Skip to Main Content

Childhood adversity and mental health: Identifying opportunities to reduce risk and promote resilience across the life course

November 30, 2022
  • 00:00Good afternoon, everyone.
  • 00:01I think we'll make a start.
  • 00:05So it's great to
  • 00:06see you all here for grand rounds.
  • 00:08Welcome to grand rounds for those of
  • 00:09you who are celebrating last week.
  • 00:11I hope you enjoyed your Thanksgiving and I
  • 00:13hope that everyone had a restful and relaxing
  • 00:16few days towards the end of last week.
  • 00:18And just a reminder about next week,
  • 00:21we'll have compassionate care rounds
  • 00:23here in the Cohen and live on zoom.
  • 00:25As a reminder,
  • 00:26that session won't be recorded.
  • 00:28So please do join us either in person
  • 00:31or live on zoom, or we'll hear from
  • 00:33an expert panel on the complex.
  • 00:36Care needs of patients dealing with
  • 00:38suicidality and disordered eating.
  • 00:40So please do join us for that.
  • 00:43Now in the spirit of the
  • 00:44holiday that we just marked,
  • 00:46I am very thankful to welcome to
  • 00:48have our speaker join us here today,
  • 00:50Doctor Aaron Dunn.
  • 00:51And so Doctor Dunn is an associate
  • 00:54professor of psychiatry and
  • 00:55Pediatrics and Harvard Medical
  • 00:57School and also an assistant
  • 00:59investigator in Mass General Hospital.
  • 01:01And I think it's fair to say that Doctor
  • 01:03Dunn has pioneered the application of life.
  • 01:06Of course,
  • 01:07epidemiological methods to study the
  • 01:09biological embedding of adversity
  • 01:11and the impact of adversity on
  • 01:14adult mental health outcomes.
  • 01:16And now Doctor Dunn has received
  • 01:18substantial support from the
  • 01:20National Institutes of Health,
  • 01:21including the National Institute
  • 01:22of Mental Health,
  • 01:23and has published prolifically,
  • 01:25as you'll have seen.
  • 01:26And just as recently as two weeks ago,
  • 01:28I think you marked your 100th
  • 01:30publication and a nice systematic
  • 01:31review of the of sensitive periods,
  • 01:34the evidence for sensitive
  • 01:35periods of exposure to child.
  • 01:36Our treatment and the prediction
  • 01:37of adult health outcomes.
  • 01:38So hopefully we'll hear a little
  • 01:40bit about that today and a new
  • 01:42area in the Dunlap looking at
  • 01:44teeth as a potential biomarker of
  • 01:46exposure to early adversity.
  • 01:48Again,
  • 01:48very excited to hear more about that today.
  • 01:51So please give a warm child study
  • 01:53center welcome to Doctor Dunn.
  • 01:59I'm impressed, Karen. You did
  • 02:02that all memorized. It's amazing.
  • 02:06All right, so let me go
  • 02:07ahead and share my screen.
  • 02:18OK. I think we're, I think we're good.
  • 02:22Well, thank you everyone for the
  • 02:23opportunity to be here today.
  • 02:24I'm really excited to share
  • 02:26with you more about my work.
  • 02:27As Kieran said, around childhood
  • 02:30adversity and mental health,
  • 02:31I'm going to tell you a little bit
  • 02:33more about opportunities I think there
  • 02:35are to identify risk and promote
  • 02:37resilience across the lifespan.
  • 02:39Can everyone hear me?
  • 02:40OK, OK, perfect.
  • 02:41So I have no disclosures.
  • 02:45So just to Orient us a little bit,
  • 02:48I want to say a little bit about
  • 02:50childhood adversity.
  • 02:51Childhood adversity,
  • 02:51I think is so critical to study because it's
  • 02:55one of the most impactful social determinants
  • 02:57of mental health as well as physical health.
  • 03:00When I think about childhood adversity,
  • 03:02I think about a range of
  • 03:04different kinds of experiences.
  • 03:05These could be events that happen within
  • 03:07the household or outside of the household.
  • 03:10They could be perpetrated by
  • 03:12loved ones or by strangers.
  • 03:15They could be.
  • 03:15Friends that are acute,
  • 03:16they could be chronic.
  • 03:18Some might meet the definition of a trauma,
  • 03:21others might not.
  • 03:22What we know from large scale
  • 03:24epidemiological studies that
  • 03:26have been done primarily in the
  • 03:28United States is that we know
  • 03:30that adversities are common,
  • 03:31so we know that more than half
  • 03:33of all kids growing up in the US
  • 03:35will experience at least one type
  • 03:37of adversity in their lifespan.
  • 03:39We also know that there are large
  • 03:41racial ethnic minority differences
  • 03:43such that kids who grow up from.
  • 03:46Racial and non white families are
  • 03:49disproportionately affected by adversity,
  • 03:51and similarly,
  • 03:52we also know that girls are
  • 03:54more likely to experience some
  • 03:56adversities compared to boys.
  • 03:58The boys are also more likely
  • 03:59to experience some types of
  • 04:01interpersonal violence in particular.
  • 04:03Now I think this following
  • 04:06statistic is both sobering.
  • 04:08These two sets of statistics are
  • 04:11both sobering but also optimistic.
  • 04:13The first is that we know that
  • 04:15adversity is estimated to at least
  • 04:17double the risk of a mental disorder.
  • 04:19Throughout the lifespan.
  • 04:20Now,
  • 04:21it might not surprise you to hear
  • 04:23that childhood adversity is associated
  • 04:25with child onset or adolescent
  • 04:27onset psychiatric disorders.
  • 04:29But we also know that these
  • 04:31adversities are associated with
  • 04:33increased risk of disorders that
  • 04:34onset for the first time in adulthood.
  • 04:37And we also know,
  • 04:38particularly from recent large
  • 04:40scale meta analysis,
  • 04:42that if these effects of adversity
  • 04:44are causal,
  • 04:44they'd explain about 30 to 40%
  • 04:47of the total variability in risk
  • 04:50for mental health problems.
  • 04:51So to me,
  • 04:52I hear that both optimistically sobering,
  • 04:55right?
  • 04:55It's a it's a scary statistic,
  • 04:57but I think it also suggests
  • 04:59opportunities for where our work
  • 05:01can really have potential impact.
  • 05:03A lot of the work that we do in
  • 05:05my group is focused on depression,
  • 05:07which for those of you who may
  • 05:08not be familiar,
  • 05:09is a major public health problem.
  • 05:11So depression is a disorder that's
  • 05:13common throughout the lifespan.
  • 05:14About one out of every five people
  • 05:16will experience an episode of
  • 05:18depression at some point in their lives.
  • 05:20We also know that depression is a disorder
  • 05:23that disproportionately effects women.
  • 05:25So during childhood, boys and girls
  • 05:27experience similar levels of depression.
  • 05:29But something happens in adolescence
  • 05:31where Girl Scout start to outnumber boys.
  • 05:34By a ratio of two to one, and that
  • 05:36disparity persists throughout the lifespan.
  • 05:39We also know that depression is
  • 05:41associated with a host of negative
  • 05:43consequences in the short and long term.
  • 05:46We know it's recurrent,
  • 05:47we know their side effects from medication.
  • 05:49We know that it affects
  • 05:51people's ability to go to work,
  • 05:53to complete school, and so forth.
  • 05:55And I think in its most severe
  • 05:57form is suicide and self harm.
  • 05:59So because depression is so common,
  • 06:01and because it disproportionately
  • 06:03affects large segments of the population.
  • 06:05And is associated with so many
  • 06:08negative consequences.
  • 06:09Hopefully,
  • 06:09it might not be a surprise to learn
  • 06:12that depression is currently the second
  • 06:14leading cause of disability worldwide.
  • 06:16So my group is really focused on trying to
  • 06:19identify ways that we can prevent depression.
  • 06:21And I think that's really critical
  • 06:23because it's a disorder that
  • 06:25strikes when people are young.
  • 06:26And once it emerges,
  • 06:28it tends to be highly recurrent.
  • 06:30So we know that between 20 to 40% of
  • 06:33people who experience depression will
  • 06:36have had their first onset by age 21.
  • 06:39And we also know that about 3
  • 06:40out of every four people with
  • 06:42depression will experience at least
  • 06:44one relapse in their lifespan.
  • 06:46So when I hear data like this,
  • 06:48to me the the the message is we need to
  • 06:51better understand what causes depression,
  • 06:54what's its etiology,
  • 06:55how does it come about,
  • 06:56so that we can use those insights to
  • 06:59then identify targets to identify kids
  • 07:01who might be at risk and prevent the
  • 07:03onset of depression and do that as early
  • 07:06on as we possibly can in the lifespan.
  • 07:08So.
  • 07:09Committed to that goal.
  • 07:10The current focus of my research group
  • 07:13has been organized in these 44 domains.
  • 07:15So we do work on The Who,
  • 07:17how and the when question
  • 07:19around depression prevention.
  • 07:21So with respect to The Who,
  • 07:23a lot of what we do is focused on trying
  • 07:25to identify people at highest risk using
  • 07:27genetic and other markers of vulnerability.
  • 07:30So that's work that we do in in relationship
  • 07:32to the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium,
  • 07:34for example.
  • 07:35We also do a lot of work,
  • 07:37and I'm going to tell you a
  • 07:38lot about this work today.
  • 07:39Around the biological embedding of
  • 07:41adversity and the mechanisms that
  • 07:44might explain how it is that these
  • 07:46stressors and traumas might get
  • 07:48under our skin to shape our health,
  • 07:50the third area is really focused
  • 07:52on sensitive periods.
  • 07:53Try to understand,
  • 07:54are there ages in the course
  • 07:56of the lifespan when our life
  • 07:58experience matters more?
  • 07:59And could that differentially
  • 08:01predict risk for depression?
  • 08:02And then finally,
  • 08:03I'm someone that really believes
  • 08:05in and committed to translation,
  • 08:07so I don't want to just do ivory tower.
  • 08:10Client,
  • 08:10so to speak,
  • 08:10I want to figure out how
  • 08:12to get our findings out to make a difference.
  • 08:14So that's where we've also been doing
  • 08:16work to try to build novel infrastructure
  • 08:19for scientific knowledge translation.
  • 08:20And I'm proud to partner with Josh Rothman,
  • 08:23a colleague in psychiatry,
  • 08:24around a birth cohort work that
  • 08:26we're doing where we're deliberately
  • 08:28from the beginning trying to design
  • 08:30it to not just observe but also
  • 08:32intervene in those participants.
  • 08:33So what I want to do in this talk is tell you
  • 08:38more about two specific aspects of my labs.
  • 08:40Work related to childhood adversity.
  • 08:43So the first is work that we've
  • 08:44been doing to try to identify these
  • 08:46sensitive periods in development.
  • 08:48And then I'll also transition into
  • 08:50telling you more about what we're
  • 08:52doing to try to overcome measurement
  • 08:54challenges that exist in capturing
  • 08:56childhood diversity exposure.
  • 08:58And then at the end,
  • 08:59I'm not a clinician,
  • 09:00but I'll try to talk a little
  • 09:02bit about some of the clinical
  • 09:04implications and applications I
  • 09:06think might exist for this work.
  • 09:08So let me just also clarify at
  • 09:10the beginning because one of the
  • 09:11questions you might have is,
  • 09:13you know, childhood adversity,
  • 09:14trauma, aces,
  • 09:15do all of these things mean the same thing?
  • 09:18So from my perspective,
  • 09:19I tend to use the language of
  • 09:21childhood adversity because I
  • 09:23think it's more encompassing.
  • 09:25Childhood adversity is generally
  • 09:27defined as circumstances or events
  • 09:29that threaten children's physical
  • 09:31and psychological well-being and
  • 09:33their deviations from what you would
  • 09:35expect kids who are typically.
  • 09:37Developing should experience.
  • 09:38I think of aces as being a a set
  • 09:42of 10 markers that have been most
  • 09:44well studied in the context of of
  • 09:47adverse childhood experiences studies,
  • 09:49and so those are sometimes overlapping
  • 09:52with the adversities that we study,
  • 09:54but tend to sometimes not include
  • 09:56all of them.
  • 09:57Some of the adversities we study
  • 09:59could be stressors,
  • 10:00some could be traumas and toxic.
  • 10:02Stress to me really differentiates
  • 10:04the context surrounding the stressors
  • 10:06and traumas.
  • 10:07The kids are going through and
  • 10:09whether or not they have buffers,
  • 10:10mainly those protective adults
  • 10:12who can help buffer those effects
  • 10:14of those stressors for them.
  • 10:16So hopefully that's clarifying in
  • 10:18terms of just getting a better feel
  • 10:20for how I think about adversity.
  • 10:22So in terms of talking about identifying
  • 10:25sensitive periods in development.
  • 10:26So one of the big questions that
  • 10:29I think exists for the field is,
  • 10:31you know,
  • 10:31how does the timing of adversity
  • 10:33shape risk for depression or any
  • 10:36other adverse health outcome?
  • 10:37And if you turn to the literature,
  • 10:39you'll see that there's been a lot
  • 10:40of different theories that have
  • 10:42been proposed on this topic.
  • 10:43So a basic model is an exposure model.
  • 10:46And this model simply states that
  • 10:48people who've been exposed to
  • 10:50adversity have an increased risk of
  • 10:52an adverse health outcome relative
  • 10:54to people who are not exposed.
  • 10:56There's also accumulation models,
  • 10:58and in its simplest presentation,
  • 11:00I'm showing here a basic dose
  • 11:03response relationship.
  • 11:04So the more adversity,
  • 11:05the more at risk you become.
  • 11:07Now that could be exposure
  • 11:09to the same type repeatedly,
  • 11:11or it could be different types of exposure.
  • 11:14There's also recency models and
  • 11:17recency models. Oops, sorry.
  • 11:19Recency models focus on the time
  • 11:21since the onset of the event.
  • 11:26So a recency model says that your risk
  • 11:30of an adverse health outcome is greatest
  • 11:33shortly after you've been exposed,
  • 11:35but then your risk decreases over time.
  • 11:38And then finally there's
  • 11:39a sensitive period model.
  • 11:41And a sensitive period model is
  • 11:43really asking, are there specific
  • 11:44age stages in the course of the
  • 11:46lifespan when our experience matters?
  • 11:48More so over the years on and very
  • 11:52symbolically marking my 100th publication.
  • 11:55I love the symbolism.
  • 11:56Of that, we've spent, you know,
  • 11:58my group has spent a lot of time over
  • 12:00the last however many years to try to
  • 12:02disentangle which of these different
  • 12:04theories might best apply to our data.
  • 12:06And the reason that we've been doing that
  • 12:08is because we think it has important
  • 12:10implications for how we intervene.
  • 12:12So if the data we find are
  • 12:14consistent with an exposure model,
  • 12:16that suggests that we can
  • 12:17intervene at any time,
  • 12:18and it suggests that our goal is to
  • 12:20try to partition the population,
  • 12:22so to speak,
  • 12:23in terms of people who've been exposed
  • 12:25in those who haven't been exposed.
  • 12:26If our data are consistent with
  • 12:28an accumulation model,
  • 12:29then that points us to want to try
  • 12:32to intervene early before people are
  • 12:34accruing those adverse exposures.
  • 12:36If it's a recency model,
  • 12:38it suggests that we want to intervene
  • 12:40quickly,
  • 12:41but it might also suggest that maybe
  • 12:43doing nothing would be OK too.
  • 12:44These symptoms might naturally resolve
  • 12:47or risk would resolve overtime,
  • 12:49and if it's a sensitive period,
  • 12:50model suggests that we want to
  • 12:52intervene during or maybe shortly
  • 12:54before those time periods of
  • 12:57increased sensitivity.
  • 12:57So we think about sensitive periods
  • 12:59as being both high risk periods or
  • 13:02windows of vulnerability when adverse
  • 13:04life experiences are more harmful.
  • 13:06But they could also be windows
  • 13:08of opportunity,
  • 13:09when enriching interventions
  • 13:11could yield greater impact.
  • 13:13So we've been working over the
  • 13:15last several years to try to bring
  • 13:17more research evidence to this.
  • 13:19And my goal ultimately is to try to
  • 13:22enable policymakers and clinicians
  • 13:24to have better data to act,
  • 13:26to know not just what to what
  • 13:28to do to intervene,
  • 13:29but specifically when and to try to do
  • 13:31that on a high resolution timescale.
  • 13:33So in other words,
  • 13:35let's get more granular than just
  • 13:37saying early or saying 1000 days.
  • 13:40The 1st 1000 days rather.
  • 13:43So I want to tell you a little bit
  • 13:45more about the the first work that
  • 13:48we did this was back when I was a
  • 13:51postdoc and was a data set called
  • 13:53AD Health that's now following it
  • 13:55started studying kids when they
  • 13:57were in middle school,
  • 13:58and they've now been following
  • 14:00them through adulthood.
  • 14:02And the way that the data were
  • 14:04recorded in AD health gave us the
  • 14:06chance to ask this question about
  • 14:08sensitive periods because people
  • 14:09were asked were you exposed to
  • 14:11physical abuse and sexual abuse?
  • 14:13And if so,
  • 14:14how old were you when that first happened?
  • 14:17Now,
  • 14:17I know there's measurement challenges here,
  • 14:19and I'm going to come back to that.
  • 14:20But just to give you a sort of
  • 14:22intuition for how we've approached
  • 14:24these sensitive period studies.
  • 14:25So we code people based on whether
  • 14:28they've been exposed to adversity
  • 14:30during these different periods.
  • 14:32And what we end up finding is that
  • 14:34compared to people who were never exposed,
  • 14:37kids who were exposed to physical
  • 14:39abuse generally across the board
  • 14:41have an increased risk of depression.
  • 14:44Compared to kids who are unexposed,
  • 14:46but when we start to compare kids
  • 14:48based on the timing of their exposure,
  • 14:50we do see some within group differences.
  • 14:52So here we see that kids who are
  • 14:54exposed as preschoolers for the first
  • 14:56time have an increased risk of high
  • 14:58depressive symptoms compared to kids
  • 14:59who were exposed for the first time in
  • 15:02adolescence and similarly for sexual abuse.
  • 15:05We find generally across the
  • 15:07board this increased risk,
  • 15:08but here too these potential
  • 15:10sensitive periods this was shifted
  • 15:12to be latency or school age period.
  • 15:14Relative to preschool or relative
  • 15:17to the prepubertal period?
  • 15:20Over the years we've searched
  • 15:22broadly for sensitive periods,
  • 15:24trying to see the level where
  • 15:25they might operate.
  • 15:26So the earliest work that we did
  • 15:28was looking at childhood adversity
  • 15:29in relation to depression and
  • 15:31other forms of psychopathology.
  • 15:33But then we started,
  • 15:34this is really based on my
  • 15:36interest in genetics,
  • 15:37starting to think about these
  • 15:39intermediate phenotypes.
  • 15:40So in other words,
  • 15:42are there these measures that we can
  • 15:44get that are maybe more proximal to risk
  • 15:47based on their timing of occurrence?
  • 15:50In other words,
  • 15:51these measures that are maybe
  • 15:53capturing more of the biology of
  • 15:55the the short-term effects of
  • 15:57exposure to childhood adversity.
  • 15:59And maybe these are the level,
  • 16:01this is the level where we might see
  • 16:03more signal and might be more readily
  • 16:05able to identify sensitive periods.
  • 16:07So we've looked at a number of
  • 16:09different intermediate phenotypes
  • 16:10and I'll tell you more about those.
  • 16:12You know everything from how kids
  • 16:14cope with stress to executive
  • 16:16function and more recently,
  • 16:18molecular targets.
  • 16:21Throughout this work,
  • 16:22we've also been focused on trying to
  • 16:24develop and apply better analytic tools,
  • 16:26particularly to analyze longitudinal
  • 16:28data where you have these repeated
  • 16:30measures of adversity and where
  • 16:32sometimes they're highly correlated.
  • 16:34So this is an approach that we've
  • 16:36been working on with my colleague
  • 16:38Andrew Smith out of the University
  • 16:40of West of England in the UK.
  • 16:42So it's called the structured
  • 16:44life course modeling approach,
  • 16:45or the slick comma and slick
  • 16:48comma is incredibly cool.
  • 16:50It works really well when you
  • 16:53have repeated measures data.
  • 16:55It can work when you have measures that are
  • 16:58measured close in time or more distally,
  • 17:00in time.
  • 17:00What I also really like about it is
  • 17:03that it forces you to have ideas up
  • 17:05front about what you think you might see.
  • 17:08So it's not just a a fishing expedition,
  • 17:10but you have to have some idea
  • 17:12about what you think might be the
  • 17:14theory at play so that you can then
  • 17:17encode your theories into testable
  • 17:19hypotheses. And So what the what?
  • 17:21The way that it works is that it
  • 17:24essentially allows you to identify
  • 17:25from the combination of theories
  • 17:27that you've identified beforehand,
  • 17:29which set explain the most amount
  • 17:31of variation in your outcome.
  • 17:33So it basically works in three stages.
  • 17:35So the first thing you do is you take
  • 17:37all of your theoretical models and
  • 17:39then you encode them into a variable.
  • 17:41So, for example, if you're going
  • 17:43to test a sensitive period model,
  • 17:45you code people based on being exposed
  • 17:48during that time period versus outside.
  • 17:51An accumulation model,
  • 17:52you're coding the number of
  • 17:54exposures and so on and so forth.
  • 17:56And these aren't the only life course models,
  • 17:58I should say, that you could test.
  • 18:00But there's other kinds of models too,
  • 18:02like mobility models.
  • 18:03So where a kid might have social
  • 18:05support and then they don't at the
  • 18:07next time and then it comes back again.
  • 18:10And So what you end up doing is you
  • 18:12take all of these variables and then
  • 18:14you bring them into a regression model.
  • 18:16And the way the regression model is
  • 18:18working is it's in a very sequential
  • 18:19fashion where you're trying to.
  • 18:21Identify the amount of variation
  • 18:23in your outcome or R-squared that
  • 18:25is explained by the greatest
  • 18:28combination of variables and.
  • 18:29So what you can see in this example
  • 18:32is where the model keeps fitting
  • 18:34until it gets to this combination
  • 18:36of both accumulation and exposure
  • 18:38during that third time period.
  • 18:40And So what you're able to also do is
  • 18:42you can look at these elbow plots,
  • 18:44which is what I'm showing here in the middle,
  • 18:46but then you can also evaluate
  • 18:49fit quantitatively using post
  • 18:51selective inference.
  • 18:52And so we've used the slick law
  • 18:54over the years and and also other
  • 18:56analysis to look at you know,
  • 18:58psychopathologies,
  • 18:59suicide risk,
  • 19:00sleep and and other more intermediate
  • 19:04phenotypes.
  • 19:04I want to tell you more about the
  • 19:06work that we've been doing where
  • 19:08we've been engaged in the most
  • 19:10work so far and that's related to
  • 19:13DNA methylation and epigenetics.
  • 19:15So these are chemical tags that are
  • 19:17essentially added to your genome.
  • 19:19They don't change how your DNA sequence.
  • 19:23Is is shaped, but they change.
  • 19:25They have the potential to change
  • 19:27how your genes function.
  • 19:28So they're one pathway through which
  • 19:31adversity might end up affecting
  • 19:33depression and other adverse health outcomes.
  • 19:36So one of the main studies that we've
  • 19:38been using for this is a study called alpac,
  • 19:41where the Avon Longitudinal
  • 19:43Study of Parents and Children,
  • 19:45which for any of you who might
  • 19:46be shopping a data set,
  • 19:47is a wonderful data set they have.
  • 19:50It's a birth cohort and the kids are,
  • 19:52the kids are now in their.
  • 19:5330 So there's you know 30 years of data.
  • 19:57They also collected as part of the
  • 19:59sub sample about 1000 mother child
  • 20:02pairs with DNA methylation data.
  • 20:04And so that was what we ended
  • 20:06up analyzing here.
  • 20:07So we had repeated measures of
  • 20:09exposure to different types
  • 20:10of adversity, things that were happening
  • 20:13within the household up through markers of
  • 20:15neighborhood disadvantage and we coded those
  • 20:17based on the timing of occurrence and then
  • 20:20we looked at these markers of adversity.
  • 20:23In relation to DNA methylation,
  • 20:26a type of epigenetic modification at about
  • 20:30500,000 different sites across the epigenome.
  • 20:33And so we applied the slickman and we asked,
  • 20:35you know, what's the best theoretical
  • 20:37model that might explain the variation
  • 20:40that we see in these epigenetic marks?
  • 20:42Is it accumulation, recency,
  • 20:44sensitive period or maybe a combination?
  • 20:48And what we did was we ended
  • 20:50up analyzing the data.
  • 20:51So on the X axis here is chromosome,
  • 20:54on the Y axis is the negative
  • 20:56log of the P value.
  • 20:57So it's basically the test
  • 21:00of statistical significance.
  • 21:02This is a Manhattan plot.
  • 21:04So ideally it looks like Manhattan
  • 21:05where you see these skyscraper like
  • 21:07effects emerging from the data.
  • 21:09You don't want a Dutch plot,
  • 21:10you don't want it to look flat because
  • 21:13these are basically regions where you're
  • 21:15seeing you know interesting signal.
  • 21:17So and then because we test literally
  • 21:21500,000 different associations,
  • 21:23we correct for that testing.
  • 21:25So anything that's considered to
  • 21:26be above that line is considered
  • 21:29epigenome wide significant.
  • 21:30And so we ended up finding 46 loci
  • 21:33that were distributed throughout
  • 21:35the epigenome as being potentially
  • 21:38impacted by adversity.
  • 21:40And when we dug deeper into these results,
  • 21:43what we ended up finding that
  • 21:45more than half of the loci.
  • 21:47We identified were influenced
  • 21:49by exposure to adversity,
  • 21:50specifically between ages three to five.
  • 21:53I actually didn't expect that we'd find
  • 21:55such strong evidence for sensitive periods.
  • 21:57I was thinking accumulation might
  • 21:59be as important, but it wasn't.
  • 22:01Here we actually didn't identify any loci.
  • 22:04What's also interesting is that
  • 22:06these DNA differences weren't
  • 22:07actually present at birth.
  • 22:08So we looked at whether they happened
  • 22:10in cord blood and they didn't.
  • 22:12And we've been now working.
  • 22:14We're probably about a month off or
  • 22:16so from wrapping up efforts around.
  • 22:17A meta analysis that we've been doing
  • 22:20to try to replicate and extend these
  • 22:22findings and other datasets to see
  • 22:25if they hold and by and large spoiler
  • 22:27alert is I think most of the data,
  • 22:29most of the evidence we are seeing
  • 22:32is for sensitive periods relative
  • 22:34to these other models.
  • 22:36One thing I also want to say too is,
  • 22:39you know,
  • 22:39one of the questions that I think is,
  • 22:41is really fair is these methods
  • 22:43seem really complicated, you know,
  • 22:45is is the juice worth the squeeze
  • 22:47so to speak?
  • 22:48Do you actually get more if you,
  • 22:49you know,
  • 22:50if you get all these repeated
  • 22:52measures and you model it with this
  • 22:54sophisticated modeling approach,
  • 22:55the answer is yes.
  • 22:57So we are able to identify with
  • 22:59the slick ma more signal that we
  • 23:01would have missed had we just
  • 23:03coded people as exposed versus.
  • 23:05Unexposed.
  • 23:06So I think hopefully you hear
  • 23:08a message here of you know
  • 23:10it is worth it to do this
  • 23:12more repeated measures data collection and
  • 23:15use these these more complicated methods.
  • 23:20We've also been working and this
  • 23:22is work led by Alex Lucier,
  • 23:23a postdoc in my group, because we have
  • 23:26longitudinal methylation data in alpac.
  • 23:29So not just looking at methylation at age 7,
  • 23:32but he's also been expanding it
  • 23:34to methylation at age 15 to 17.
  • 23:37So we can understand these patterns
  • 23:39of stability and change across time.
  • 23:41And these, these data are really interesting
  • 23:43and I'm just going to present a a little
  • 23:46bit of what we've been finding here.
  • 23:48So what I'm showing here.
  • 23:49Are the 46 low side that I showed before,
  • 23:53so these were the top low side
  • 23:55that we identified at age 7.
  • 23:57Now we're looking at them at age 15 and
  • 23:59saying do we still see them being you
  • 24:02know largely important and generally
  • 24:04what we find is that the direction of
  • 24:07change or the the pattern of direction
  • 24:11of association is generally the same
  • 24:14but the results are attenuating slightly.
  • 24:16So had we run an epigenome Wide Association
  • 24:19study we wouldn't have identified these.
  • 24:21Game low Sci at age 15.
  • 24:25What we're finding actually now
  • 24:27is a new set of loci at age 15,
  • 24:31so 41 in total and interesting.
  • 24:34These are also underscoring the
  • 24:36importance of this early childhood
  • 24:38of this age three to five period.
  • 24:40So we didn't see them before at 7,
  • 24:42but now we're starting to see them.
  • 24:43So sort of interesting to think about maybe
  • 24:46potential sleeper effects or latency effects.
  • 24:49We don't really know what's
  • 24:50necessarily going on here,
  • 24:51but we're we're starting to
  • 24:53try to unpack this and ask,
  • 24:54you know what might be giving rise to these?
  • 24:56These patterns.
  • 24:57We've also looked,
  • 24:59you know,
  • 25:00at these data and we can find
  • 25:02so far at least six different
  • 25:04patterns of adversity associated
  • 25:06methylation differences across time.
  • 25:08And these patterns essentially
  • 25:10reflect differences that emerge
  • 25:11early versus later in development,
  • 25:14those that happen among people
  • 25:15who are exposed to adversity.
  • 25:17But what's interesting here is we
  • 25:19see differences based on whether
  • 25:21you were exposed during the
  • 25:22sensitive period we think might be
  • 25:24impactful versus outside of it.
  • 25:26And there's some cases where
  • 25:27people who were exposed.
  • 25:29During the sensitive period,
  • 25:31look like people who were unexposed.
  • 25:34And then we're also seeing
  • 25:36differences in in age differences
  • 25:39based on age and assessment.
  • 25:41And I think this is really interesting
  • 25:43in terms of thinking about again
  • 25:44a kind of is the juice worth the
  • 25:46squeeze question of you know,
  • 25:48is it worth us getting these repeated
  • 25:50measures of methylation and and I think
  • 25:52at least from what we're seeing it is.
  • 25:57So. Umm. You might also be wondering,
  • 26:02OK, this is interesting,
  • 26:04adversaries predicting methylation,
  • 26:05but what's actually
  • 26:06happening in terms of health?
  • 26:09And Alex, who's a postdoc,
  • 26:10as I mentioned, and Brooke Smith,
  • 26:12who was a data analyst in my group,
  • 26:14have been doing a mediation analysis,
  • 26:16mediation analysis to essentially
  • 26:18ask is adversity leading to changes
  • 26:21in these DNA methylation signatures
  • 26:23that then predict risk for depression
  • 26:26and what we're looking at here.
  • 26:29So we could basically calculate
  • 26:30all of these different paths.
  • 26:32Using regression and what we're looking
  • 26:34for is to try to identify, you know,
  • 26:37how much of the association is explained
  • 26:40by these methylation signatures.
  • 26:42And So what we found overall is so far
  • 26:4570 total mediators that were identified
  • 26:49across these different adversities,
  • 26:51corresponding to 667 unique CPG
  • 26:55sites that that each explained
  • 26:58between 10 and 71% of the variation.
  • 27:02In risk for depression.
  • 27:04So you can see that there's differences in,
  • 27:06you know,
  • 27:07how much is being explained across
  • 27:09these different types of adversities.
  • 27:11And then what I think is maybe really
  • 27:14interesting is that the epigenetic
  • 27:16adaptation that we're seeing is not uniform.
  • 27:19So when we plot the direction of these
  • 27:22different associations and whether
  • 27:24adversities associated with increased
  • 27:26methylation or decreased methylation,
  • 27:29we're seeing a lot of variation.
  • 27:31So what this is essentially showing
  • 27:33is that most of what we're finding
  • 27:37are effects where methylation changes
  • 27:39are actually protective against.
  • 27:42Depression.
  • 27:43So adversity is associated with a
  • 27:46methylation change that protects
  • 27:48people from developing depression.
  • 27:50We've also been finding that some
  • 27:52sites that we've identified are linked
  • 27:54to cortical development and and other
  • 27:56aspects of brain development and we've
  • 27:58been able to replicate some of the
  • 28:01LOCI and some independent cohorts.
  • 28:03And I think this is another area
  • 28:05that's just ripe for investigation
  • 28:07because it's counterintuitive.
  • 28:09I think most of us would expect
  • 28:11that these things are, you know,
  • 28:13more deleterious.
  • 28:13But it might be that we're,
  • 28:15our bodies are trying to reach homeostasis.
  • 28:17And so we're some of the damage
  • 28:20that's done is protective and
  • 28:22some of it is also harmful.
  • 28:24I also want to just kind of
  • 28:26zoom out and sort of share with
  • 28:28you the last set of work around
  • 28:30sensitive periods in terms of this,
  • 28:32this review paper that John Schaefer,
  • 28:34who's a a postdoc collaborator of
  • 28:37mine and I worked on around the
  • 28:39question of sensitive periods.
  • 28:41So I've been really surprised that the
  • 28:44data we've been seeing for methylation
  • 28:47has been so consistent for sensitive
  • 28:50periods and we wanted to know you know,
  • 28:52does this really extend to.
  • 28:54Other domains.
  • 28:54So we ended up publishing this review.
  • 28:57It just came out a couple weeks ago
  • 28:59looking at a range of different outcomes.
  • 29:02So psychopathology,
  • 29:03neuroimaging, epigenetics,
  • 29:05psychophysiology and behavior.
  • 29:07It's defined by our doc,
  • 29:09the research domain criteria.
  • 29:11So we found 118 unique cross-sectional
  • 29:15observational studies.
  • 29:17Most of these studies focused
  • 29:19on psychopathology as at least
  • 29:21one of their outcomes,
  • 29:22so depressive symptoms or diagnosis
  • 29:25or other PTSD or so on and so forth.
  • 29:30Other ones we're looking at are
  • 29:31other R DOC domains and a handful.
  • 29:33We're also looking at more neural indices.
  • 29:37What we ended up finding was that most
  • 29:39studies did report a timing difference.
  • 29:42In other words, they reported that kids
  • 29:45exposed to maltreatment in one time
  • 29:47period had an increased risk relative
  • 29:48to kids exposed at another time period.
  • 29:51But when we dug deeper into
  • 29:53these timing effects,
  • 29:54we essentially didn't find any consistent
  • 29:57evidence for peak periods of vulnerability.
  • 30:00So it's not as though we saw three to five or
  • 30:046 to 8 is this time period of vulnerability.
  • 30:07This was also very surprising.
  • 30:09So we didn't see that these
  • 30:12biological markers, you know,
  • 30:14the neural indices or other indicators were
  • 30:18any better able than the symptom measures
  • 30:21to identify potential sensitive periods.
  • 30:24We also didn't see any
  • 30:26differences based on study rigor.
  • 30:27So if you had a study where you
  • 30:30compared your models to accumulation
  • 30:32models or you were a larger study,
  • 30:35we didn't see any differences based on that.
  • 30:37Neither we did interestingly share find
  • 30:41that there were similarities in terms of
  • 30:44internalizing and externalizing symptoms.
  • 30:46They did share peak periods of vulnerability,
  • 30:49but specific types of maltreatment did not.
  • 30:51So this maybe speaks to maltreatment
  • 30:54types having potential different impacts
  • 30:57with respect to sensitive periods.
  • 30:59Studies were also split with respect
  • 31:02with respect to sex differences.
  • 31:05We also generally saw a huge risk of bias.
  • 31:09Most of these studies were under powered
  • 31:11and so as a result we ended up providing
  • 31:14a set of recommendations at the end
  • 31:16that we hope will guide future studies,
  • 31:19including but not limited
  • 31:20to issues of of measurement,
  • 31:22which I'm going to turn to next.
  • 31:24So in terms of the issue of measurement,
  • 31:28so this is something I've been
  • 31:30frustrated about for for a while.
  • 31:33So we know that current measures
  • 31:35of childhood adversity have
  • 31:37some pretty serious limitations.
  • 31:39So what we most often do in in research
  • 31:41studies is we ask people and this
  • 31:43is also in clinical practice too.
  • 31:45We ask people retrospectively.
  • 31:47So when you're an adult or maybe an an
  • 31:50adolescent, we ask you how old you know,
  • 31:52did you experience these adverse
  • 31:54events and so.
  • 31:55You might imagine that there's
  • 31:57a lot of potential bias here.
  • 31:58So, you know,
  • 31:59it's subjects to people's memory.
  • 32:01It's subject to whether they're
  • 32:03comfortable disclosing what are
  • 32:05oftentimes very painful events.
  • 32:07So it's no surprise that, you know,
  • 32:09there might be bias in these
  • 32:11retrospective measures.
  • 32:11The other thing that we can also
  • 32:13do is go prospectively.
  • 32:15So we can ask parents,
  • 32:17oftentimes moms,
  • 32:18whether their child is exposed
  • 32:20to certain kinds of events.
  • 32:22But this is another area where
  • 32:24there's potential problems.
  • 32:26So moms might not want to talk
  • 32:28about painful events or events,
  • 32:30particularly when she's the perpetrator
  • 32:33of those sources of adversity.
  • 32:35For adolescence,
  • 32:36there might be some adversities
  • 32:37that parents don't know about,
  • 32:39and I think This is why it's maybe
  • 32:41there's no surprise that when you
  • 32:43ask both children and their parents,
  • 32:45you see very low levels of
  • 32:47agreement between the two of them.
  • 32:49Another source of data would
  • 32:50be the official reports,
  • 32:51like health and Social service records,
  • 32:53but we know that those are also
  • 32:55dramatic undercounts of people's.
  • 32:57Exposure to adversity and they probably
  • 33:00only get about 30% of all true cases.
  • 33:03So, so sort of borne from these
  • 33:05frustrations and a very serendipitous
  • 33:07conversation I had with a colleague
  • 33:10that I started thinking about baby
  • 33:12teeth and this idea that maybe baby
  • 33:14teeth could serve as fossilized records
  • 33:17of people's early life experiences.
  • 33:19So we published this paper.
  • 33:22Back in 2020 in biological psychiatry,
  • 33:24where we outline this hypothesis
  • 33:26and so we said we basically put
  • 33:28forward this teeth conceptual model,
  • 33:31this idea that teeth are as encoding
  • 33:34experiences to transform health.
  • 33:36And So what we said is that
  • 33:37you have this exposure,
  • 33:38so a psychosocial stressor,
  • 33:41it disrupts some biological process.
  • 33:44It leaves behind an imprint of that
  • 33:47biological process somewhere and
  • 33:48that that predicts health outcomes.
  • 33:50And So what we were saying.
  • 33:52That essentially primary tooth
  • 33:53development might be altered as a
  • 33:56result of this adversity and that could
  • 33:59then therefore be captured in baby
  • 34:01teeth that started forming prenatally.
  • 34:04So let me tell you a little
  • 34:06bit more about teeth.
  • 34:07I could talk an entire talk about teeth
  • 34:09because they're like absolutely fascinating,
  • 34:11but in the interest of time,
  • 34:12I won't do that.
  • 34:13But,
  • 34:14but just to give you a little bit more of a
  • 34:16flavor for teeth and in how cool they are.
  • 34:18So,
  • 34:18so most of us are born with 20 primary teeth.
  • 34:21These are our baby.
  • 34:22Death or milk teeth,
  • 34:23they start forming during about the
  • 34:25second trimester of life and then they
  • 34:27continue forming over the first few
  • 34:29years of life and then around age 5 or six,
  • 34:32they fall out.
  • 34:32They're the only part of our
  • 34:34body that actually falls out
  • 34:35as part of a healthy process.
  • 34:37And then they're replaced
  • 34:39by 32 permanent teeth.
  • 34:41And those form postnatally up
  • 34:43through about mid adolescence.
  • 34:45And so teeth are also amazing because
  • 34:47they record the timing of their
  • 34:50incremental growth, so the outside.
  • 34:52Part of our tooth is called the
  • 34:54Crown and that's comprised of the
  • 34:56enamel that we hopefully brush
  • 34:58twice a day in our underlying
  • 35:00dentin and then the pulp and root.
  • 35:02And the way that teeth develop
  • 35:04is really very much reminiscent
  • 35:06of a circadian like process.
  • 35:08So there are cells called ameloblasts
  • 35:10and those are the cells that form
  • 35:12enamel and they're basically acting
  • 35:14in a in a circadian like process
  • 35:16to lay down this matrix of enamel.
  • 35:18And as every sort of passage of time goes on,
  • 35:22it leaves.
  • 35:23Behind an imprint of that recording.
  • 35:26So this is similar to the way that
  • 35:28tree rings develop and that every
  • 35:30year of the trees development
  • 35:31you see a new ring recorded.
  • 35:33Well, our teeth have very similar lines.
  • 35:36There are sets of lines that
  • 35:38correspond to about weekly
  • 35:39development, and then lines that also
  • 35:42correspond to about daily development.
  • 35:44What's also unique is that this recording
  • 35:46of development is found across evolution,
  • 35:48so we see similar tree similar rings
  • 35:51within the teeth across different species.
  • 35:54And teeth also record insults or disruptions
  • 35:56that happen during their development.
  • 35:58So in this way we can think about teeth
  • 36:01just telling us not just whether a stressor
  • 36:03occurred in development but potentially when.
  • 36:05And this can happen on both a low
  • 36:08resolution time scale where you can
  • 36:10see for example these white marks or
  • 36:12these enamel hypoplasia or concentrated
  • 36:14to those two central incisors.
  • 36:16So that maybe speaks to something
  • 36:18that was happening as those particular
  • 36:21teeth were forming,
  • 36:22but then you can get even more granular
  • 36:24and look really at a high resolution.
  • 36:26Time scale and leverage what we know
  • 36:28about those tree ring like structures.
  • 36:30So you could take a tooth,
  • 36:31cut it in half,
  • 36:32take thin sections of the tooth,
  • 36:34put it on a slide,
  • 36:36put it under a microscope and
  • 36:38look at the incremental formation
  • 36:40of that tooth development.
  • 36:42And one of the lines that you can look
  • 36:44at among others is this neonatal line.
  • 36:47So this is a line that actually
  • 36:49differentiates the time of our birth.
  • 36:51So it differentiates prenatal enamel
  • 36:53from post Natal enamel and it's often.
  • 36:56Is in studies of archaeology and
  • 36:58anthropology as as a way of differentiating
  • 37:01those different time periods.
  • 37:03But then there's also other
  • 37:04lines that you can look at,
  • 37:05and these are generally referred
  • 37:06to as stress lines.
  • 37:08Whether they happen prenatally,
  • 37:09anthropologists don't really know.
  • 37:11So this is part of what
  • 37:12we're trying to look at, Umm.
  • 37:14And also these lines occur,
  • 37:17as I said, at different time scales.
  • 37:19So.
  • 37:19So you can get pretty granular with teeth.
  • 37:22Most of the work that's been done
  • 37:24so far around teeth as markers of
  • 37:27stress really focus on Physiology,
  • 37:29physiological stressors,
  • 37:31so disease, malnutrition.
  • 37:33The process of our birth and the recording
  • 37:35of that neonatal line and most of this
  • 37:38happens in archaeological populations
  • 37:39and there is a little bit that's
  • 37:41going on in more modern populations.
  • 37:44But what I think is interesting
  • 37:46is that there are primate studies
  • 37:48that have shown that teeth might
  • 37:50record psychosocial stress.
  • 37:51So Simona Lemmers is a postdoc in my group.
  • 37:54She's a biological anthropologist.
  • 37:56She's been doing some of this work
  • 37:58and essentially shows that different
  • 38:00kinds of events that happen within.
  • 38:03For her study,
  • 38:04it was mandrills.
  • 38:05So you can see these stress lines appearing
  • 38:08shortly after the occurrence of a stressor.
  • 38:11So, for example,
  • 38:13you separate the offspring.
  • 38:14From the mother and you'll see that the
  • 38:18baby tooth will show evidence of that
  • 38:21calendar timed event appearing in the tooth.
  • 38:24So it sort of suggests that maybe there
  • 38:27is something going on about teeth
  • 38:30recording these early life stressors.
  • 38:32So now going from, OK,
  • 38:35so maybe early life stress can
  • 38:37get recorded in teeth.
  • 38:38Well,
  • 38:38what about teeth as a marker
  • 38:40of mental health?
  • 38:40Well, there's been work mostly in
  • 38:43environmental health showing that pesticides,
  • 38:45things that you can ingest or inhale,
  • 38:48those can appear in teeth.
  • 38:50And those are also indicative
  • 38:51of mental health risks.
  • 38:52So for example,
  • 38:54studies have focused on heavy metals
  • 38:57and lead like lead and and other things,
  • 39:01and showing risk for a range of.
  • 39:02Different psychiatric disorders,
  • 39:04autism spectrum disorder,
  • 39:05schizophrenia and the like.
  • 39:08But there really hasn't been a
  • 39:10lot that's been done specifically
  • 39:12in child psychiatry, I think,
  • 39:14and in depression in particular.
  • 39:16And I think this is where teeth Perot
  • 39:18may provide this enormous and unique
  • 39:21opportunity for primary prevention.
  • 39:23So if you think back to the beginning
  • 39:25of the talk where I shared that 20 to
  • 39:2740% of people will have had a first
  • 39:30onset of depression before age 21.
  • 39:31And you think about what I just shared
  • 39:33in terms of the timing of tooth formation
  • 39:36happening early in development,
  • 39:37you can think about every time.
  • 39:39Point when teeth are lost as a
  • 39:41potential opportunity to intervene.
  • 39:43So the first time happens when
  • 39:45teeth are naturally exfoliated,
  • 39:46they fall out of your mouth around
  • 39:48school age, you know, 5 or 6.
  • 39:50Instead of throwing those in the garbage,
  • 39:53what if they were potentially used to
  • 39:55help guide primary prevention efforts?
  • 39:57Similarly,
  • 39:58second opportunity comes for
  • 40:00orthodontic work.
  • 40:01So in the US about 20% of kids will
  • 40:04have at least one tooth extracted
  • 40:06to make room for those braces.
  • 40:08Here too is another opportunity and
  • 40:10also at a time where we start to
  • 40:12see upkicks in risk for depression
  • 40:14and other forms of psychopathology.
  • 40:16And then the last time comes with
  • 40:18wisdom tooth removal surgery.
  • 40:20So this also happens in that transition
  • 40:23to from adolescence to adulthood,
  • 40:25kids are starting to live outside
  • 40:27of the home.
  • 40:27For the first time we start to
  • 40:29see psychosis and other major
  • 40:31psychiatric disorders.
  • 40:32So here imagine again if we might
  • 40:34be able to use these as potential
  • 40:37biomarkers in combination with other
  • 40:39tools to help identify kids at risk.
  • 40:41So so seeing all of these this
  • 40:44under you know under studied area
  • 40:46and seeing the potential I decided
  • 40:48several years ago that I wanted
  • 40:50to become the science tooth fairy
  • 40:52and and try to study baby teeth.
  • 40:55So this is a the cover of a children's.
  • 40:58Look, we actually wrote,
  • 40:59so when kids are recruited into our study,
  • 41:02we use this book as a way of talking
  • 41:04with kids and family about why they
  • 41:06should donate their teeth to us.
  • 41:08I have copies of the book too,
  • 41:09if anyone's interested in it.
  • 41:12I'll just share a very high level,
  • 41:13a couple of ideas part in the pond,
  • 41:15but I just have to. I love puns.
  • 41:17Well, we've been sinking our teeth into,
  • 41:19in terms of this teeth conceptual model.
  • 41:21So Simona Lemmers, Mona lawyer,
  • 41:242 postdocs in my lab and Ryan Lisanne,
  • 41:26who's a pediatric dental resident
  • 41:29at Children's.
  • 41:31So we've been doing work on
  • 41:32the empirical side,
  • 41:33you know, can we see evidence of
  • 41:36markers in in teeth being predicted
  • 41:38by exposure to early life stress?
  • 41:41We published a paper.
  • 41:42Last year showing that markers of Mom
  • 41:45depression and social support were
  • 41:47associated with that neonatal line
  • 41:49and in the direction we expected.
  • 41:51So more stressful births in the
  • 41:54form of higher psychopathology,
  • 41:56wider neonatal line,
  • 41:57conversely more social support,
  • 42:00narrower neonatal line.
  • 42:02And that was also pack.
  • 42:05We also started a study,
  • 42:06we just finished recruitment for
  • 42:08it in the spring called strong
  • 42:09the stories teeth record of
  • 42:11newborn growth where we have.
  • 42:12In recruiting the moms,
  • 42:13recruiting the offspring of women
  • 42:15who are pregnant or raising a
  • 42:17newborn during the timing of
  • 42:19the Boston Marathon bombing.
  • 42:20So the idea here is we have a calendar
  • 42:22dated major stressful life event.
  • 42:24Can we see evidence of that
  • 42:27recorded in kids teeth?
  • 42:29We've also been doing work to then link
  • 42:31what we see in teeth with mental health.
  • 42:33So we published a paper showing
  • 42:35that markers derived from micro
  • 42:37CT of enamel volume and thickness
  • 42:39predicted levels of psychopathology
  • 42:41symptoms in kindergarten age kids.
  • 42:44And then we also have some work
  • 42:46looking at kind of the timing
  • 42:48and pacing of these growth marks
  • 42:50predicting weight gain in adolescence.
  • 42:53And then the last thing that
  • 42:54we've been working on are more
  • 42:55feasibility kinds of studies.
  • 42:56So teeth are new biomarkers,
  • 42:58you know we need to.
  • 42:59You know,
  • 43:00it's scientists and clinicians
  • 43:01how we should be talking about
  • 43:02them with parents and families,
  • 43:03particularly help us understand
  • 43:05how we can use them.
  • 43:06So we've also been doing studies
  • 43:08to try to understand.
  • 43:09What do people think about teeth?
  • 43:12I I laugh when I get emails about them.
  • 43:14Sometimes Mom will say, you know,
  • 43:16dear Doctor Dunn, I've read about your study.
  • 43:19I saved my child's baby teeth.
  • 43:20And then it's either one of two things.
  • 43:23I'm so glad I saved them or you.
  • 43:25Isn't that gross?
  • 43:26I don't know why I saved them.
  • 43:27Something along those lines.
  • 43:29So.
  • 43:29I think there's a lot that we
  • 43:31can potentially learn here,
  • 43:31and I think that will be important
  • 43:33for building a solid foundation
  • 43:35for this work to unfold.
  • 43:37So let me just wrap up by saying a
  • 43:39little bit more on the translational
  • 43:41side in terms of where I see this
  • 43:44work potentially going in terms of
  • 43:46promoting resilience and trying
  • 43:48to reduce health disparities.
  • 43:50You know we talked very early on
  • 43:52in the pandemic about us living
  • 43:54through unprecedented times.
  • 43:55I don't feel like we,
  • 43:56you hear that as much now,
  • 43:57but I still think we very much are.
  • 44:00So you know,
  • 44:01we have all these stressors that
  • 44:02people experience before the pandemic,
  • 44:05you know add on these additional stressors.
  • 44:07That people are experiencing as
  • 44:09a result of the pandemic.
  • 44:10But I also think simultaneously
  • 44:12we're seeing these shifts that are
  • 44:15happening largely as a result of the
  • 44:17civil rights movement around racial
  • 44:19equality and some institutional
  • 44:21practices that are
  • 44:22starting to shift where.
  • 44:27Oh, OK. Thank you.
  • 44:29Umm, where we're seeing some
  • 44:31movement to potentially better
  • 44:33address some of these areas.
  • 44:34And I think where we are as scientists
  • 44:36and also as clinicians is that,
  • 44:38you know, we have the chance to really,
  • 44:40I think, develop a deeper and more meaningful
  • 44:43research agenda to try to understand,
  • 44:45you know, opportunities to identify
  • 44:48ways to promote health and reduce
  • 44:51risk and build some interventions
  • 44:53to really promote resilience.
  • 44:56I think there's at least two.
  • 44:57Main starting points for
  • 44:58where we can go in this front,
  • 45:01I think one is we spend a ton
  • 45:03of time focusing on the bad.
  • 45:05We do a lot of work and adversity and trauma,
  • 45:08you know,
  • 45:09and I think the resilience world,
  • 45:10there's definitely been a
  • 45:11lot of work in this area,
  • 45:13but I don't think that the
  • 45:15resilience work has necessarily
  • 45:16been as integrated in areas of
  • 45:18biology where I think it could.
  • 45:20So I was sharing with some of you that
  • 45:22we just had a grant that hopefully
  • 45:24will get funded that will allow us
  • 45:26to look at the biological embedding.
  • 45:28Of protective factors.
  • 45:29And I think this is something that
  • 45:31we need to bring in as part of our
  • 45:33research model so that we're not just
  • 45:35studying risk because we know that
  • 45:37risk alone doesn't predict outcomes,
  • 45:39but it's a constellation
  • 45:41of different factors.
  • 45:42I think the other thing too is that
  • 45:45we also need to do more to develop
  • 45:47and implement tools to measure
  • 45:49childhood adversity and differentiate
  • 45:51exposure from the biological
  • 45:53consequences of that exposure.
  • 45:54And I think this is really, really hard,
  • 45:57but I'm hoping maybe we're baby.
  • 45:59Keith and and some of our epigenetic
  • 46:01work can go and I think this is really
  • 46:03critical because you might find that
  • 46:05some kid has the exposure but seems
  • 46:07to be doing OK you know there's
  • 46:09individual differences in this adversities,
  • 46:11not deterministic.
  • 46:12So I think being able to disentangle
  • 46:15these is going to be really critical.
  • 46:17In terms of the applications and
  • 46:20implications of the epigenetics
  • 46:21and exfoliated primary teeth work,
  • 46:24you know, I'm an epidemiologist,
  • 46:25so I don't always have the the
  • 46:27the fortune of being able to talk
  • 46:30with parents and families.
  • 46:31But when I do,
  • 46:32I'm always struck by the questions they ask.
  • 46:35And they always ask two things.
  • 46:37The first thing is they want answers.
  • 46:38They want to know why did my loved
  • 46:41one develop a mental health issue?
  • 46:43They want to know if you know their
  • 46:45child being exposed at this age,
  • 46:46you know, caused this,
  • 46:47and then they also want hope.
  • 46:49They want to know what can be done
  • 46:50to prevent some mental health
  • 46:52issue and someone else they love.
  • 46:54And so I think, you know,
  • 46:55what if baby teeth,
  • 46:57when paired with existing tools
  • 46:58and insights like family history
  • 47:00and genetic and other markers,
  • 47:02could provide answers to some
  • 47:04of these burning questions.
  • 47:06And you know,
  • 47:07they these things are something
  • 47:09that naturally
  • 47:09fall out of our mouth and most times they're
  • 47:12either stored or they're thrown away.
  • 47:14But what if instead, these really hidden
  • 47:16in plain sight objects could be used
  • 47:19to give new insights that could help
  • 47:21identify people that might be at risk,
  • 47:23and use the data from that to target
  • 47:27towards specific strategies for prevention?
  • 47:30And I think this is where maybe one day
  • 47:33we might be able to add methylation
  • 47:36signatures or these epigenetic signatures
  • 47:38and teeth as part of our screening tools.
  • 47:41So, you know, imagine a world where somewhere
  • 47:43in the future a child loses a tooth,
  • 47:45whether it falls out,
  • 47:47it's lost for orthodontia
  • 47:49or wisdom tooth surgery.
  • 47:51And that tooth is taken to a healthcare
  • 47:54provider who sends it off then to
  • 47:57a specialized lab and that that
  • 47:59lab is then able to combine.
  • 48:01Data from other omic markers,
  • 48:03genetic markers and epigenetic markers
  • 48:05and survey data about early life
  • 48:08stress and other stressors and more
  • 48:10about the family context and pair that
  • 48:13with family history data and that you
  • 48:15could then use that to then identify
  • 48:17people who might be at highest risk and
  • 48:20connect them with preventative treatments.
  • 48:22I think there's a lot we have
  • 48:23to do on this space,
  • 48:24but I think it's really promising when
  • 48:26we think about what we know already,
  • 48:28we know that exercise is protective,
  • 48:30we know that social support.
  • 48:31Protective.
  • 48:31So can we get that data in the hands
  • 48:34of people and create interventions
  • 48:35that really leverage that so that
  • 48:38we can try to reduce risk?
  • 48:39And it might also be someday too
  • 48:41that we're able to shift these
  • 48:43methylation signatures too.
  • 48:44So we see something turning on
  • 48:45that might be deleterious,
  • 48:47maybe there's an intervention,
  • 48:49biological or not,
  • 48:51that can also produce those shifts.
  • 48:53And then in just my last slide,
  • 48:55I'll also say too that I think one thing
  • 48:58that we also want to be mindful of IS,
  • 49:01is.
  • 49:01This idea of of screening and I
  • 49:03think there's a lot of interest
  • 49:06in people doing screening.
  • 49:07I think we have to be careful around
  • 49:10screening though and and we published this,
  • 49:12this commentary a couple months
  • 49:15ago where we tried to just put a
  • 49:17little bit of context around this
  • 49:18area of screening because I think
  • 49:20we're at this tipping point where
  • 49:22there's the potential,
  • 49:23the real potential for screening for
  • 49:25childhood adversity to do potential
  • 49:27more harm than it does good.
  • 49:29So in this commentary.
  • 49:30We just described some recommendations
  • 49:32that folks should consider when
  • 49:34deploying these kinds of screenings.
  • 49:36And you know,
  • 49:37being very clear about what things
  • 49:39measure and and deploying screening
  • 49:41at the right time and making sure that
  • 49:43there are appropriate interventions to use.
  • 49:45And also just creating systems that
  • 49:47are nimble and adaptable knowing
  • 49:49that the science of adversity and
  • 49:51resilience is changing and therefore
  • 49:52we want to be able to leverage
  • 49:54that best evidence in support of
  • 49:56of future interventions.
  • 49:57So with that, just to thank everyone who's.
  • 50:01And part of my career journey
  • 50:05and my collaboration team.
  • 50:07Immigration is good because
  • 50:09science is a global enterprise.
  • 50:11And thank my outstanding lab
  • 50:14members and sources of funding and
  • 50:16I'm happy to take any questions
  • 50:18you might have. Thank you.
  • 50:26Wonderful. Thank you so much Doctor
  • 50:28Dunn and fantastic mix of topics there.
  • 50:31And I know that we've already
  • 50:32got some questions on the chat.
  • 50:33Are there any questions in
  • 50:33the room to get us started?
  • 50:42And so one question that we had in
  • 50:44the chat and actually from Doctor
  • 50:45Martin was can you talk about the
  • 50:47parallels between telomere length and
  • 50:49some of those markers that you're
  • 50:51observing in teeth? Oh, there is.
  • 50:58I thought you were maybe going to ask
  • 51:00about parallels between Umm Tillman
  • 51:02or length and epigenetic aging.
  • 51:05We don't. What are you? So. Umm.
  • 51:09So I think that this is an area
  • 51:12where I don't know that I've seen
  • 51:15a lot of very good comparisons.
  • 51:18There's the epigenetic clocks
  • 51:19that people tend to use.
  • 51:21There's now, there's now about
  • 51:231/2 a dozen dozen of them.
  • 51:26Some of them are correlating with each other,
  • 51:28some of them aren't.
  • 51:29It depends on what tissue type you get,
  • 51:31whether you have buckle cells
  • 51:33or saliva or blood.
  • 51:34So I think part of what we as a
  • 51:37field have to grapple with is trying
  • 51:39to build studies that allow us to
  • 51:42better understand similarities and
  • 51:43differences in these markers and then
  • 51:46also piece together that with the context of.
  • 51:48Development because as I shared,
  • 51:50a lot of these markers also vary,
  • 51:52you know,
  • 51:53over over the course of lifespan,
  • 51:55telomeres and teeth,
  • 51:56I haven't thought about it and we
  • 51:59haven't done anything on that just yet.
  • 52:01I think teeth are really understudied
  • 52:03and an area where there's a lot
  • 52:05of a lot that we can learn.
  • 52:07I don't know if there maybe there's
  • 52:10something in that circadian process
  • 52:13that can be indicative of of aging
  • 52:16related processes or something,
  • 52:18but I have we haven't gotten.
  • 52:20To that yet, but but it's a great question,
  • 52:22something to think about.
  • 52:30I just had a quick question about.
  • 52:33The you brought up measure difficulties with
  • 52:36measurement and bringing it back to age,
  • 52:38and age being kind of just
  • 52:40a proxy for development,
  • 52:41and then you're interested in
  • 52:43looking for sensitive periods.
  • 52:47I noticed in across the development
  • 52:50age was bent in and I think routinely
  • 52:54about two year increments and I'm
  • 52:57wondering if that is was informed
  • 52:59by by research or because that
  • 53:02really can either hinder or help.
  • 53:04Finding these sort of sensitive periods,
  • 53:08if something falls in between
  • 53:09one of those bins or so I just
  • 53:11wondering if you could speak to
  • 53:13how those are are gathered.
  • 53:15I love your question and doing
  • 53:17sensitive period work in relying on
  • 53:19age I think as your questions may be
  • 53:22saying is just an imperfect measure.
  • 53:24So we we tend to use the most.
  • 53:29The narrowest age we can and then we
  • 53:33afterwards Bennett into developmental stages.
  • 53:36So in other words we try to leverage.
  • 53:38So we have differences based on month of age.
  • 53:41So we have eight months and you know 17
  • 53:44months or whatever and then we'll group
  • 53:45after we do the analysis into just a
  • 53:48developmental stage and the thinking there
  • 53:50is that's just sort of how we think,
  • 53:52we think of you know based on school
  • 53:54age and non school age or preschool
  • 53:57period or what have you so.
  • 53:59It's really just meant to try to
  • 54:01help better translate that work.
  • 54:03I think really to understand
  • 54:05sensitive periods,
  • 54:05we need to have measures of plasticity.
  • 54:08And in order to have measures of plasticity,
  • 54:10we need to know what plasticity
  • 54:12actually is and how what we mean
  • 54:14by it and how we define it.
  • 54:16So I have a postdoc in my group
  • 54:18that's actually working on that.
  • 54:19That's just saying can we get all on the
  • 54:21same page about what we mean by plasticity.
  • 54:24So the plan is to write a paper on that
  • 54:26and then to follow that with a paper on,
  • 54:28OK, now that we're hopefully maybe.
  • 54:30More on the same page about plasticity.
  • 54:32Can we then start to think
  • 54:35about markers of plasticity?
  • 54:36Because we're a lot of us are
  • 54:38really interested in plasticity.
  • 54:39But plasticity means something really
  • 54:41different to a neuroscientist who
  • 54:43thinks about it at a synaptic level and
  • 54:46someone who's thinking about it in the
  • 54:48context of like stroke recovery for example,
  • 54:51and and rehabilitation and
  • 54:52those kinds of outcomes.
  • 54:54So I think this is another,
  • 54:56I think this is a Holy Grail
  • 54:58for our field is to really,
  • 54:59I think if we nail the sensitive.
  • 55:01Period question and we did that through
  • 55:03plasticity and had a good markers of that.
  • 55:05I think that would be pretty,
  • 55:06pretty amazing.
  • 55:07Thank you.
  • 55:12Aye, thank you for such an amazing talk,
  • 55:16so I'm very curious about.
  • 55:19The effects that you observe on
  • 55:22the protective effects of the DNA
  • 55:25methylation changes that specific loci.
  • 55:28Could you elaborate a little bit
  • 55:30more how they were defined and also?
  • 55:32Will that be dependent depending on?
  • 55:36That it occurred during the sensitive
  • 55:39periods and that you like look at that
  • 55:42during that specific time whereas.
  • 55:44You know, compared to auto hold for example.
  • 55:46How would that compare? Um, like?
  • 55:50I will argue that maybe adulthood we
  • 55:53will observe more deleterious effects.
  • 55:56But you know, I wonder about.
  • 55:59Your thoughts on that and I
  • 56:00have a second question,
  • 56:01but if you want to answer that,
  • 56:03thank you for just asking one at a time.
  • 56:05That's that's great.
  • 56:07I think so.
  • 56:08I think there's a lot to still unpack
  • 56:10in this mediation work and there's not
  • 56:12been from what we've seen any other
  • 56:14work that's been done in this space.
  • 56:17And it took a lot for us to
  • 56:19just figure out the methods,
  • 56:20you know,
  • 56:21because you're bringing these
  • 56:22methods that have been developed
  • 56:24typically for what we call like a
  • 56:26small data setting and then you're
  • 56:27applying it to data where you have,
  • 56:29you know, as you know,
  • 56:31500,000 different associations that you can,
  • 56:34you know, study.
  • 56:34So a lot of the time we spent,
  • 56:37you know,
  • 56:37was was built in on that and then we
  • 56:40carried forward our sensitive period
  • 56:42work to try to bring in more of this
  • 56:45information about these different
  • 56:46life course models to try to.
  • 56:48Understand, you know these effects.
  • 56:51I don't.
  • 56:51I was not expecting to see such variation.
  • 56:55I think the next set of questions
  • 56:57that will be really key is, you know,
  • 56:59we just looked at depression at one
  • 57:01point in time in late adolescence.
  • 57:03We can look at later markers of
  • 57:05depression to see if this persists.
  • 57:08And I think, you know,
  • 57:10I come from the camp of let's see it
  • 57:12once and if we see something interesting,
  • 57:14let's try to see it again in
  • 57:16another data set and try to.
  • 57:18Replicate it.
  • 57:19And then that's where let's if we do that,
  • 57:22then let's start digging in on biology.
  • 57:24Let's get into cell culture models,
  • 57:26let's get into animal models.
  • 57:28Let's, you know,
  • 57:29really try to probe this to see if this is,
  • 57:32you know, real and what might be
  • 57:35some of the the consequences.
  • 57:37Thank you.
  • 57:38And then make my second question is sort
  • 57:40of a follow-up of their previous question,
  • 57:43how these sensitive fears
  • 57:44are defined in terms of?
  • 57:48The age or and following up on on
  • 57:53the definition of of that in terms of
  • 57:56like have you considered biological
  • 57:58age like not only tell me your land
  • 58:01but also epigenetic aging and I
  • 58:04always wonder what does that mean
  • 58:06during childhood because we often see
  • 58:09accelerated at beginning of aging in
  • 58:11adults being associated with trauma
  • 58:13but that what does that really mean.
  • 58:17In childhood and if these could be?
  • 58:20A marker for biological aging to
  • 58:23define better sensitive periods.
  • 58:25Yeah, that's a great question.
  • 58:27So believe it or not,
  • 58:28I think it's counterintuitive in a way
  • 58:30for us to think about kids as aging,
  • 58:32but they are.
  • 58:33And we, we did a study actually in
  • 58:36alspach where we showed that some
  • 58:38early life markers of stress were
  • 58:41associated with accelerated aging at
  • 58:43age 7 by as much as seven months.
  • 58:45So a 7 year old could look,
  • 58:48you know, Cellularly older than us.
  • 58:507 year old by they would look 7 point you
  • 58:53know seven years with seven months added on.
  • 58:56So I think for us we wanted to
  • 59:00have in order to do the sensitive
  • 59:02period work you need to either have.
  • 59:05You ideally have repeated measures so
  • 59:08that you can get these markers of timing,
  • 59:12not that you're relying on
  • 59:14retrospective reports.
  • 59:15So we there's not a lot of data sets
  • 59:17that have that repeated methylation data
  • 59:19where you could derive those repeated scores.
  • 59:22But we just got a grant last year where
  • 59:25we're doing work in a South African
  • 59:27cohort and where we're going to have,
  • 59:29we're going to be driving epigenetic
  • 59:32signatures at 1/3 and five and I think
  • 59:34that would be a. Great opportunity.
  • 59:36I'm glad you said this.
  • 59:38I think this is something we should
  • 59:40look into there and see if if how
  • 59:42similar or different it is relative
  • 59:43to findings you get for age.
  • 59:46So I know we're almost at time,
  • 59:48but I didn't realize Doctor Lombroso
  • 59:49that your hand was up actually was
  • 59:51fading into the background there.
  • 59:52So Paul, please, please ask your question.
  • 59:56So can you hear me?
  • 59:58Yes. Yes. OK great.
  • 59:59I I that was a fantastic talk and
  • 01:00:02then specifically because it was
  • 01:00:05introducing such a for me anyway a
  • 01:00:07novel area couple of questions that
  • 01:00:10I try to get my head around this.
  • 01:00:13If a child has early onset
  • 01:00:15depression or childhood psychosis
  • 01:00:20or early childhood onset diabetes,
  • 01:00:24are you saying that that there will be a
  • 01:00:26marker for for in in the teeth of this event.
  • 01:00:30And are the epigenetic.
  • 01:00:33Findings,
  • 01:00:35I would imagine they're all different
  • 01:00:38in these three very distinct disorders.
  • 01:00:41Just to help me understand,
  • 01:00:42you probably already mentioned this, but
  • 01:00:45no, I think it's a good,
  • 01:00:47I think it's a good question.
  • 01:00:48Pardon me, I don't know.
  • 01:00:49I think I'm going to look
  • 01:00:51here even though you're here.
  • 01:00:53Trying to answer you, but Umm,
  • 01:00:56so in terms of what we've seen so
  • 01:00:58far with teeth and psychopathology.
  • 01:01:01So we are correlating marker that's
  • 01:01:04derived from micro CT imaging about
  • 01:01:06how thick the enamel basically marker
  • 01:01:09of enamel volume is and seeing that
  • 01:01:12kids who have thinner volume have
  • 01:01:15higher psychopathology symptoms.
  • 01:01:16I think that's just correlational.
  • 01:01:19Who knows whether this is actually causal.
  • 01:01:22I think we need more studies to try to.
  • 01:01:24Impact that.
  • 01:01:25I think teeth might be a marker.
  • 01:01:28So I think of teeth as the marker
  • 01:01:30of early life stress and then
  • 01:01:33potentially those biological
  • 01:01:34markers of early life stress can
  • 01:01:37be informative for mental health.
  • 01:01:39I don't know that teeth necessarily
  • 01:01:42independent of early life stress would
  • 01:01:44be informative for mental health.
  • 01:01:47However,
  • 01:01:47I think there is maybe a kind
  • 01:01:50of tooth brain access where
  • 01:01:52teeth might be informative.
  • 01:01:54For characterizing and understanding
  • 01:01:57processes of brain development that might
  • 01:02:00be harder to interrogate otherwise.
  • 01:02:03So that's sort of my thought on on that.
  • 01:02:06And then whether the epigenetic
  • 01:02:09signatures are similar across disorders,
  • 01:02:11we've not really looked at that,
  • 01:02:15but I think it's something that
  • 01:02:17that you know the work that has
  • 01:02:19been done is more so where you
  • 01:02:21group kids into internalizing
  • 01:02:23versus externalizing symptoms.
  • 01:02:25Part of the challenge is that you
  • 01:02:28really just need incredibly large
  • 01:02:30sample sizes in order to find potential
  • 01:02:33signal when you bring epigenetic work
  • 01:02:36to psychiatric disorders on the order of,
  • 01:02:39you know, thousands, 10s of thousands.
  • 01:02:42To give you context,
  • 01:02:44you may or may not know about
  • 01:02:47genetic association studies.
  • 01:02:48Those are starting to see results.
  • 01:02:51After 500,000, you know,
  • 01:02:53a million participants,
  • 01:02:54so.
  • 01:02:55I think we're seeing more with epigenetic
  • 01:02:57work starting to emerge with less than that,
  • 01:03:00but it's still the scale is very,
  • 01:03:02very large because these effects
  • 01:03:04are are pretty small.
  • 01:03:09Great. Well, thank you all for
  • 01:03:10this rich discussion and please
  • 01:03:12join me again in thanking Dr Dunn
  • 01:03:13for a wonderful presentation.