiovascular disease.
  • 13:52This is increased when CRP levels
  • 13:56are even just mildly elevated,
  • 13:59and so we were wanting to further
  • 14:02examine the autonomic involvement.
  • 14:05One of the hypothesis is that the
  • 14:09inflammation that we see in shorter term.
  • 14:14I sleep deprivation may be a stress
  • 14:19response and may be related to.
  • 14:23I. Two shear stresses,
  • 14:27and so we wanted to look at vascular
  • 14:31tone and to look at some of the.
  • 14:35Inflammatory regulation mediators.
  • 14:41We conducted a study here you can see
  • 14:44a four hour condition in an 8 hour
  • 14:47condition in the four hour condition.
  • 14:50The participants were kept awake until
  • 14:533:00 o'clock and allowed to sleep from
  • 14:563 until seven and this is a 22 day
  • 14:59protocol an we studied heavy recording
  • 15:02days are indicated here in the green
  • 15:05and on these heavy recording days
  • 15:08we did beat to beat blood pressure.
  • 15:11And we also recorded continuous e.g.
  • 15:15And we had vascular reactivity
  • 15:19testing during the day.
  • 15:22Following the last recording
  • 15:26from each of these blocks.
  • 15:30We also measured mediators
  • 15:32before bed and after bed.
  • 15:36So here you can see the blood pressure
  • 15:39data and you see that that the control
  • 15:43is shown here in the in the black line
  • 15:46and the dotted or the hatched line.
  • 15:49Here is the sleep deprivation.
  • 15:51The partial sleep deprivation and these are
  • 15:54showing the first block and second block,
  • 15:57third block and 4th block and
  • 15:59then the recovery.
  • 16:00So what you'll notice here is
  • 16:03that the systolic blood pressure
  • 16:05is elevated and the diastolic.
  • 16:07Blood pressure is elevated and
  • 16:10the heart rate is.
  • 16:12Is is elevated in the restriction
  • 16:15condition and these are daily averages?
  • 16:19I will show you some of the
  • 16:23tracings but here you see the.
  • 16:26This is the Porter press system.
  • 16:29There's a block worn on the wrist,
  • 16:32and the finger cuffs inflate and
  • 16:35deflate at 15 minute intervals.
  • 16:37We use so that it's reasonably
  • 16:40comfortable and participants
  • 16:41can be awake and performing or.
  • 16:46Doing different activities in the day it is
  • 16:49actually marketed as an ambulatory system,
  • 16:51although I wouldn't want to try that.
  • 16:54I we have people stay with this
  • 16:56system in the lab and they can
  • 16:59sleep with this through the night,
  • 17:01and our hope is that we actually.
  • 17:05Do not disrupt sleep as much as we
  • 17:08would with something like spacelabs.
  • 17:11We did do a little bit of a head
  • 17:14to head comparison and didn't
  • 17:17find a lot of difference in fact,
  • 17:20but in any case,
  • 17:22with this 24 hour blood pressure
  • 17:24we did repeated repeated cycles.
  • 17:27As you saw a moment ago and what we
  • 17:30saw is that the sleep restriction
  • 17:33led to elevation of blood pressure.
  • 17:37When permitted to sleep in the
  • 17:39short sleep condition,
  • 17:40you can see here in the red
  • 17:43bar at the bottom,
  • 17:44the participants had elevated blood
  • 17:46pressure compared to when they were
  • 17:49allowed to sleep earlier in the night.
  • 17:51So the slow wave actually was
  • 17:54quite well preserved in the
  • 17:56first part of the night here,
  • 17:58but they did not get down as far
  • 18:01as they did in normal sleep.
  • 18:05And the recovery is here in the tracing
  • 18:08at the bottom here in the green.
  • 18:11So they go pretty quickly back to
  • 18:14the range of where they were in
  • 18:18baseline in the first recovery.
  • 18:21This is actually the second recovery night,
  • 18:23but I'll get into that later.
  • 18:25But in any case,
  • 18:27what I wanted to sorry what I
  • 18:30wanted to show you here is the.
  • 18:33Period from 8:00 PM until 3:00 AM
  • 18:38and what you can see is that the.
  • 18:43Later cycles the 3rd and 4th cycle of
  • 18:46sleep restriction showed a decrease,
  • 18:48so it starts the blood.
  • 18:50Blood pressure tries to go down
  • 18:52if you will eat earlier,
  • 18:54and this is after the meal where
  • 18:57usually you have and you can see
  • 19:00a little bit of a peak here in
  • 19:03the black and meal associated Pick
  • 19:06peak here after lunch.
  • 19:08But in the evening this blood pressure
  • 19:11was starting to decrease at 8:00 PM.
  • 19:13Long before they get into bed,
  • 19:15they got into bed at 10:30 PM and
  • 19:18lights were out at 11 actually.
  • 19:20And So what you'll notice is that
  • 19:23the blood pressure begins to
  • 19:24decrease and then it at bedtime.
  • 19:26It does go up and there is a
  • 19:29pre sleep increasing in blood
  • 19:30pressure that we see here as well.
  • 19:33In this short sleep condition.
  • 19:36We looked at that period between
  • 19:388:00 and 3:00 and we found no
  • 19:41difference in subjective sleepiness.
  • 19:43We had thought maybe they were
  • 19:45getting tired and that's why blood
  • 19:48pressure was dropping early and
  • 19:50we saw no increase in microsleeps
  • 19:53we we had the EG and looked and
  • 19:56scored through and didn't find
  • 19:58that there were more microsleep.
  • 20:00In the 8:00 to 8:00 PM to bedtime,
  • 20:04whether that was eleven,
  • 20:06there was no difference there,
  • 20:09so that was 11 for the sleep condition.
  • 20:15So we we looked at, I'm sorry.
  • 20:21So we. We wanted to look at the recovery
  • 20:26sleep also for homeostatic regulation
  • 20:29of sleep and and autonomic involvement.
  • 20:33So we have done additional
  • 20:35studies and just to give you a
  • 20:39little bit of the background,
  • 20:42we all know that with the
  • 20:45homeostatic regulation of sleep,
  • 20:47there's increased sleep pressure
  • 20:49and when participants are sleep
  • 20:52deprived and allowed to sleep again.
  • 20:55There's an increase in slow
  • 20:58wave sleep and and Delta power,
  • 21:01and there's one study only that showing
  • 21:04only one study that looked at heart
  • 21:09rate variability in the recovery.
  • 21:11In sleep during recovery sleep
  • 21:15following the sleep deprivation and
  • 21:18Glos and colleagues showed that
  • 21:21the low frequency high frequency,
  • 21:24so this is showing the
  • 21:28sympathetic predominant.
  • 21:33The increase of parasympathetic predominant.
  • 21:41Heart rate variability
  • 21:42following sleep deprivation.
  • 21:44So during recovery sleep there is an
  • 21:47increase and you can see that very nicely
  • 21:50here in jurist as beautiful illustration,
  • 21:54showing the sleep histogram here with
  • 21:57slow wave sleep and REM sleep and
  • 22:01what use notice here is the Delta
  • 22:04power coinciding with this low.
  • 22:07Obviously with the slow wave sleep,
  • 22:09but the high frequency Spectra
  • 22:12of of the heart.
  • 22:14Rate the high frequency is shown down
  • 22:18here on panel C and it is coinciding
  • 22:22it with actually the Delta power
  • 22:25and the peak in the high frequency,
  • 22:29the autonomic.
  • 22:32Parasympathetic peak,
  • 22:33if you will, is in advance of
  • 22:36the Delta power by a little bit.
  • 22:39The Delta Power does show an
  • 22:42increase here in advance of that,
  • 22:46so clearly there is a regulation
  • 22:49with the sympathetic parasympathetic
  • 22:52balance and those sleep stages.
  • 22:55So we wanted to look in the recovery
  • 22:59sleep following the short sleep
  • 23:02boats and look for any rebound.
  • 23:05We did not have a recording on
  • 23:09the 1st Recovery Night because
  • 23:12as I showed you earlier,
  • 23:15we were recording the heavy
  • 23:17recording was done on the third
  • 23:20night of the deprivation throughout
  • 23:23these cycles and then.
  • 23:26The first recovery night was always
  • 23:28without EG to give participants a
  • 23:30break and then we did the full each
  • 23:33G and beat to beat blood pressure
  • 23:36monitoring again on recovery
  • 23:37night 2IN Recovery Night 3.
  • 23:42And we were interested in looking at
  • 23:45the spontaneous cardio, vagal BRS,
  • 23:48Barros reflect sensitivity,
  • 23:50an we used a sequence method,
  • 23:53and we were looking at the up
  • 23:56sequence so when the up up sequence
  • 23:59when blood pressure goes up the
  • 24:02heart rate should come down and when
  • 24:06blood pressure goes down the heart
  • 24:10rate should go up the RR interval.
  • 24:14Response to the blood pressure and what we
  • 24:18saw is that the normalized high frequency.
  • 24:22So this is the parasympathetic
  • 24:24response to sleep loss in that second
  • 24:28recovery night we see here quite a
  • 24:32pronounced increase this these data
  • 24:34that I'm showing you here on this slide
  • 24:38are from the first hour of sleep.
  • 24:42On the baseline and then the recovery sleep.
  • 24:47Not the recovery sleep.
  • 24:49Sorry the third night of sleep deprivation.
  • 24:52And then finally the second recovery night.
  • 24:55So we would expect that these
  • 24:58first nights of.
  • 25:00Partial sleep deprivation that
  • 25:02these nights of partial sleep
  • 25:05deprivation might show a rebound.
  • 25:08So I'm very interested to know what the
  • 25:12first recovery full night would look like,
  • 25:15but what we see is the recovery too.
  • 25:19Has the increase in the high
  • 25:22frequency and also the BRS down down.
  • 25:26So when the down the the blood pressure.
  • 25:30Is going down there.
  • 25:32Heart rate is more more responsive here
  • 25:36and also the Delta power is increased on the.
  • 25:42On each of those nights, as one would expect.
  • 25:46This shows you the correlation and
  • 25:48the correlation for the baseline night
  • 25:51as well as the recovery nights shows
  • 25:54a very tight correlation between the
  • 25:56high frequency and the slow wave sleep,
  • 26:00just as seen here in the juristic data.
  • 26:04So what what is it about?
  • 26:08Restoration, Ann? Can we?
  • 26:11Can we further understand the recovery
  • 26:15sleep by looking at subjective
  • 26:18indices and we know that 20 to 30%
  • 26:22of patients seek attention in primary
  • 26:26care settings for significant fatigue
  • 26:29and over 700 million office visits
  • 26:32per year in the United States.
  • 26:35Are are with presenting concerns
  • 26:38about fatigue and sleep.
  • 26:40Is sleepiness and fatigue as we know
  • 26:44are produced by insufficient sleep.
  • 26:48We we decided to compare the data
  • 26:50that I was just talking about
  • 26:53with the four hours sleep.
  • 26:55Three nights of insufficient sleep
  • 26:57followed by recovery repeated four times
  • 26:59and then three nights of recovery sleep.
  • 27:01Compare that with another protocol
  • 27:03where we gave 4 hours of sleep,
  • 27:06but we spread it out and this is again.
  • 27:09This is 3 cycles and we're comparing
  • 27:11the the three cycles first three
  • 27:14cycles in the four hour condition
  • 27:16and what we did here was allow.
  • 27:1940 minutes of sleep and
  • 27:2120 minutes of wakefulness,
  • 27:23so the total amount of sleep is
  • 27:264 hours here and is also four
  • 27:29hours in the upper graph.
  • 27:34What what I'm showing here is the visual
  • 27:37analog scale ratings of sleepiness.
  • 27:40You can see that that each cycle
  • 27:43of insufficient sleep causes an
  • 27:45increase in sleepiness as we expect
  • 27:48the consolidated short sleep.
  • 27:50The four hours of sleep is shown here,
  • 27:54and the fragmented 4 hours are shown here.
  • 27:59What you'll notice is that there's an
  • 28:02increase in the base in the baseline.
  • 28:05This is after recovery sleep.
  • 28:07There is still an elevation
  • 28:09of sleepiness the next day,
  • 28:11and this is showing fatigue.
  • 28:13This is the fragmented sleep,
  • 28:15and this is the consolidated short sleep.
  • 28:18And these are the levels of eight hours of
  • 28:22of sleep that you see in Gray at the bottom.
  • 28:27Of particular interest, I think,
  • 28:29is that if you compare what
  • 28:32happens on the day after,
  • 28:34so this is showing the baseline and then
  • 28:37what about one night of recovery sleep
  • 28:40following either the fragmented short
  • 28:43sleep or the consolidated short sleep?
  • 28:46What you'll see here in the red?
  • 28:49That the fragmented short sleep leads to
  • 28:52a much greater impairment the next day,
  • 28:55so this is in all of these.
  • 28:59I'm showing you what it looks like after
  • 29:01they've had recovery sleep subjectively,
  • 29:04so they're reporting elevated
  • 29:06levels of subjective sleepiness
  • 29:08even after recovery sleep.
  • 29:11Following the fragmented but not
  • 29:13the consolidated short sleep.
  • 29:15So they're feeling much worse,
  • 29:17and this is persisting even
  • 29:19after the second night.
  • 29:21It's not significant anymore,
  • 29:23but you still see some elevation.
  • 29:26However,
  • 29:27with fatigue,
  • 29:27and this is showing this same thing
  • 29:31with the fatigue self report the the
  • 29:34fragmented short sleep causes an
  • 29:37elevation in fatigue that persists even
  • 29:39on the third night after recovery sleep.
  • 29:44So the fatigue and sleepiness here
  • 29:48which you'll notice is that they
  • 29:52are separable in in the post.
  • 29:55Deprivation exposure when they
  • 29:57are recovering. They they are.
  • 30:00Separating fatigue and sleepiness and
  • 30:03the sleepiness system more quickly resolved.
  • 30:08So what about translational?
  • 30:09I wanted to talk a little bit about
  • 30:12translational opportunities for the field an.
  • 30:15We've done a study,
  • 30:17so I've been talking with you about,
  • 30:20you know the effects of.
  • 30:23Insufficient sleep on blood pressure.
  • 30:26Can we help people with hypertension
  • 30:29by increasing their sleep duration?
  • 30:32And this study is ongoing,
  • 30:34but.
  • 30:36I just want to show you a little bit of
  • 30:40preliminary data and I can tell you we don't.
  • 30:43We're not unblinded yet,
  • 30:45so we don't know which.
  • 30:48With the data I'm going to show you,
  • 30:51we don't know which condition
  • 30:53that that the participants are in,
  • 30:55but we randomized to a sleep
  • 30:57extension or asleep timing and we're
  • 30:59hoping to look at sex differences.
  • 31:01Covid has really had an impact on
  • 31:04our ability to run this study,
  • 31:06so we won't have as many subjects
  • 31:08as we had hoped,
  • 31:10but we're still hoping to look at
  • 31:13sex differences as well. Alright.
  • 31:18We are.
  • 31:21We advertised for individuals who had
  • 31:25hypertensive hypertension and this
  • 31:28is not a high level of hypertension.
  • 31:32We're looking for participants
  • 31:35with less than Stage 2 or 160 / 100
  • 31:40and yet still over the 120 / 80.
  • 31:46So in the in the pre hypertension and stage
  • 31:50one phase one hypertension if you will.
  • 31:55We we. Determine what their regular time
  • 32:00is based on a couple of weeks of sleep.
  • 32:04Log in, act graph and we then design.
  • 32:10What is an increased amount of time
  • 32:13for them by an hour or we maintain
  • 32:17the time that they most usually?
  • 32:20They usually would go to bed and get up.
  • 32:25So we either maintain or we extend.
  • 32:28But in both cases it's based on their
  • 32:33circadian placement and duration of
  • 32:36time prior to coming into the study.
  • 32:39So I'm going to show you as I said some.
  • 32:43Preliminary data,
  • 32:44but essentially we have that evaluation
  • 32:46phase with the screening visit and
  • 32:49then they do an overnight stay where
  • 32:51we do beat to beat blood pressure,
  • 32:54an EEG recording.
  • 32:55Then they go home and they are
  • 32:58on a wait list.
  • 32:59This is a wait list control for this study.
  • 33:03They come back and repeat the same thing.
  • 33:06We did this because we were concerned
  • 33:09that maybe being in a study,
  • 33:11maybe the adaptation to the.
  • 33:14Stressful condition within the being in
  • 33:17clinical Research Center might have affects,
  • 33:20so we're interested in
  • 33:22this overnight stay too,
  • 33:25as well as the overnight stay 3 where
  • 33:29we evaluate whether the sleep extension
  • 33:33or the sleep timing condition improves.
  • 33:37New blood pressure and this work
  • 33:40that I'm going to show you has been.
  • 33:44Can analyze Dan process by.
  • 33:49Quan Yang and Michael Vasquez,
  • 33:52who are shown here.
  • 33:56And here I'm showing you the blood pressure,
  • 34:01beatbeat blood pressure and the
  • 34:04heart rate tracings.
  • 34:06And with the Valsalva maneuver what we
  • 34:10what we do is instruct the individual
  • 34:14to take a deep breath and this is
  • 34:19the early phase here early phase two
  • 34:23where the individual is holding their breath.
  • 34:27It's inspiring and holding their breath.
  • 34:32And then when they released their breath,
  • 34:35we see a drop in blood pressure
  • 34:39corresponding with the peak in the ECG,
  • 34:42and then as the blood pressure
  • 34:45comes back to normal,
  • 34:47then the heart rate goes back down.
  • 34:50We see we're in phase four here and
  • 34:54we look at the the responsibility
  • 34:57of the part to this.
  • 34:59These changes in blood pressure.
  • 35:02So we're looking at the baroreflex
  • 35:04sensitivity in the data that I will show you.
  • 35:10Alright, so here what we're looking at is
  • 35:13the slope and you see that the early phase.
  • 35:17So this phase over here.
  • 35:20Over sorry over here the slope is not
  • 35:24changing between state one and State 2,
  • 35:28but then with stage state three is increased.
  • 35:34And the blood pressure maximum
  • 35:36is decreasing on Stage 3.
  • 35:39Not a lot of difference
  • 35:41between stage one and two,
  • 35:44but the blood pressure Max during
  • 35:47the early phase is decreased on
  • 35:50stage three and the phase four.
  • 35:54So the responsiveness the slope
  • 35:57is increased through each state.
  • 35:59The most important for our study will
  • 36:03be stay two to stay 3 and you see there
  • 36:08is still an increase showing here,
  • 36:12but it's not reached
  • 36:14statistical significance.
  • 36:15This is, as I said,
  • 36:18a combination of both conditions were
  • 36:21not unblinded yet, so we don't know.
  • 36:25Who's in what condition?
  • 36:27But we can see that there is a
  • 36:31signal also with the BP Max dropping,
  • 36:35so the the system is getting more
  • 36:39responsive with the sleep conditions and.
  • 36:43It it we consider the stabilization
  • 36:45of sleep as an active control.
  • 36:48So the fact that we are seeing
  • 36:50anything here is encouraging.
  • 36:52And if we have a difference between our
  • 36:56conditions that will also be important.
  • 36:59Of course for the sleep extension we
  • 37:01expected to see a greater effect,
  • 37:04but having any effects here compared to.
  • 37:08Overnight stays one in particularly
  • 37:11overnight stays.
  • 37:12Two will will be of interest for
  • 37:15sleeping circadian scientists.
  • 37:17So what about future directions for
  • 37:19behaviorally based interventions
  • 37:21to improve sleep?
  • 37:22Well,
  • 37:23we know there's a lot of work that has
  • 37:27been done and more to be done with CBT eyes.
  • 37:32Cognitive behavioral therapies for insomnia.
  • 37:34Sleep extension we've,
  • 37:36we,
  • 37:37and others have been working with.
  • 37:40Some studies have started to look at
  • 37:43more at breathing and meditation,
  • 37:46and this is just showing you Juan
  • 37:50Yang and Michael Goldstein,
  • 37:52who one is an instructor in our group.
  • 37:56And Michael Goldstein is a postdoc who's
  • 38:00recently joined us and they applied
  • 38:04for and were awarded Oscher Center
  • 38:07grant to look at slow paced breathing.
  • 38:10And mindfulness,
  • 38:12they are trying to differentiate the.
  • 38:17Effects on.
  • 38:18On blood pressure with slow paced
  • 38:21breathing or mindfulness trying
  • 38:23to tease apart these effects so
  • 38:26people will be doing mindfulness.
  • 38:29Yoga's yoga,
  • 38:30breathing plus mindfulness or just
  • 38:32the slow paced breathing and they're
  • 38:35hoping to tease apart the effects
  • 38:38on blood pressure with these three
  • 38:41different usually if court with
  • 38:43these three different approaches.
  • 38:45Usually of course we see apps in a lot of.
  • 38:49Advertising too.
  • 38:52To combine these and it's not
  • 38:54really known what is the relative
  • 38:56contribution of each,
  • 38:57so I'm looking forward to seeing
  • 39:00the results of those studies.
  • 39:02I've, I think that there's it's an.
  • 39:05It's an ideal time to do research
  • 39:08in the home,
  • 39:09and Tele medicine has really taken off
  • 39:11even even more during the pandemic,
  • 39:14and I think that it is developing
  • 39:17some opportunities for us in the
  • 39:19field to do more research in the
  • 39:21home with ambulatory monitoring methods.
  • 39:24Tele medicine. As I said,
  • 39:26I think can augment augment
  • 39:28some of that so we can do some.
  • 39:34Work in interacting with participants
  • 39:37with through Tele medicine and OPT based
  • 39:41interventions have been and are being
  • 39:45developed now and can integrate then the
  • 39:48sleep log and sound or motion detection.
  • 39:51To be able to create different interventions
  • 39:55that may be helpful in improving sleep,
  • 39:59consolidating sleep.
  • 40:00For insomnia, for instance,
  • 40:03maybe even helping to reduce blood pressure.
  • 40:07So I wanted to just acknowledge that
  • 40:10our team and our our funding sources.
  • 40:14This is Monica Hack and she is doing a lot
  • 40:19of different models as I've shown you.
  • 40:24Looking at sleep affects and immune function.
  • 40:27Michael Goldstein is a clinical
  • 40:30psychologist who's working.
  • 40:32With us, and as I showed you,
  • 40:34is going to be starting to do
  • 40:37those that Oshir grant.
  • 40:38Looking at mindfulness
  • 40:39and breathing and sleep.
  • 40:41And blood pressure and Larissa Angert
  • 40:44doing a lot of work now on the resolvins.
  • 40:49And she's fellow,
  • 40:51who's with us now from from Germany.
  • 40:54And this is 1 Yang who is an
  • 40:58instructor in neurology in our group,
  • 41:02and she has currently a HK early
  • 41:06career investigator and Sleep Research
  • 41:09Society grant to look at some of the.
  • 41:13Um?
  • 41:15Some of the renal aspects of blood
  • 41:18pressure control and sleep deprivation,
  • 41:20so thank you very much for your attention.
  • 41:24I will.
  • 41:26Stop it there and I just need to.
  • 41:35Sorry, I should stop sharing the screen
  • 41:37I guess. Thank you Doctor Millington,
  • 41:40this is under the truck.
  • 41:41This was a really long talk,
  • 41:44not really, only just we all know
  • 41:46that sleep deprivation is bad,
  • 41:48but it's really impressive to see
  • 41:50the specific mechanisms by which they
  • 41:52lead to disease and importantly,
  • 41:54some of the mechanisms
  • 41:56for potential recovery,
  • 41:57especially with the sleep extension.
  • 42:00So now the forms open to questions,
  • 42:02so please use the chat to ask your questions
  • 42:05and there's a couple in there already,
  • 42:07so I might ask him to you right after that.
  • 42:10And so first question is,
  • 42:12is there data of recovery,
  • 42:14sleep and disease states such as sleep
  • 42:15apnea or other medical conditions?
  • 42:19Um so so we have lot done.
  • 42:26We have not investigated apnea.
  • 42:31Um? We haven't done any of
  • 42:34these studies with apnea.
  • 42:37I think that that would be a very interesting
  • 42:42and important line of of work to do look.
  • 42:46At in more detail.
  • 42:48I mean there there is some data
  • 42:51looking at see pop effects Now, yeah,
  • 42:54but I haven't seen any studies that have
  • 42:58done these sleep deprivation in apnea.
  • 43:01I would expect them to be more sensitive,
  • 43:05and we have actually been looking at
  • 43:09AT and we will be looking at some.
  • 43:13Responsivity in insomnia population.
  • 43:15So I think that's an important area
  • 43:19that that we're starting to move into,
  • 43:22but I think it's definitely.
  • 43:25Relevant often, particularly in shift work,
  • 43:28where there is increased incidence
  • 43:31or increased prevalence of of of.
  • 43:35Uh, you know diabetes and cardiovascular
  • 43:38disease associated with insufficient sleep.
  • 43:41I think, definitely,
  • 43:43it's important to pursue that.
  • 43:46He I I'd like this say something here,
  • 43:50Andre.
  • 43:50This is Mayor Krieger and I I'd
  • 43:54like to remind Janet that the
  • 43:56very she she presented a paper at
  • 44:00the Canadian Sleep Society when
  • 44:02she was a student.
  • 44:05And she got an award and I was
  • 44:08very pleased to give her an award.
  • 44:10What seems like many, many years ago,
  • 44:13and it was a copy of my textbook and Ann.
  • 44:17And who knew that years later you
  • 44:20would be a professor at Harvard.
  • 44:22Anyways, congratulations on a great career.
  • 44:24Thank you so much.
  • 44:26My thought was really very, very nice.
  • 44:29You know, I actually when I got that award
  • 44:32it was really a very special for me.
  • 44:35And that had been work that I did with
  • 44:39a lot of data that Roger Broughton had,
  • 44:42and I did my as you remember,
  • 44:45I did my PhD work with Roger Broughton
  • 44:48that was on the timing and placement of
  • 44:51napping in narcolepsy and circadian aspects.
  • 44:54And I actually got a call from Roger out of
  • 44:58the blue on the weekend this past weekend,
  • 45:01so he's over in France,
  • 45:03sheltering in place and doing.
  • 45:06All. Nice to see you, yeah.
  • 45:10Great thank you.
  • 45:11Thank you for the kind comment.
  • 45:13Merona reminder at that.
  • 45:14Encouragement early on it can be
  • 45:17very meaningful at lead to success
  • 45:19and so so there's a couple of other
  • 45:23questions and so one question is,
  • 45:25in this sleep extension protocol
  • 45:27that you mentioned is the
  • 45:29sleep extension just extending
  • 45:30the opportunity to sleep?
  • 45:32Or are you providing some
  • 45:34sort of interventions to
  • 45:36actually extend sleep?
  • 45:37And yes, we are we.
  • 45:39We give them instruction
  • 45:41that are behaviorally based,
  • 45:42so it's really around sleep hygiene.
  • 45:45We're not doing CBT,
  • 45:46but we we actually do coach and
  • 45:49we have a psychologist working
  • 45:51with us who calls them checks in
  • 45:54with them weekly asking them how
  • 45:56they're doing with it and giving,
  • 45:59giving them some assistance
  • 46:00with following their times.
  • 46:02So they are just given those times.
  • 46:05But they are also given a set of
  • 46:08recommendations for good sleep hygiene.
  • 46:10No blue light before you know
  • 46:12computer screen time and blue
  • 46:15light before bed and.
  • 46:16No caffeine in the afternoon
  • 46:19and keep your exercise and.
  • 46:22Food in consumption not just before bed for.
  • 46:27Sleep hygiene and
  • 46:29all the wonderful things that we
  • 46:31intend to invite patients to do,
  • 46:33and so are the patients asleep
  • 46:35for the entirety of that
  • 46:37sleep extension protocol. Or
  • 46:39are you measuring that in some way?
  • 46:42Yes, we are, so they have.
  • 46:44They have actigraphy for the whole time,
  • 46:46and we have looked,
  • 46:48and they we are not blinded,
  • 46:50but they are wearing their active
  • 46:53graphs and so we have just looked
  • 46:56and so I cannot tell you how long.
  • 46:59Currently with that protocol,
  • 47:01but I actually have preliminary data from
  • 47:05a pilot study we did that did show that
  • 47:08we were able to increase about 35 minutes,
  • 47:11the sleep duration,
  • 47:13and that was measured with actigraphy,
  • 47:16so we're monitoring that again.
  • 47:18They're coming in,
  • 47:19so they start wearing the actigraphy,
  • 47:22and they wear it until they're completed,
  • 47:25which is usually it's taking.
  • 47:29You know three months or so,
  • 47:31so we have a very large data set of
  • 47:35actigraphy on these participants,
  • 47:37as well as they get into the protocol
  • 47:40of and and into the treatment arm.
  • 47:44We are monitoring all of that
  • 47:47and what is interesting we can,
  • 47:49we can tell you that.
  • 47:52The variability of sleep duration
  • 47:55there variability decreases when
  • 47:57they come into the study and that
  • 47:59I think is pretty interesting.
  • 48:01So just by knowing that there they
  • 48:04haven't gotten any instructions yet,
  • 48:07they've just been asked is keep
  • 48:09asleep blog and where in actigraph
  • 48:12and between just over the course
  • 48:15of time until they are randomized
  • 48:17we see this kind of.
  • 48:22Decrease in variability,
  • 48:23so I think they become more aware
  • 48:26of the importance of sleep or they
  • 48:29become more sleep sensitized and
  • 48:31that doesn't influence which is
  • 48:33part of why we wanted to do this.
  • 48:38Wait list control approach because on
  • 48:41our pilot study we didn't have a wait
  • 48:44list control we we measured them and
  • 48:47then sent them on their way for eight
  • 48:49weeks and had them come back and.
  • 48:53And evaluated again, but we thought,
  • 48:55you know, it might also be that now this
  • 48:58is the second time in the lab and you
  • 49:01know it might be a regression to the mean,
  • 49:04so we wanted to add this wait list
  • 49:07control so I'm glad I'm glad we did
  • 49:10because I think looking at at the
  • 49:12second and third overnight stay
  • 49:14is going to be most important.
  • 49:16Yeah, that's a really nice
  • 49:18way to address that challenge
  • 49:19or regression to the mean,
  • 49:21and so there's a couple of more questions.
  • 49:24Methodological, less.
  • 49:25I'll just group them together and
  • 49:27so the other one is your recovery.
  • 49:30Sleep period was eight hours and
  • 49:32often the patients tend to sleep
  • 49:35much longer on the weekends,
  • 49:37trying to make up the sleep, and
  • 49:39so does the greater sleep
  • 49:41extension change their response.
  • 49:43We we can't say, but I think that's a
  • 49:46really important question we we needed
  • 49:48to draw the line somewhere and we rather
  • 49:52than have participants have different.
  • 49:54Durations of sleep.
  • 49:56We decided to go for what might be more.
  • 50:00Commonly experienced,
  • 50:01many adults don't have the opportunity.
  • 50:04Certainly I think you're right with
  • 50:07early adulthood and teens they might
  • 50:10be more likely to extend their sleep,
  • 50:13and we know that early adulthood,
  • 50:15those people in their late late teens
  • 50:19early 20s can sometimes extend sleep.
  • 50:23To an unusually long duration of 12 hours
  • 50:26and even 14 hours that's been reported.
  • 50:30So rather than deal with the.
  • 50:35Large variation in in ad Lib sleep.
  • 50:38We decided to cut it off at 8 hours,
  • 50:41which might be more typical of
  • 50:43people who are working and have
  • 50:45to do other things or take care of
  • 50:48children during their day times.
  • 50:50But I think that's an interesting question.
  • 50:53Can you actually sleep it out if you
  • 50:56allow extended sleep rather than just
  • 50:58the 8 hours of recovery as we chose?
  • 51:02Great, thank you. Let's see.
  • 51:04Then there's there's several other
  • 51:06questions, and so I will have to.
  • 51:10Focus a little bit on the kind
  • 51:11of clinical end of things and so.
  • 51:15My question is, do you think that
  • 51:17the so-called short sleepers without
  • 51:19any symptoms are biologically
  • 51:21protected from potentially harmful
  • 51:22changes of sleep restriction?
  • 51:25Right, well I can. I can tell you that.
  • 51:32Well, we don't know with regards to the
  • 51:34Physiology, and I think that's a really
  • 51:38important and exciting area to do.
  • 51:40To study. We do know that.
  • 51:46That people who claim to sleep shorter
  • 51:49with regards to their performance,
  • 51:51like if you look at Psycho motor vigilance
  • 51:54tests, we do know that they are.
  • 51:57Also they are impaired when
  • 52:00they don't get adequate sleep.
  • 52:02So there might be some.
  • 52:05Definitely I.
  • 52:06I think that there are different
  • 52:09degrees of vulnerability to sleep loss.
  • 52:13We have a lot of data on the
  • 52:18neurobehavioural and to show that,
  • 52:20but I do believe that there are also
  • 52:24different degrees of sensitivity to
  • 52:27sleep loss that system specific.
  • 52:30So you might have more metabolic
  • 52:34vulnerability than neurobehavioural.
  • 52:35Vulnerability or you may have
  • 52:39more autonomic vulnerability.
  • 52:41So I think it needs a good systems approach.
  • 52:46He's not one apart.
  • 52:49And the last question that's here
  • 52:51is an interesting one. It's in.
  • 52:54Is there any information on sleep
  • 52:56restriction and migraine headaches
  • 52:58that you are familiar with?
  • 53:02So Rami Burstein at our institution has
  • 53:04done some work in Susie Birtish has
  • 53:07done some work on migraine and and.
  • 53:12I think that the the actual
  • 53:15research in the area is still
  • 53:19pretty pretty new and there are.
  • 53:22There's some different findings clinically.
  • 53:25I know that patients with migraine
  • 53:29often complain of. You know of.
  • 53:34Sleep loss bringing bringing
  • 53:36on migraine and if they can get
  • 53:40to sleep at an early point in
  • 53:43the process there is anecdotal.
  • 53:46Evidence if you will,
  • 53:48that that sleep can actually turn
  • 53:51it around for some patients,
  • 53:53so prevent the full blown migraine
  • 53:57attack if they get sleep at and
  • 54:01at a critical point in time.
  • 54:06But that's that's anecdotal.
  • 54:09Great, thank you very
  • 54:10much well the commotion.
  • 54:11Thank you very much for a
  • 54:13wonderful talk and answering.
  • 54:14All these questions are fully and
  • 54:16thank you everybody for attending
  • 54:18yet another excellent addition of
  • 54:20the Joint Seminar Series and Hope
  • 54:22you guys have a great holiday and
  • 54:24we will resume the joint seminars
  • 54:26in January with a special guest
  • 54:29James Nestore who is an author and.
  • 54:31I will discuss his recent book
  • 54:33Breathe The Science of the Lost Art.
  • 54:38And then great. Good luck with
  • 54:40the pipeline development.
  • 54:42Obviously you guys are doing a great job.
  • 54:46Super. Bye bye thanks very much.